Children’s Moral Values

Wonderfully Conventional Krysta Caltabiano displays her poster, “Ways to Be a Good Citizen,” which won the Good Citizenship Contest sponsored by the Connecticut Secretary of State.

The origins of morality are debatable (see Chapter 10), but there is no doubt that middle childhood is prime time for moral development. These are:

years of eager, lively searching on the part of children … as they try to understand things, to figure them out, but also to weigh the rights and wrongs…. This is the time for growth of the moral imagination, fueled constantly by the willingness, the eagerness of children to put themselves in the shoes of others.

[Coles, 1997, p. 99]

Many forces drive children’s growing interest in moral issues. Three of them are (1) child culture, (2) personal experience, and (3) empathy. As already explained, the culture of children includes moral values, such as loyalty to friends and keeping secrets. Personal experiences also matter.

For all children, empathy increases in middle childhood as children become more socially perceptive. This increasing perception can backfire, however. One example was just described: Bullies become adept at picking victims (Veenstra et al., 2010). An increase in social understanding makes noticing and defending rejected children possible, but in a social context that allows bullying, bystanders may decide to be self-protective rather than to intervene (Pozzoli & Gini, 2013).

Children who are slow to develop theory of mind—which, as discussed in Chapter 9, is affected by family and culture—are also slow to develop empathy (Caravita et al., 2010). The authors of a study of 7-year-olds “conclude that moral competence may be a universal human characteristic, but that it takes a situation with specific demand characteristics to translate this competence into actual prosocial performance” (van Ijzendoorn et al., 2010, p. 1). In other words, school-age children can think and act morally, but they do not always do so.

Moral Reasoning

Piaget wrote extensively about the moral development of children, as they developed and enforced their own rules for playing games together (Piaget, 1932/1997). His emphasis on how children think about moral issues led to a famous description of cognitive stages of morality (Kohlberg, 1963).

Kohlberg’s Levels of Moral Thought

Lawrence Kohlberg described three levels of moral reasoning and two stages at each level (see Table 13.3), with parallels to Piaget’s stages of cognition.

  1. Preconventional moral reasoning is similar to preoperational thought in that it is egocentric, with children most interested in their personal pleasure or avoiding punishment.

  2. Conventional moral reasoning parallels concrete operational thought in that it relates to current, observable practices: Children watch what their parents, teachers, and friends do, and try to follow suit.

  3. Postconventional moral reasoning is similar to formal operational thought because it uses abstractions, going beyond what is concretely observed, willing to question “what is” in order to decide “what should be.”

435

According to Kohlberg, intellectual maturation advances moral thinking. During middle childhood, children’s answers shift from being primarily preconventional to being more conventional: Concrete thought and peer experiences help children move past the first two stages (level I) to the next two (level II). Postconventional reasoning is not usually present until adolescence or adulthood, if then.

Kohlberg posed moral dilemmas to school-age boys (and eventually girls, teenagers, and adults). The most famous example of these dilemmas involves a poor man named Heinz, whose wife was dying. He could not pay for the only drug that could cure his wife, a drug that a local druggist sold for 10 times what it cost to make.

Heinz went to everyone he knew to borrow the money, but he could only get together about half of what it cost. He told the druggist that his wife was dying and asked him to sell it cheaper or let him pay later. But the druggist said “no.” The husband got desperate and broke into the man’s store to steal the drug for his wife. Should the husband have done that? Why?

[Kohlberg, 1963, p. 19]

The crucial element in Kohlberg’s assessment of moral stages is not what a person answers but the reasons given. For instance, suppose a child says that Heinz should steal the drug. That itself does not indicate the child’s level of moral reasoning. The reason could be that Heinz needs his wife to care for him (preconventional), or that people will blame him if he lets his wife die (conventional), or that a human life is more important than obeying a law (postconventional).

Or suppose another child says Heinz should not steal. The reason could be that he will go to jail (preconventional), or that the child has been taught that stealing is wrong (conventional), or that for a community to function, no one should take another person’s livelihood (postconventional).

Criticisms of Kohlberg

Kohlberg has been criticized for not appreciating cultural or gender differences. For example, loyalty to family overrides any other value in some cultures, so some people might avoid postconventional actions that hurt their family. Also, Kohlberg’s original participants were all boys, which may have led him to discount nurturance and relationships, thought to be more valued by females than males (Gilligan, 1982). Overall, Kohlberg seemed to value abstract principles more than individual needs and to evaluate people on how rational they are.

However, emotional thinking may be more influential than logic in moral development (Haidt, 2013). Thus, according to critics of Kohlberg, emotional regulation, empathy, and social understanding, all of which develop throughout childhood, may be more crucial for morality than intellectual development is.

Furthermore, Kohlberg did not seem to recognize that although children’s morality differs from that of adults, they may be quite moral. School-age children tend to question or ignore adult rules that seem unfair (Turiel, 2006, 2008), and that may indicate postconventional thinking.

436

In one respect, however, Kohlberg was undeniably correct. He was right in noting that children use their intellectual abilities to justify their moral actions. In one experiment, children aged 8 to 18 were grouped with two others about the same age and were allotted some money. They were then asked to decide how much to share with another trio of children.

No age trends were found in sharing: Some groups chose to share equally; other groups were more selfish. However, there were age differences in the reasons. Older children suggested more complex rationalizations for their choices, both selfish and altruistic (Gummerum et al., 2008).

What Children Value

Many lines of research have shown that children develop their own morality, guided by peers, parents, and culture (Killen & Smetana, 2014). Some prosocial values are evident in early childhood including caring for close family members, cooperating with other children, and not hurting anyone intentionally.

Even very young children think stealing is wrong, and even infants seem to appreciate social support and punish mean behavior in experiments with a good and a mean puppet (Hamlin, 2014). If a puppet is seen hitting another puppet for no good reason, and then a child is asked to allocate candy to the two puppets, the child might give candy to the victim, not the bully.

As children become more aware of themselves and others in middle childhood, they realize that one person’s values may conflict with another’s. Concrete operational cognition, which gives children the ability to understand and use logic, propels them to think about morality. In the opening anecdote of this chapter, when the boy argued that it was “so unfair” that he could not kill zombies in the video game, a sense of morality was evident.

Adults Versus Peers

When child culture conflicts with adult morality, children often align themselves with peers. A child might lie to protect a friend, for instance. Friendship itself has a hostile side: Many close friends reject other children who want to join a game, or conversation, with friends (Rubin et al., 2013). They may also protect a bully if he or she is a friend.

Not Victims An outsider might worry that these two boys would be bullied, one because he is African American and the other because he appears to be disabled. But both are well liked for the characteristics shown here: friendliness and willingness to help and be helped.

The conflict between the morality of children and that of adults is evident in the value that children place on education. Adults usually prize school and respect teachers, but children may encourage one another to play hooky, cheat on tests, harass a substitute teacher, and so on.

Three common moral imperatives among 6- to 11-year-olds are the following:

  1. Protect your friends.

  2. Don’t tell adults what is happening.

  3. Conform to peer standards of dress, talk, behavior.

These three can explain both apparent boredom and overt defiance, as well as standards of dress that mystify adults (such as jeans so loose that they fall off or so tight that they impede digestion—both styles worn by my children, who grew up in different cohorts). This may seem like mere social conformity, but children may elevate it to a standard of right and wrong, as adults might do for whether or not a woman wears a head covering, a revealing dress, or even high heels. Given what is known about middle childhood, it is no surprise that children do not echo adult morality.

437

Fortunately, peers during adulthood as well as childhood help one another develop morals. Research finds that children are better at stopping bullying than adults are, because bystanders are pivotal. Since bullies tend to be low on empathy, they need peers to teach them that their actions are not admired (many bullies believe people admire their aggression). During middle childhood, morality can be scaffolded just as cognitive skills are, with mentors—peers or adults—using moral dilemmas to advance moral understanding, while they also advance the underlying moral skills of empathy and emotional regulation (Hinnant et al., 2013).

Developing Moral Values

Throughout middle childhood, moral judgment becomes more comprehensive, considering psychological as well as physical harm, intentions as well as consequences. In one study 5- to 11-year-olds saw pictures depicting situations when a child hurt another to prevent further harm (stopping a friend from a dangerous climb to retrieve a ball) or when one child was simply mean (such as pushing a friend off the swings so the child can swing). The younger children were more likely to judge based on results, but the older children considered intention.

When harm was psychological, not physical (hurting the child’s feelings, not hitting) more than half of the older children considered intentions, but only about 5 percent of the younger children did. Compared to the younger children, the older children more often said justifiable harm was okay but unjustifiable harm should be punished (Jambon & Smetana, 2014).

A detailed examination of the effect of peers on morality began with an update on one of Piaget’s moral issues: whether punishment should seek retribution (hurting the transgressor) or restitution (restoring what was lost). Piaget found that children advance from retribution to restitution between ages 8 and 10 (Piaget, 1932/1997).

To learn how this occurs, researchers asked 133 9-year-olds to consider this scenario:

Late one afternoon there was a boy who was playing with a ball on his own in the garden. His dad saw him playing with it and asked him not to play with it so near the house because it might break a window. The boy didn’t really listen to his dad, and carried on playing near the house. Then suddenly, the ball bounced up high and broke the window in the boy’s room. His dad heard the noise and came to see what had happened. The father wonders what would be the fairest way to punish the boy. He thinks of two punishments. The first is to say: “Now, you didn’t do as I asked. You will have to pay for the window to be mended, and I am going to take the money from your pocket money.” The second is to say: “Now, you didn’t do as I asked. As a punishment you have to go to your room and stay there for the rest of the evening.” Which of these punishments do you think is the fairest?

[Leman & Björnberg, 2010, p. 962]

The children were split almost equally in their initial responses. Then 24 pairs were formed of children who had opposite views. Each pair was asked to discuss the issue, trying to reach agreement. (The other children did not discuss it.) Six pairs were boy–boy, six were boy–girl with the boy favoring restitution, six were boy–girl with the girl favoring restitution, and six were girl–girl.

The conversations typically took only five minutes, and the retribution side was more often chosen—which Piaget would consider a moral backslide, since more restitution than retribution advocates switched. However, two weeks and eight weeks later all the children were queried again. Many responses changed toward the more advanced, restitution response (see Figure 13.2). This advance occurred even for the children who merely thought about the dilemma again (no discussion) but children who had discussed it with another child were particularly likely to decide that restitution was best.

FIGURE 13.2
Benefits of Time and Talking The graph on the left shows that most children, immediately after their initial punitive response, became even more likely to seek punishment rather than to repair damage. However, after some time and reflection, they chose the response Piaget considered more mature. The graph on the right indicates that children who had talked about the broken window example moved toward restorative justice even in examples they had not heard before. That was not true for those who had not talked about the first story.

438

The main conclusion from this study was that “conversation on a topic may stimulate a process of individual reflection that triggers developmental advances” (Leman & Björnberg, 2010, p. 969). Parents and teachers take note: Raising moral issues, and letting children discuss them, advances morality—not immediately, but soon.

Think again about the opening anecdote for this chapter (killing zombies) or the previous chapter (piercing ears). In both cases, the parent used age as a criterion, and in both cases the child rejected that argument. A better argument might raise a higher standard—in the first example, for instance, that killing is never justified. The child might disagree, but such conversations might help the child think more deeply about moral values, as happened in this experiment. That deeper thought might protect the child during adolescence, when life-changing moral issues arise, which is described in the next three chapters.

SUMMING UP   Moral issues are of great interest to children in middle childhood, when children become more aware of the values of their cultures, their parents, and particularly of their peers. Kohlberg’s stages of moral thought are based on advancing rationalism, in parallel to Piaget’s stages of development. He believed the progression is universal, from the young child’s egocentrism to the adults’ highest level of morality, which transcends the norms of any particular nation. Kohlberg has been criticized for not having a multicultural understanding, but he seems correct in his conclusion that moral judgment advances from ages 6 to 11. Children develop moral standards that they try to follow. These may differ from adult morals, in part because children’s morality includes loyalty to peers. Maturation, reflection, and discussion all foster moral development.

WHAT HAVE YOU LEARNED?

  1. Question 13.20

    e/O8GduSerKhlkI3wCshXsVXlcaJe9e+cl+5xXPndZDz45/nM41uIGT5lme+5DZgjztze/F5VarN2S4kn+zG9GSY3IcRtBDm78dsvmj0TGNoq1S1
    A variety of answers will work for this question. An example might be:

    Preconventional moral reasoning: Children are most interested in their personal pleasure or avoiding punishment. For example, a child may avoid taking a brownie from a batch that his mother made for his sister's sleepover. His decision is based on the knowledge that if he gets caught, he will be in trouble.

    Conventional moral reasoning: Children watch what their parents, teachers, and friends do, and try to follow suit. For example, if a child observes her parents being kind to others, she is likely to engage in similar behavior. In contrast, if a child observes her parents frequently yelling or cursing, she may imitate that behavior with peers and siblings.

    Postconventional moral reasoning: Adolescents use abstractions, going beyond what is concretely observed, willing to question “what is” in order to decide “what should be.” For example, a young person may observe a homeless man being kicked out of a restaurant. Realizing that the man may simply want a drink or a small bite to eat, the adolescent decides that the manager was out of line. He speaks his mind and gives his leftovers to the homeless man.
  2. Question 13.21

    BP4sz+7MkylRQHk8Ig5+Je3okXoGBrP6G/opU3VzdwQAECFBXVooUxkCv5tiSz3vte+Dzig4oEE=
    Kohlberg has been criticized for ignoring cultural and gender differences—not considering, for example, how females and some whole societies value social harmony over abstract justice.
  3. Question 13.22

    0q3pxHVvmp0cg2X842RSKCiAsUBHyjeMRjsEGvsBthSrKy8Us/1ClP2c0WQ0TfBCnqrmOl+oH1pY3XURM83Le658Vu70bZCs8AJmfqegZpp8V7waBnVgIhzdHkRNWOl3q539n6FXDyKMt4+P
    Three values include:

    Protect your friends.

    Don't tell adults what's happening.

    Don't be too different from your peers.
  4. Question 13.23

    jLavbaB3YFFXcfpz/Qv6w5yooSWVXymnYxTzS38nJ7nGk0DqUDYMi492StGz6mp/qf1SAIKK35fk2Ac5cBz2L+0JSPnnHJ3oyHYe8EvlGT8tzfZTd8BTD8t9N40=
    Throughout middle childhood, moral judgment becomes more comprehensive, considering psychological as well as physical harm, intentions as well as consequences. Research findings suggest that “conversation on a topic may stimulate a process of individual reflection that triggers developmental advances.” Accordingly, raising moral issues, and letting children discuss them, advances morality—not immediately, but soon.

439