Understanding How and Why

The science of human development seeks to understand how and why people—all kinds of people, everywhere, of every age—change over time. The goal of this science is to help Earth’s 7 billion people fulfill their potential.

Each aspect of this definition above merits explanation.

The Need for Science

Developmental study is a science. It depends on theories, data, analysis, critical thinking, and sound methodology, just like every other science. All scientists ask questions and seek answers in order to ascertain “how and why.” In this process, scientists gather evidence on whatever they are studying, be it chemical elements, rays of light, or, here, child behavior.

One hallmark of the science of human development is that it is interdisciplinary; that is, scientists from many academic disciplines (biology, psychology, sociology, anthropology, economics, and history among them) contribute to our understanding of how and why people grow.

Science is especially useful when we study children: Lives depend on it. What should pregnant women eat and drink? How much should babies cry? When and how should children be punished? What should 5-year-olds learn? Should they learn in school or at home? Through required memorization or free play? About God or about algebra?

People have disagreed about almost every question in child development—often vehemently, sometimes violently. Some parents beat their children; others imprison such parents. Some parents sacrifice vast sums to send their children to schools that others abhor. Science informs, guides, and redirects those on all sides of every dispute.

The Scientific Method

To discard unexamined opinions and to rein in personal biases, we follow the five steps of the scientific method (see Figure 1.1):

FIGURE 1.1
Process, Not Proof Built into the scientific method—in questions, hypotheses, tests, and replication—is a passion for possibilities, especially unexpected ones.
  1. Begin with curiosity. On the basis of theory, prior research, or personal observation, pose a question.

  2. Develop a hypothesis. Shape the question into a hypothesis, a specific prediction to be examined.

  3. Test the hypothesis. Design and conduct research to gather empirical evidence (data).

    5

  4. Analyze data and draw conclusions. Determine whether the evidence supports the hypothesis.

  5. Report the results. Share the data, procedures, statistics, conclusions, and alternative explanations.

The Need for Replication

As you see, scientists begin with curiosity and then seek the facts, drawing conclusions only after careful research. Replication—repeating the procedures and methods of a study with different participants—may be a final step (Jasny et al., 2011). Scientists learn from each other, building on what has gone before. They hesitate to draw conclusions or to believe the results of others’ research until replication has occurred—although often the media broadcast surprising findings before replication.

This method is not foolproof. Scientists sometimes draw conclusions too quickly, misinterpret data, or ignore alternative perspectives, as discussed at the end of this chapter. Something that is valid for one group of children in one time and place may not be valid elsewhere or in another time. Scientists continually refine methods, question the conclusions drawn by others, and occasionally discover—to their shock and horror—that another scientist has not followed the procedures outlined above. Replication is needed to verify conclusions.

Always, however, asking questions and testing hypotheses by gathering data is the foundation of science; always, scientists seek facts, not untested assumptions.

The Nature–Nurture Controversy

A great puzzle of development—the nature–nurture debate—is an easy example of the need for science. Nature refers to the influence of the genes that people inherit. Nurture refers to environmental influences, which begin with the health and diet of the embryo’s mother and continue lifelong, including the impacts of family, school, community, and culture.

The nature–nurture debate has many other names, among them heredity–environment and maturation–learning. Under whatever name, the basic question is: How much of any characteristic, behavior, or emotion is the result of genes, and how much is the result of experience?

Some people believe that most traits are inborn, that children are innately good (“an innocent child”) or bad (“beat the devil out of them”). Others stress nurture, crediting or blaming parents, neighborhoods, drugs, or even food when someone is good or bad, a hero or a criminal.

Neither belief is accurate. The question is “how much,” not “which,” because both genes and the environment affect every characteristic: Nature always affects nurture, and then nurture affects nature. Even “how much” is misleading if it implies that nature and nurture each contribute a fixed amount (Eagly & Wood, 2013; Lock, 2013).

6

A complex nature–nurture interaction is apparent in every moment of our lives, as is evident in the opening vignette of this chapter. I fainted at Caleb’s birth because of at least ten factors (age, air quality, exhaustion, exertion, gender, hormones, joy, low blood sugar, memory, relief), each influenced by both nature and nurture. The combination, and no single factor, landed me on the floor.

Some People Are Vulnerable

Red Means Stop At top, the red areas on this PET scan show abnormally low metabolic activity and blood flow in a depressed person’s brain, in contrast to the normal brain at bottom. Neuroscience confirms that depression is biological, not just psychological.

Each aspect of nature and nurture depends on other aspects of nature and nurture, in ways that vary for each individual (Manuck & McCaffery, 2014). For instance, the negative impact of a beating, or of any other experience, might be magnified because of the particular versions of genes that a person has. The opposite is true as well: Some genes are protective, making people less vulnerable to difficult or traumatic experiences. Similarly, differences in nurture can either protect against or worsen the impact of a person’s genetic make-up.

For example, some people inherit genes (nature) for diabetes but never get that disease because nurture (in this case, diet and exercise) protects them. Or a person could be overweight and sedentary (both risk factors for diabetes and many other ailments) but never become diabetic because their genes buffer the effects of their habits.

Sometimes protective factors, in either nature or nurture, outweigh liabilities. As one review explains, “there are, indeed, individuals whose genetics indicate exceptionally high risk of disease, yet they never show any signs of the disorder” (Friend & Schadt, 2014, p. 970).

This is called differential susceptibility (or differential sensitivity)—that is, how sensitive a person is to any particular environmental experience differs from one person to another because of the particular genes each person has inherited. Some people are like dandelions—hardy, growing and thriving in good soil or bad, with or without ample sun and rain. Other people are like orchids—quite wonderful, but only when ideal growing conditions are met (Ellis & Boyce, 2008; Laurent, 2014).

For example, in one study, depression in pregnant women was assessed and then the emotional maturity of their children was measured. Those children who had a particular version of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) were likely to be emotionally immature if their mothers had been depressed, but more mature than average if their mothers had not been depressed (Babineau et al., 2014).

The Baby with Colic

An example of differential susceptibility comes from the 10 to 20 percent of all infants who cry for hours at a time in the first 3 months of life, presumably as a result of genes. They are said to have colic, and their frustrated parents cannot comfort them (J.S. Kim, 2011).

A colicky baby is like an orchid, and future development depends on nurture. For some, their inconsolable crying makes the parents unusually responsive. Then the children become better than average (more outgoing, generous, high–achieving) when they outgrow their early difficulties. Other naturally difficult orchids provoke parental anger, or even rejection, the effects of which last long after the colic has subsided. They become low-achieving, unhappy children.

One study of colicky babies confirms that parents react in many ways (Landgren et al., 2012). One mother said:

There were moments … when she was apoplectic and howling so much that I almost got this thought, ‘now I’ll take a pillow and put it over her face just until she quietens down, until the screaming stops.’

7

By contrast, another mother said:

In some way, it made me stronger, and made my relationship with my son stronger…. Because I felt that he had no one else but me. ‘If I can’t manage, no one can.’ So I had to cope.

As two developmental experts explain:

Differential susceptibility implies … that it may be mistaken to regard some children—like highly negative infants—as simply more vulnerable to the negative effects of adversity. And this is because such children may also benefit more than others from environmental support and enrichment.

[Belsky & Pluess, 2012, p. 3]

These experts find that genetic vulnerability (in this case, the DRD4 gene) does not disappear. During adolescence even well-nurtured orchids are more rebellious and impulsive than the less temperamental dandelions, but nurture always affects nature.

The specifics of differential susceptibility require complex and extensive empirical data, as thousands of scientists seeks to understand exactly how nature and nurture interact to produce each particular human trait with each version of each gene. But the simple conclusion remains: Neither genes nor upbringing alone make a child amazingly good or incredibly bad (Masten, 2014). Both nature and nurture matter.

The Three Domains

Obviously, it is impossible to examine nature and nurture on every aspect of human development at once. Typically therefore, individual scientists study one characteristic at a time. A century ago, physical development (such as tooth eruption or running speed) was the main focus of developmental research, but scientists now realize that not only the body but also the intellect and emotions develop throughout life. To understand the whole person, an interdisciplinary approach to human development has replaced the old silo approach of the past.

To make it easier to study, development is often considered in three domains—biosocial, cognitive, and psychosocial. (Figure 1.2 describes each domain.) Each domain includes several academic disciplines: The biosocial includes biology, neuroscience, and medicine; the cognitive includes psychology, linguistics, and education; and the psychosocial includes economics, sociology, and history. Typically, each scholar follows a particular thread within one domain, using clues and conclusions from other scientists who have concentrated on that same thread.

FIGURE 1.2
The Three Domains The division of human development into three domains makes it easier to study, but remember that very few factors belong exclusively to one domain or another. Development is not piecemeal but holistic: Each aspect of development is related to all three domains.

8

However, since every person is a whole tapestry of multi-colored threads, every aspect of growth is biopsychosocial, touching on all three domains. For example, babies start speaking because of maturation of the brain, mouth, and vocal cords (biological), which allows them to express connections between objects and words (cognitive), which could not occur unless people talked to them (psychosocial).

This constant interaction of domains presents a problem: Words and pages follow in sequence and the mind thinks one thought at a time. That makes it impossible to describe or grasp all domains simultaneously. The scientific method weaves disparate threads together, as evidence-based conclusions from many sources advance our understanding of the whole person.

Childhood and Adulthood

Are children more important than adults? For decades, because the focus of developmentalists was on physical growth, the answer to this question was yes. Consequently, the study of human development was the study of child development, with a nod to the physical declines of old age. That produced, as a famous critic described, “a curiously broken trajectory of knowledge … [with] a brave beginning, a sad ending, and an empty middle (Bronfenbrenner, 1977, p. 525).

Since people were not thought to develop over the years of adulthood, developmentalists did not study adults. The opposing giants of developmental theory, Sigmund Freud and Jean Piaget, agreed on one thing: The final stage of development began in early adolescence and then continued without significant change until death.

Recently, however, that empty middle has been filled. Scientists now gather data about adulthood, discovering many developmental changes. For example, sexual appetites, cognitive perspectives, and employment attitudes all change markedly from ages 15 to 65.

No one now thinks that development stops at age 15. Although many scholars still focus on one part of the lifespan, every developmentalist considers what happened before, and what will happen after, each particular period. For instance, one influential scholar believes infancy is “the foundation and catalyst of human development” (Bornstein, 2014, p. 121). In other words, he studies infancy to understand the rest of life.

Is childhood more important, influential, and determinative than adulthood? If a person is, say, malnourished from age 30 to 33, is that as harmful as if that same person had been malnourished from birth to age 3? The answer to that specific question is no. Neuroscientists have proven that early malnutrition can stunt brain growth and have long-lasting effects far worse than those of later malnutrition.

But scientists are not certain about the cognitive and psychosocial domains. If you had to choose only one developmental period in which to invest billions of education dollars, should you choose preschools or colleges? Or if you were a psychologist who wanted to treat people who would benefit most, would they be children or adults?

The answer is not obvious. Some research suggests that the first years of life are the most crucial for intellectual or emotional development, but other research finds the opposite: Educating parents, or even grandparents, may be the best way to help children. Some researchers find that the adolescent years are more pivotal for later development than those of early childhood (Falconi et al., 2014).

9

Political debates need solid data. Should the U.S. Congress protect fetuses and infants (e.g., WIC, mother/infant food supplements), or preschoolers (e.g. Head Start), or older children (e.g., public schools), or emerging adults (e.g., college subsidies), or employees (e.g., raising the minimum wage) or seniors (e.g., Social Security and Medicare)?

All of these age groups need help, but government programs are expensive. If we knew that a particular investment in one age group would have greater impact overall than the same money invested at another age, that would guide policy. But developmentalists disagree, even on who needs financial support most, much more on who most needs education, or family support. More science is needed. This leads us to the second phase of our definition.

SUMMING UP   The scientific study of human development follows five steps: curiosity, hypothesis, data collection, conclusions, and reporting. Scientists build on prior studies, examining procedures and replicating results—thus confirming or refuting conclusions. This research method is designed to avoid wishful thinking, untested assumptions, and prejudice.

For instance, scientists no longer assume that development is either totally genetic or totally environmental. Instead, nature and nurture always interact, with variations between one person and another, as highlighted by differential susceptibility. Colic is one example.

The scientific method is followed within many disciplines and in all three domains—biosocial, cognitive, and psychosocial. Although each researcher typically concentrates on one aspect of development in one domain, no one stays in their own silo because science connects everyone’s data and conclusions.

Once developmental scientists focused almost exclusively on children. Then adult development was recognized. Now researchers seek to understand which interventions at what ages are most effective for optimal development.

WHAT HAVE YOU LEARNED?

  1. Question 1.1

    2nG+MZgbLP0aDAzaDIWfcpAfVA7rgfXr6lP1rtwxWz1FKbtzKNoUeDYzDNnQUjApOw7AQ9GSX3s=
    Just like every other science, developmental study depends on theories, data, analysis, critical thinking, and sound methodology.
  2. Question 1.2

    Kyq/GN9jaG09pGNgcWu0D+BZVdRaVbQvktKVZc2sil11VvOLrFzi4HoSH25Fs457iq3zSYuGAoNQgcQRBHVXadXp63TsqfmziW2IxnZqp38GIIh13I2vPpGYIo8RmzfIWZOX1bnQ9K3OYprzSEfLcamP66pt31TcPbgfNq+5uubgrVzgxrjopWUABL37eZXglPEZgI9fRcp5uQAYYFuJZg==
    ReplicationReplication involves repeating a study, usually with different participants. Scientists learn from each other, building on what has gone before. They hesitate to draw conclusions or to believe the results of others' research until replication has occurred.
  3. Question 1.3

    G0hoax+B+YyeN7AITCPSstf3d+6IBmkNmthCZ4vl0dcTOQ70rV+K2xJePrWJsEc8IiKyhLbO/tDBwyifTlEnJ/dCIGadxKSEiS8M5g3uSSrSj8yfxyQ22A==
    Both genes and the environment affect every human characteristic: Nature always affects nurture, and nurture always affects nature.
  4. Question 1.4

    wpMbLaZU7V/JfsUQFOPKtCehPkey2uXvx05df88m8JYYPyySyCyLqvT1B3zh1CfeHz2k16yto23wIY7+RyMDM1g77/G4+FQbfSndKA==
    Differential susceptibility is the idea that people vary in how sensitive they are to particular environmental experiences. Susceptibility differs from one child to the next because of the particular genes each person has inherited. Some people are like dandelions—hardy, growing and thriving in good soil or bad, with or without ample sun and rain. Others are like orchids—quite wonderful but only when ideal growing conditions are met.
  5. Question 1.5

    I12DJ/kKb9yK3qSIskYmbMSX+/Mzr3HaFdXAQFwnpai0xacD5VB4dJTUoyUlxV6dhbNi+0b/TMCjtCeMkXWFQ2eGbW2UC9SWXnrFSLa1+GqWD1JmOKOMxNzjckNfSBVEdEGZfpdv504aISL5SMI9mN5W3A4Mo3OifUbWl2wnG+/IdUX4/d6Dy4q42vMaF+FDPiwUlNlZqGA=
    The biosocial domain includes all of the growth and change that occur in a person's body and the genetic, nutritional, and health factors that affect that growth and change. The cognitive domain includes all the mental processes that a person uses to obtain knowledge or to think about the environment. The psychosocial domain includes development of emotions, temperament, and social skills.
  6. Question 1.6

    XhtXTUUzzRWabNqkWMfxC/4N6eEtsx3KHEqLqjYHZPZzKeHem5qHQKiYJYK01J9Ay+jzkoseNX0jVh6IAZtRoLciTYXnzrjIr2gR0UXrMEsr+34TV+59m2EdGqbe+akvICton8HmtESO//Nu20/e7YTFodacUix8
    For decades, because the focus of developmentalists was on physical growth, many believed that the childhood years were more crucial for development than the adulthood years. Today, researchers recognize the importance of child, adolescent, and adult development.