Emotional Development

In their first two years, infants progress from reactive pain and pleasure to complex patterns of social awareness (see At About This Time), a movement from reflexive emotions to learned and then thoughtful ones (Panksepp & Watt, 2011). Infant emotions arise more from basic impulses than from the cortex, so speedy, uncensored reactions—crying, startling, laughing, raging—are common.

130

Table 4.1: At About This Time: Developing Emotions
Birth Distress; contentment
6 weeks Social smile
3 months Laughter; curiosity
4 months Full, responsive smiles
4–8 months Anger
9–14 months Fear of social events (strangers, separation from caregiver)
12 months Fear of unexpected sights and sounds
18 months Self-awareness; pride; shame; embarrassment

As always, culture and experience influence the norms of development. This is especially true for emotional development after the first eight months.

Early Emotions

At first there is pleasure and pain. Both are evident throughout infancy, even throughout life, but the triggers, reasons, and expressions change with development, and many new emotions appear.

AT BIRTH Newborns are happy and relaxed when fed and drifting off to sleep. They cry when they are hurt or hungry, tired or frightened (as by a loud noise or a sudden loss of support). Some infants have bouts of uncontrollable crying, called colic, probably the result of immature digestion; some have reflux, probably the result of immature swallowing. About 20 percent of babies cry “excessively,” defined as more than three hours a day, more than three days a week, more than three weeks (J. S. Kim, 2011).

Curiosity is also present, although briefly, as sleep and hunger overtake almost every other reaction. Even a crying baby can suddenly grow quiet, when sucking and food overcome the emotion.

social smile

A smile evoked by a human face, normally first evident in full-term infants about 6 weeks after birth.

SMILING AND LAUGHING Soon, additional emotions become recognizable. Happiness is expressed by the social smile, evoked by a human face at about 6 weeks for full-term babies, a few weeks later for preterm infants.

Smiles All Around Joy is universal when an infant smiles at her beaming grandparents—a smile made even better when the tongue joins in. This particular scene took place in Kazakhstan in central Asia, an independent nation since 1991.

Infants worldwide express social joy, even laughter, between 2 and 4 months (Konner, 2007; Lewis, 2010). Laughter builds as curiosity does; a typical 6-month-old laughs loudly upon discovering new things, particularly social experiences that balance familiarity and surprise, such as Daddy making a funny face. They prefer looking at happy faces rather than sad ones, even if the happy faces are not looking at them (Kim & Johnson, 2013).

ANGER AND SADNESS The positive emotions of joy and contentment are joined by negative emotions. Obvious anger appears by 6 months, usually triggered by frustration, such as when infants are prevented from moving or grabbing. Infants hate to be strapped in, caged in, closed in, or even just held in place when they want to explore.

In infancy, anger is generally a healthy response to frustration, unlike sadness, which also appears in the first months. Sadness indicates withdrawal and is accompanied by a greater increase in the body’s production of cortisol.

Since sadness produces physiological stress (measured by cortisol levels), sorrow negatively impacts the infant. All social emotions, particularly sadness and fear, probably shape the brain (Fries & Pollak, 2007; Johnson, 2011). As you learned in Chapter 3, experience matters. Sad and angry infants whose mothers are depressed become fearful toddlers (Dix & Yan, 2014). Too much sadness early in life correlates with depression later on.

FEAR Fear in response to some person, thing, or situation (not just being startled in surprise) is evident at about 9 months and soon becomes more frequent and obvious. Two kinds of social fear are typical:

separation anxiety

An infant’s distress when a familiar caregiver leaves; most obvious between 9 and 14 months.

stranger wariness

An infant’s expression of concern—a quiet stare while clinging to a familiar person, or a look of fear—when a stranger appears.

131

Developmentally Correct Both Santa’s smile and Olivia’s grimace are appropriate reactions for people of their age. Adults playing Santa must smile no matter what, and if Olivia smiled, that would be troubling to anyone who knows about 7-month-olds. Yet every Christmas, thousands of parents wait in line to put their infants on the laps of oddly dressed, bearded strangers.

Separation anxiety is normal at age 1, intensifies by age 2, and usually subsides after that. Fear of separation interferes with infant sleep. For example, infants who fall asleep next to familiar people may wake up frightened if they are alone (Sadeh et al., 2010). The solution is not necessarily to sleep with the baby, but neither is it to ignore the child’s natural fear of separation.

Some babies are comforted by a “transitional object,” such as a teddy bear or blanket, beside them as they transition from sleeping in their parents’ arms to sleeping alone. Music, a night light, or an open door may ease the fear.

Transitional objects are not pathological; they are the infant’s way of coping with anxiety. However, if separation anxiety remains strong after age 3 and impairs the child’s ability to leave home, go to school, or play with friends, it is considered an emotional disorder. Separation anxiety as a disorder can be diagnosed up to age 18 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013); some clinicians diagnose it in adults as well (Bögels et al., 2013).

Separation anxiety may be apparent outside the home. Strangers—especially those who do not look or move like familiar caregivers—merit stares, not smiles, at age 1. This is a good sign: Infant memory is active and engaged. Fear of strangers is normative, which means that every toddler typically manifests it, but genes and parents make it stronger (as when the child cries and hides) or weaker (with a wary look). Children whose parents are themselves anxious are likely to fear new people throughout childhood (Brooker et al., 2013).

Many normal 1-year-olds fear anything unexpected, from the flush of the toilet to the pop of a jack-in-the-box, from closing elevator doors to the tail-wagging approach of a dog. With repeated experience and reassurance, older infants might enjoy flushing the toilet (again and again) or calling the dog (crying if the dog does not come). Note the transition from instinct to learning to thought (Panksepp & Watt, 2011).

Toddlers’ Emotions

Emotions take on new strength during toddlerhood, as both memory and mobility advance (Izard, 2009). For example, throughout the second year and beyond, anger and fear become less frequent but more focused, targeted toward infuriating or terrifying experiences. Similarly, laughing and crying are louder and more discriminating. This is the familiar path from sensation to perception to cognition.

The new strength of emotions is apparent in temper tantrums. Toddlers are famous for fury. When something angers them, they might yell, scream, cry, hit, and throw themselves on the floor. Logic is beyond them; if adults respond with anger or teasing, that makes it worse.

One child was angry at her feet and said she did not want them. When a parent offered to get a scissors and cut them off, a new wail of tantrum erupted. With temper tantrums, soon sadness comes to the fore, at which time comfort—rather than acquiescence or punishment—is helpful (Green et al., 2011).

THINK CRITICALLY: Which is more annoying, people who brag or people who call themselves “stupid”?

SOCIAL AWARENESS Temper can be seen as an expression of selfhood. So can new toddler emotions: pride, shame, jealousy, embarrassment, disgust, and guilt. These emotions require social awareness, which typically emerges from family interactions. For instance, in a study of infant jealousy, when mothers deliberately paid attention to another infant, babies moved closer to their mothers, bidding for attention. Brain activity also registered social awareness (Mize et al., 2014).

Culture is crucial here, with independence a value in some families but not in others. Many North American parents encourage toddler pride (saying, “You did it yourself”—even when that is untrue), but Asian families typically cultivate modesty and shame. Such differences may still be apparent in adult personality and judgment.

132

My Finger, My Body, and Me Mirror self-recognition is particularly important in her case, as this 2-year-old has a twin sister. Parents need to make sure each child develops his or her own identity. Parents may enjoy dressing twins alike and giving them rhyming names, but each baby needs to forge an identity.

Disgust is also strongly influenced by other people. According to a study that involved children of various ages, many 18-month-olds (but not younger infants) express disgust at touching a dead animal. None, however, were disgusted when a teenager cursed at an elderly person—something that parents and older children often find disgusting (Stevenson et al., 2010).

self-awareness

A person’s realization that he or she is a distinct individual whose body, mind, and actions are separate from those of other people.

SELF-AWARENESS In addition to social awareness, another foundation for emotional growth is self-awareness, the realization that one’s body, mind, and activities are distinct from those of other people (Kopp, 2011). Closely following the new mobility that results from walking is an emerging sense of “me” and “mine” that leads the infant to develop a new consciousness of self and others at about age 1.

In a classic experiment (Lewis & Brooks, 1978), 9- to 24-month-olds looked into a mirror after a dot of rouge had been surreptitiously put on their noses. If they reacted by touching the red dot on their noses, that meant they knew the mirror reflected their own faces. None of the babies younger than 12 months did that, although they sometimes smiled and touched the dot on the “other” baby in the mirror.

However, at some time between 15 and 24 months, babies become self-aware, touching their noses with curiosity and puzzlement. Self-recognition in the mirror/rouge test (and in photographs) usually emerges at about 18 months, along with two other advances: pretending and using first-person pronouns (I, me, mine, myself, my) (Lewis, 2010).

As another scholar explains it, “an explicit and hence reflective conception of the self is apparent at the early stage of language acquisition at around the same age that infants begin to recognize themselves in mirrors” (Rochat, 2013, p. 388). This is yet another example of the interplay of all the infant abilities—walking, talking, and emotional self-understanding work together to make the 18-month-old a quite different person from the 8-month-old.

Brain and Emotions

Brain maturation is involved in the developments just described because every reaction begins in the brain (Johnson, 2011). Experience produces connections between neurons and emotions.

Links between expressed emotions and brain growth are complex and thus difficult to assess and describe with precision (Lewis, 2010). Compared with the emotions of adults, discrete emotions—the difference between fear and excitement, for instance—during early infancy are murky and unpredictable.

The growth of synapses and dendrites is a likely explanation for the gradual refinement and expression of each emotion, the result of past experiences and ongoing maturation. Already by 6 months, an infant’s brain patterns and stress hormones seem affected by caregiver responsiveness (Enlow et al., 2014).

LaunchPad

image

Video Activity: Self-Awareness and the Rouge Test shows the famous assessment of how and when self-awareness appears in infancy.

GROWTH OF THE BRAIN Many specific aspects of brain development support social emotions (Lloyd-Fox et al., 2009). For instance, the social smile and laughter appear as the cortex matures (Konner, 2010). The same is probably true for fear, self-awareness, and anger. The brains of 8-month-olds respond to other people who look or act afraid, and that probably enhances the infant’s own expression of fear (Missana et al., 2014).

Cultural differences become encoded in the infant brain, called “a cultural sponge” by one group of scientists (Ambady & Bharucha, 2009, p. 342). It is difficult to measure exactly how infant brains are molded by their context, in part because few parents give permission for scanning the brains of their normally developing infants. However, in one study (Zhu et al., 2007) adults—half born in the United States and half in China—were asked whether certain adjectives applied to them. For both groups, a particular area of the brain (the medial prefrontal cortex) was activated by that question. Then they were asked whether the same adjectives applied to their mothers. That medial prefrontal cortex was activated for the Chinese participants, but not for the U.S. ones.

133

Researchers consider this finding to be “neuroimaging evidence that culture shapes the functional anatomy of self-representation” (Zhu et al., 2007, p. 1310). They speculate that brain activation occurs because the Chinese participants learned, as babies, that they are closely aligned with their mothers, whereas the U.S. participants learned to be independent.

Beyond culture, an infant’s brain activity interacts with caregiver responses, probably in the first months of life as well as through the years of childhood (E. Nelson et al., 2014). Highly reactive 15-month-olds (who were quick to cry) with wonderfully responsive caregivers (not hostile or neglectful) became less fearful, less angry, and so on (Ursache et al., 2013).

By age 4, they could regulate their emotions, no longer bursting into tears at any distress. Presumably they had developed neurological links between brain excitement and emotional response, so their excitement was connected to thought. However, highly reactive toddlers whose caregivers were less responsive were often overwhelmed by their emotions later on (Ursache et al., 2013). Differential susceptibility is apparent: Innate reactions and caregiver actions together sculpt the brain.

LEARNING ABOUT OTHERS The tentative social smiles of 2-month-old infants to any face soon become quick and full smiles when they see their familiar caregivers. This occurs because, with repeated experience, the neurons that fire together become more closely and quickly connected to each other (via dendrites).

Social preferences form in the early months, connected not only with a person’s face but also with voice, touch, and smell. Early social awareness is one reason adopted children ideally join their new parents in the first days of life (a marked change from 100 years ago, when adoptions began after age 1).

Social awareness is also a reason to respect an infant’s reaction to a babysitter: If a 6-month-old screams and clings to the parent when the sitter arrives, another babysitter probably needs to be found. (Do not confuse this reaction with separation anxiety at 12 months—a normal, expected reaction.)

Every experience that a person has—especially in the early days and months—activates and prunes neurons, firing patterns from one axon to a dendrite reflect past learning.

As illustrated in Visualizing Development in Chapter 3, p. 95, this was first shown dramatically with baby mice: Some were licked and nuzzled by their mothers almost constantly, and some were neglected. A mother mouse’s licking of her newborn babies reduced methylation of a gene (Nr3c1), which allowed more serotonin (a neurotransmitter) to be released by the hypothalamus (a region of the brain discussed in Chapter 5).

Serotonin not only increased momentary pleasure (mice love being licked) but also started a chain of epigenetic responses to reduce cortisol from many parts of the brain and body, including the adrenal glands. The effects on both brain and behavior are lifelong for mice and probably for humans as well.

For many humans, social anxiety is stronger than any other anxiety. Certainly to some extent this is genetic. But epigenetic research finds environmental influences are important as well. This was clearly shown in a longitudinal study of 1,300 adolescents (twins and siblings): Their genetic tendency toward anxiety was evident at every age, but life events affected how strong that anxiety was felt (Zavos et al., 2012).

A smaller study likewise found that if the infant of an anxious biological mother is raised by a responsive adoptive mother who is not anxious, the inherited anxiety does not materialize (Natsuaki et al., 2013).

134

No Tears Needed In the first weeks of life, babies produce no tears. However, sadness is obvious—unlike adults who smile when tears betray them. Given what is known about the infant brain, we hope photography did not postpone baby-comforting.

Parents need to be comforting (as with the nuzzled baby mice) but not overprotective. Fearful mothers tend to raise fearful children, but fathers who offer their infants exciting but not dangerous challenges (such as a game of chase, crawling on the floor) reduce later anxiety (Majdandži´c et al., 2013).

STRESS Emotions are connected to brain activity and hormones; they are affected by genes, past experiences, and additional hormones and neurotransmitters. Many of the specifics are not yet understood, but one link is clear: Excessive fear and stress harms the developing brain.

The specific mechanism seems to be that normally “the mother’s presence acts as a social buffer,” keeping cortisol levels low and thus fear at bay. However, if that does not occur, the brain develops more rapidly than normal. In that case, a child who seems adultlike in some emotions later becomes impaired because the parts of the brain that respond to emotions developed too quickly (Tottenham, 2014).

Brain scans of children who were maltreated in infancy show abnormal responses to stress, anger, and other emotions later on, including to photographs of frightened people. Some children seem resilient, but many areas of the brain (the prefrontal cortex, discussed in Chapter 3, and the hypothalamus, amygdala, HPA axis, and hippocampus, which will be explained in later chapters) are affected by abuse, especially if the maltreatment begins before age 2 (Bernard et al., 2014; Cicchetti, 2013a).

Since early caregiving affects the brain lifelong, caregivers should be consistent and reassuring. This is not always easy. Remember that some infants cry inconsolably in the early weeks. One researcher notes:

Empathy Wins Crying babies whose caregivers sympathize often become confident, accomplished, and caring children. Sleep deprivation makes anyone unhappy, but this man’s response is much better for both of them than anger or neglect.

An infant’s crying has 2 possible consequences: it may elicit tenderness and desire to soothe, or helplessness and rage. It can be a signal that encourages attachment or one that jeopardizes the early relationship by triggering depression and, in some cases, even neglect or abuse.

[J. S. Kim, 2011, p. 229]

Sometimes mothers are blamed, or blame themselves, when an infant cries. This attitude is not helpful: A mother who feels guilty or incompetent may become angry at her baby, which leads to unresponsive parenting, an unhappy child, and then hostile interactions. (Presumably the results would be the same if the father, or grandmother, were the primary caregiver, although extensive research on those infant–caregiver relationships has not yet been published.) Years later, first-grade classmates and teachers are likely to consider children of unresponsive mothers disruptive and aggressive (Lorber & Egeland, 2011).

But the opposite may occur if early crying produces solicitous parenting. Then, when the baby outgrows the crying, the parent–child bond may be exceptionally strong.

A study of colicky babies found both reactions in the parents (Landgren et al., 2012). One mother said:

There were moments when, both me and my husband … when she was apoplectic and howling so much that I almost got this thought, ‘now I’ll take a pillow and put over her face just until she quietens down, until the screaming stops.’

By contrast, another mother said

In some way, it made me stronger, and made my relationship with my son stronger … Because I felt that he had no one else but me. ‘If I can´t manage, no one can.’ So I had to cope.

Remember from Chapter 3 that cross-modal perception occurs in infancy, making synesthesia more common than later on. A caregiver’s smell and voice may evoke a vision of that person, for instance, or a sound may be connected with a shape. For example, when 4-month-old infants hear words that seem more staccato—such as “kiki”—they are more likely to look at an angular shape than a round one (Ozturk et al., 2013).

135

The tendency of one part of the brain to activate another may also occur for emotions. An infant’s cry can be triggered by pain, fear, tiredness, surprise, or excitement; laughter can turn to tears. Infant emotions may erupt, increase, or disappear for unknown reasons (Camras & Shutter, 2010). Brain immaturity is a likely explanation.

Temperament

Video: Temperament in Infancy and Toddlerhood

temperament

Inborn differences between one person and another in emotions, activity, and self-regulation. It is measured by the person’s typical responses to the environment.

Temperament is defined as the “biologically based core of individual differences in style of approach and response to the environment that is stable across time and situations” (van den Akker et al., 2010, p. 485). “Biologically based” means that these traits originate with nature, not nurture. Confirmation that temperament arises from the inborn brain comes from an analysis of the tone, duration, and intensity of infant cries after the first inoculation, before much experience outside the womb. Cry variations at this very early stage correlate with later temperament (Jong et al., 2010).

Stranger Danger Some parents teach their children to be respectful of any adult; others teach them to fear any stranger. No matter what their culture or parents say, each of these two sisters in Nepal reacts according to her inborn temperament.

Temperament is not the same as personality, although temperamental inclinations may lead to personality differences. Generally, personality traits (e.g., honesty and humility) are learned, whereas temperamental traits (e.g., shyness and aggression) are genetic. Of course, for every trait, nature and nurture interact.

In laboratory studies of temperament, infants are exposed to events that are frightening or attractive. Four-month-olds might see spinning mobiles or hear unusual sounds. Older babies might confront a noisy, moving robot or a clown who quickly moves close to them. During such experiences, some children laugh, some cry, others are quiet, and still others exhibit a combination of these reactions.

Generally, three dimensions of temperament are found (Hirvonen et al., 2013; van den Akker et al., 2010; Degnan et al., 2011), each of which affects later personality and achievement.

Each of these dimensions is associated with distinctive brain patterns as well as behavior, with the last of these (exuberance versus shyness) most strongly traced to genes (Wolfe et al., 2014). Of course, all temperament traits are thought to be biologically based with a genetic component, but the distinction between temperament (biology) and personality (learned) is clearer on paper than in people. Social scientists sometimes interpret the data in opposite ways, as you will now see.

OPPOSING PERSPECTIVES

Mothers or Genes?

Traditionally, as you will later read, psychologists emphasized mothers. Their actions in the early years were thought to affect their child lifelong. Many adults credit, or blame, their mothers for their success and failure.

Recently, however, genetic research and neuroscience suggest a strong role for genes and neurotransmitters, making one person fearful and another foolhardy, one person angry and another sanguine, and so on. These studies often include photos of brain scans, and statistical analysis of monozygotic twins, leading many social scientists to be impressed by the biological basis for human differences.

Many neuroscientists seek to discover which alleles affect specific emotions. For example, researchers have found that the 7-repeat allele of the DRD4 VNTR gene, when combined with the 5-HTTLPR genotype, results in 6-month-olds who are difficult—often crying, hard to distract, slow to laugh (Holmboe et al., 2011; Windhorst et al., 2015). Infants with a particular allele of the MOA gene are reported to be quick to anger (Sung et al., 2015). You need not remember the letters of these alleles, but the data have convinced almost everyone that infant emotions vary partly for genetic reasons (M. H. Johnson & Fearon, 2011).

136

Many other scientists trace the traits of children to aspects of early caregiving and culture. For example, the same study that noted a link between MOA and infant anger compared Dutch and American babies, and it reported that culture was a crucial influence (Sung et al., 2015). The impact of the 7-repeat allele of DRD4 depends on a mother’s reaction to her difficult baby (Windhorst et al., 2015).

The most detailed, longitudinal study of temperament assessed the same individuals at 4, 9, 14, 24, and 48 months and again in middle childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. The scientists designed laboratory experiments with specifics appropriate for the age of the children; collected detailed reports from the mothers and later from the participants themselves; and gathered observational data and physiological evidence, including brain scans. Past data were reevaluated each time, and cross-sectional and international studies were considered (Fox et al., 2001, 2005, 2013; Hane et al., 2008; L. Williams et al., 2010; Jarcho et al., 2013).

Half of the participants did not change much over time, reacting the same way and having similar brain-wave patterns when confronted with frightening experiences. Curiously, the participants most likely to change from infancy to age 4 were the inhibited, fearful ones. Least likely to change were the exuberant babies (see Figure 4.1). That could be ascribed to the environment, because adults coax frightened infants to be brave but let exuberant children stay happy.

Figure 4.1: FIGURE 4.1 Do Babies’ Temperaments Change? Sometimes it is possible—especially if they were fearful babies. Adults who are reassuring help children overcome fearfulness. If fearful children do not change, it is not known whether that’s because their parents are not sufficiently reassuring (nurture) or because the babies themselves are temperamentally more fearful (nature).

The researchers found unexpected gender differences. As teenagers, the formerly inhibited boys were more likely than the average adolescent to use drugs, but the inhibited girls were less likely to do so (L. Williams et al., 2010). Perhaps shy boys use drugs to become less anxious, but shy girls may be more fearful of authority and more accepted as they are. Is this nature (sex hormones) or nurture (social expectations)?

Examination of these participants in adulthood again found intriguing differences between brain and behavior. Those who were inhibited in childhood still showed, in brain scans, evidence of their infant temperament. That confirms that biology affected their traits.

However, learning (specifically cognitive control) was also evident: Their outward behavior was similar to those with a more outgoing temperament, unless other factors caused serious emotional problems. Apparently, most of them had learned to override their initial temperament—not to erase social anxiety but to keep it from impairing adult behavior (Jarcho et al., 2013).

Continuity and change were also found in another study that found that angry infants were likely to make their mothers hostile toward them, and, if that happened, such infants became antisocial children. However, if the mothers were loving and patient, despite the difficult temperament of the children, hostile traits were not evident later on (Pickles et al., 2013).

The two trends evident in all these studies—continuity and improvement—have been replicated in many longitudinal studies of infant temperament, especially for antisocial personality traits. Difficult babies tend to become difficult children, but not always. Family and culture sometimes deflect negative outcomes.

The reason both opposing interpretations thrive may depend more on the person drawing the conclusions than on the babies. Some people are inclined to accept things as they are. They are likely to emphasize inborn traits that do not change. Other people believe that change is always possible, even likely. They seek ways that early caregiving, or the social context, or even the national political structure, shape behavior.

Which of these two folk sayings are you more likely to tell your friends?

A leopard cannot change his spots,

or

If at first you don’t succeed, try, try again.

137

WHAT HAVE YOU LEARNED?

Question 4.1

1. What are the first emotions to appear in infants?

Crying and contentment are present from birth. The social smile appears around 6 weeks of age. Infants express social joy and laughter between 2 and 4 months of age.

Question 4.2

2. Why is it better for an infant to express anger than sadness?

Anger is usually triggered by frustration. Sadness, however, usually indicates withdrawal and is accompanied by an increase in the body’s production of cortisol, the primary stress hormone.

Question 4.3

3. What do 1-year-olds fear?

Typical 1-year-old children fear strangers and separation from their caregivers. Many also fear anything unexpected, from the flush of the toilet to the pop of a jack-in-the-box.

Question 4.4

4. How do emotions differ between the first and second year of life?

Emotions take on new strength during toddlerhood. For example, anger and fear become less frequent but more focused, targeted toward especially infuriating or terrifying experiences. Similarly, laughing and crying are louder and more discriminating. Social awareness develops, ushering in the new emotions of pride, shame, embarrassment, disgust, and guilt.

Question 4.5

5. How do family interactions and culture shape toddlers’ emotions?

The expression of pride, shame, embarrassment, disgust, and guilt requires social awareness and self-awareness. Social awareness and self-awareness emerge from family interactions and are shaped by the culture. For example, North American parents encourage toddlers to feel proud of their accomplishments, but Asian families typically discourage pride and instead cultivate modesty and shame.

Question 4.6

6. What evidence is there that toddlers become more aware of themselves?

Evidence can be found in the classic experiment in which 9- to 24-month-olds looked into a mirror after a dot of rouge had been surreptitiously put on their noses. If a child touched the red dot on his nose, it proved that he knew the mirror reflected his own face. None of the babies younger than 12 months did that, but the babies between 15 and 24 months were self-aware, touching their noses with curiosity and puzzlement.

Question 4.7

7. What is known about the impact of brain maturation on emotions?

The social smile, laughter, fear, self-awareness, and anger appear as the cortex matures. The maturation of the anterior cingulate gyrus (a part of the cortex) is directly connected to emotional self-regulation, allowing a child to hide or express his or her feelings.

Question 4.8

8. What is not yet known about how brain maturation affects emotions?

It is still unknown how infant brains are molded by their environment and culture and how this affects their expression of emotions.

Question 4.9

9. How are memory and emotion connected?

All emotional reactions, particularly those connected to self-awareness, depend partly on memory. Memory is fragile at first and gradually improves over the first year. This explains why toddlers are more quickly angered than younger babies when teased by an older sibling. Unlike young infants, toddlers have vivid memories of the previous time a sibling frustrated them.

Question 4.10

10. How does stress affect early brain development?

Excessive stress harms the developing brain. For example, the hypothalamus grows more slowly if an infant is often frightened.

Question 4.11

11. What three dimensions of temperament are evident in children?

Effortful control (regulating attention and emotion, self-soothing); negative mood (fearful, angry, unhappy); exuberant (active, social, not shy)

Question 4.12

12. What is the difference between temperament and personality?

Generally, personality traits (like honesty and humility) are learned, whereas temperamental traits (like aggression or shyness) are genetic. Of course, for every trait, nature and nurture interact.

Question 4.13

13. Why are temperament traits more apparent in some people than others?

Although temperament originates with genes, the expression of emotions over the life span is modified by experience—the result of child-rearing methods, culture, and learning. This means that in some people, certain temperaments are more apparent.