12.1 Building on Theory

Learning is rapid. By age 11, some children beat their elders at chess, play music that adults pay to hear, publish poems, win trophies. Others survive on the streets or kill in wars, learning lessons that no child should know. How do they learn so quickly?

Piaget and School-Age Children

concrete operational thought Plaget’s term for the ability to reason logically about direct experiences and perceptions.

Piaget called the cognition of middle childhood concrete operational thought, characterized by new concepts that enable children to use logic. Operational comes from the Latin word operare, meaning “to work; to produce.” By calling this period operational, Piaget emphasized productive thinking.

The school-age child, no longer limited by egocentrism, performs logical operations. Children apply their new reasoning skills to concrete situations. Those are situations that are concrete, like a cement sidewalk; they are visible, tangible, and real (not abstract). A shift from preoperational to concrete operational occurs between ages 5 and 7: Children become more systematic, objective, scientific—and educable.

Product of Cognition Concrete thinking is specific, such as caring for a lamb until it becomes an award-winning sheep, as this New Jersey 4-H member did.
© KELLY-MOONEY PHOTOGRAPHY/CORBIS

340

A Hierarchy of Categories

classification The logical principle that things can be organized into groups (or categories or classes) according to some characteristic they have in common.

One logical operation is classification, the organization of things into groups (or categories or classes) according to some characteristic that they share. For example, family includes parents, siblings, and cousins. Other common classes are animals, toys, and food. Each class includes some elements and excludes others; each is part of a hierarchy.

Food, for instance, is an overarching category, with the next-lower level of the hierarchy being meat, grains, fruits, and so on. Most subclasses can be further divided: Meat includes poultry, beef, and pork, each of which can be divided again. Adults realize that items at the bottom of a classification hierarchy belong to every higher level: Bacon is always pork, meat, and food, but most food, meat, and pork are not bacon. However, the mental operation of moving up and down the hierarchy are beyond preoperational children.

Piaget devised many classification experiments. For example, a child is shown a bunch of nine flowers—seven yellow daisies and two white roses. Then the child is asked: “Are there more daisies or more flowers?”

Until about age 7, most children answer, “More daisies.” The youngest children offer no justification, but some 6-year-olds explain that “there are more yellow ones than white ones” or “because daisies are daisies, they aren’t flowers” (Piaget et al., 2001). By age 8, most children can classify: “More flowers than daisies,” they say.

Other Logical Concepts

Several logical concepts were already discussed in Chapter 9 in the explanation of ideas that are beyond preoperational children, such as conservation and reversibility.

seriation The concept that things can be arranged in a logical series, such as the number sequence or the alphabet.

Another example of concrete logic is seriation, the knowledge that things can be arranged in a logical series. Seriation is crucial for using (not merely memorizing) the alphabet or the number sequence. By age 5, most children can count up to 100, but because they do not yet grasp seriation they cannot correctly estimate where any particular two-digit number would be placed on a line that starts at 0 and ends at 100 (Meadows, 2006).

Concrete operational thought correlates with primary school math achievement, although many other factors contribute (Desoete et al., 2009). For example, logic helps with arithmetic: Children at the stage of concrete operational thought eventually understand that 12 + 3 = 3 + 12, and that 15 is always 15 (both conservation), that all the numbers from 20 to 29 are in the 20s (classification), that 134 is lower than 143 (seriation), and that if 5 × 3 = 15, then 15 ÷ 5 is 3 (reversibility). [Lifespan Link: These four concepts are explained in Chapter 9.]

Math and Money Third-grader Perry Akootchook understands basic math, so he might beat his mother at “spinning for money” shown here. Compare his concrete operational skills with that of a typical preoperational child, who would not be able to play this game and might give a dime for a nickel.
© SAM HARREL/ZUMA PRESS/CORBIS

The Significance of Piaget’s Findings

Although logic connects to math, researchers find more continuity than discontinuity as children master number skills. Thus, Piaget’s stage idea was mistaken: There is no sudden shift between preoperational and concrete operational intelligence.

Nonetheless, Piaget’s experiments revealed something important. School-age children use mental categories and subcategories more flexibly, inductively, and simultaneously than younger children can. They are more advanced thinkers, capable in ways that younger children are not.

341

Vygotsky and School-Age Children

Like Piaget, Vygotsky felt that educators should consider children’s thought processes, not just the outcomes. He appreciated the fact that children are curious, creative learners. For that reason, Vygotsky believed that an educational system based on rote memorization rendered the child “helpless in the face of any sensible attempt to apply any of this acquired knowledge” (Vygotsky, 1934/1994, pp. 356–357).

The Role of Instruction

Especially for Teachers How might Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s ideas help in teaching geography to a class of third-graders?

Response for Teachers: Here are two of the most obvious ways. (1) Use logic. Once children can grasp classification and class inclusion, they can understand cities within states, states within nations, and nations within continents. Organize your instruction to make logical categorization easier. (2) Make use of children’s need for concrete and personal involvement. You might have the children learn first about their own location, then about the places where relatives and friends live, and finally about places beyond their personal experience (via books, photographs, videos, and guest speakers).

Unlike Piaget, Vygotsky stressed the centrality of instruction. For Vygotsky, school can be crucial for cognitive growth. He thought that peers and teachers provide the bridge between developmental potential and needed skills via guided participation and scaffolding, in the zone of proximal development. [Lifespan Link: Vygotsky’s theory is discussed in Chapters 2 and 9.]

Confirmation of the role of social interaction and instruction comes from a U.S. study of children who, because of their school’s entry-date cutoff, are either relatively old kindergartners or quite young first-graders. At the end of the school year, achievement scores of the 6-year-old first-graders far exceeded those of kindergarten 6-year-olds who were only one month younger (Lincove & Painter, 2006). Obviously, they had learned a great deal from first grade.

Internationally as well, children who begin first grade earlier tend to be ahead in academic achievement compared to those who enter later, an effect noted even at age 15. The author of this study noted that Vygotsky’s explanation is not the only one, and that these results were not found in every nation (Sprietsma, 2010). However, no matter what explanation is correct, children’s academic achievement seems influenced by social context.

Vygotsky would certainly agree with that, and he would explain those national differences by noting that the education of some nations is far better than others. Remember that Vygotsky believed education occurs everywhere, not only in school. Children learn as they play with peers, watch television, eat with their families, walk down the street. Every experience, from birth on, teaches them something, with some contexts much more educational than others.

Girls Can’t Do It As Vygotsky recognized, children learn whatever their culture teaches. Fifty years ago girls were in cooking and sewing classes. No longer. This 2012 photo shows 10-year-olds Kamrin and Caitlin in a Kentucky school, preparing for a future quite different from that of their grandmothers.
MIRANDA PEDERSON/DAILY NEWS/ASSOCIATED PRESS

For instance, a study of the reading and math achievement of more than a thousand third- and fifth-grade children from ten U.S. cities found that high-scoring primary school children were likely to have had extensive cognitive stimulation. There were three main sources of intellectual activity:

  1. Families (e.g., parents read to them daily when they were toddlers)
  2. Preschool programs (e.g., a variety of learning activities)
  3. First-grade curriculum (e.g., emphasis on literacy with individual evaluation)

In this study, although most children from families of low socioeconomic status did not experience all three sources of stimulation, those who did showed more cognitive advances by fifth grade than the average high-SES child (Crosnoe et al., 2010).

Generally, poverty reduces children’s achievement because they are less likely to have these three. However, for low-SES children especially, maternal education makes a notable difference in academic achievement—presumably because educated mothers read, listen, and talk to their children more, and find a preschool with a curriculum that encourages learning.

342

International Contexts

In general, Vygotsky’s emphasis on sociocultural contexts contrasts with Piaget’s maturational, self-discovery approach. Vygotsky believed that cultures (tools, customs, and mentors) teach. For example, if a child is surrounded by reading adults, by full bookcases, by daily newspapers, and by street signs, that child will read better than a child who has had little exposure to print, even if both are in the same classroom.

The same applies to math. If children learn math in school, they are proficient at school math; if they learn math out of school, they are adept at solving mathematical problems in situations similar to the context in which they learned (Abreu, 2008). Ideally, though, children learn math both in and out of school.

Never Lost These children of Varanasi sleep beside the Ganges in the daytime. At night they use their excellent sense of direction to guide devotees from elsewhere.
©ARKO DATTA/X01337/REUTERS/CORBIS

Context affects more than academic learning. A stunning example of knowledge acquired from the social context comes from Varanasi, a city in northeast India. Many Varanasi children have an extraordinary sense of spatial orientation: They know whether they are facing north or south, even when they are inside a room with no windows. In one experiment, children were blindfolded, spun around, and led to a second room, yet many still knew which way they were now facing (Mishra et al., 2009). How did they know?

In Varanasi, everyone refers to the spatial orientation to locate objects. (The U.S. equivalent might be, not that the dog is sleeping by the door, but that the dog is sleeping southeast.) From their early days, children learned north/south/east/west, in order to communicate with others. By middle childhood, their internal sense of direction was acute.

Culture affects how children learn, not just what they learn. This was evident in a two-session study in California of 80 Mexican American children, each with a sibling (Silva et al., 2010). Half of the sibling pairs were from indigenous Indian families, in which children learn by watching, guided by other children. The other half were from families more acculturated to U.S. norms, learning by direct instruction, not observation. Those children had learned that they should work independently, sitting at desks, not collaboratively, not crowding around a teacher.

In the first session of this study, a Spanish-speaking “toy lady” showed each child how to make a toy while his or her sibling sat nearby. First, the younger sibling waited while the older sibling made a toy mouse, and then the older sibling waited while the younger sibling made a toy frog. Each waiting child’s behavior was videotaped and coded every 5 seconds as sustained attention (alert and focused on the activity), glancing (sporadic interest, but primary focus elsewhere), or not attending (looking away).

A week later, each child individually was told there was some extra material to make the toy that his or her sibling had made the week before, and encouraged to make the mouse or the frog (whichever one that child had not already made.) In this second session, the toy lady did not give the children step-by-step instructions as she had for the sibling a week earlier, but she had a long list of possible hints if the child needed help.

The purpose of this experiment was to see how much the children had learned by observation the week before. The children from indigenous backgrounds scored higher, needing fewer hints, because they had been more attentive when their siblings made the toy (Silva et al., 2010) (see Figure 12.1).

Two Ways to Learn Even when children currently live in the same settings and attend the same schools, they follow family cultural traditions in the way they learn.
Data from Silva et al., 2010.

343

The same conclusions have been found in other research. For example, in another study, children born and raised in the United States who are accustomed to learning by observation (as in some American Indian cultures) were more proficient at remembering an overheard folktale (Tsethlikai & Rogoff, 2013).

Information Processing

Today’s educators and psychologists regard both Piaget and Vygotsky as insightful. International research confirms the merits of their theories. Piaget described universal changes; Vygotsky noted cultural impact.

A third, and more recent, approach to understanding cognition adds crucial insight. The information-processing perspective benefits from technology that allows much more detailed data and analysis than was possible 50 years ago. [Lifespan Link: Information processing is introduced in Chapter 2.]

Thousands of researchers who study cognition can be said to use the information-processing approach. Not all of them would identify themselves as such because some people contend that “information processing is not a single theory but, rather, a framework characterizing a large number of research programs” (Miller, 2011, p. 266).

The basic assumption of all those research programs is that, like computers, people can access large amounts of information. They then: (1) seek specific units of information (as a search engine does), (2) analyze (as software programs do), and (3) express their conclusions so that another person can understand (as a networked computer or a printout might do). By tracing the paths and links of each of these functions, scientists better understand the learning process.

The brain’s gradual growth, now seen in neurological scans, confirms the usefulness of the information-processing perspective. So does data on children’s school achievement: Absences, vacations, new schools, and even new teachers may set back a child’s learning because learning each day builds on the previous day. Brain connections and pathways are forged from repeated experiences, allowing advances in processing. Without careful building and repetition of various skills, fragile connections between neurons break.

344

Skills of the Street Children In many nations, children sell to visitors, using math and business skills that few North American children know. This boy was offering necklaces to visitors at the Blue Mosque in Afghanistan.
JANE SWEENEY/LONELY PLANET IMAGES/GETTY IMAGES

One of the leaders of the information-processing perspective is Robert Siegler. He has studied the day-by-day details of children’s cognition in math (Siegler & Chen, 2008). Apparently, children do not suddenly grasp the logic of the number system, as Piaget expected at the concrete operational stage. Instead, number understanding accrues gradually, with new and better strategies for calculation tried, ignored, half-used, abandoned, and finally adopted.

Siegler compared the acquisition of knowledge to waves on a beach when the tide is rising. There is substantial ebb and flow, although eventually a new level is reached.

An example is children’s ability to estimate where a number might fall on a line, such as where the number 53 would be placed on a line from zero to 100. This skill predicts later math achievement (Libertus et al., 2013). U.S. kindergartners are usually lost when asked to do this task; Chinese kindergartners are somewhat better (Siegler & Mu, 2008), but proficiency gradually builds from the first grade on, predicting later math skills (Feigenson et al., 2013). This has led many information-processing experts to advocate giving children practice with number lines in order to develop math, such as multiplication and division.

Curiously, knowing how to count to high numbers seem less important for math mastery than these estimates of magnitude (Thompson & Siegler, 2010). For example, understanding the size of fractions (e.g., that 3/16 is smaller than 1/4) is connected to a thorough understanding of the relationship between one number and another (Siegler et al., 2011). Overall, information processing guides teachers who want to know exactly which concepts and skills are crucial foundations for mastery, not only for math but for reading and writing as well.

Memory

Many scientists who study memory take an information-processing approach. They have learned that various methods of input, storage, and retrieval affect the increasing cognitive ability of the schoolchild. Each of the three major steps in the memory process—sensory memory, working memory, and long-term memory—is affected by both maturation and experience.

sensory memory The component of the information-processing system in which incoming stimulus information is stored for a split second to allow it to be processed. (Also called the sensory register.)

Sensory memory (also called the sensory register) is the first component of the human information-processing system. It stores incoming stimuli for a split second, with sounds retained slightly longer than sights. To use terms explained in Chapter 5, sensations are retained for a moment, and then some become perceptions. This first step of sensory awareness is already quite good in early childhood.

working memory The component of the information-processing system in which current conscious mental activity occurs. (Formerly called short-term memory.)

Once some sensations become perceptions, the brain selects the meaningful ones and transfers them to working memory for further analysis. It is in working memory (formerly called short-term memory) that current, conscious mental activity occurs. Processing, not mere exposure, is essential for getting information into working memory; for this reason, working memory improves markedly in middle childhood (Cowan & Alloway, 2009) (see Table 12.1).

Table : TABLE 12.1Advances in Memory from Infancy to Age 11
Child’s Age Memory Capabilities
Under 2 years Infants remember actions and routines that involve them. Memory is implicit, triggered by sights and sounds (an interactive toy, a caregiver’s voice).
2-5 years Words are now used to encode and retrieve memories. Explicit memory begins, although children do not yet use memory strategies. Children remember things by rote (their phone number, nursery rhymes).
5-7 years Children realize they need to remember some things, and they try to do so, usually via rehearsal (repeating an item again and again). This is not the most efficient strategy, but repetition can lead to automatization.
7-9 years Children can learn new strategies, including visual clues (remembering how a particular spelling word looks) and auditory hints (rhymes, letters), evidence of brain functions called the visual-spatial sketchpad and phonological loop. Children benefit from organizing things to be remembered.
9-11 years Memory becomes more adaptive and strategic as children become able to learn various memory techniques from teachers and other children. They can organize material themselves, developing their own memory aids.
Source: Based on Meadows, 2006.

As Siegler’s waves metaphor suggests, memory strategies do not appear suddenly. Gradual improvement occurs from toddlerhood through adolescence (Schneider & Lockl, 2008). Children develop strategies to increase working memory (Camos & Barrouillet, 2011), and they use these strategies occasionally at first, then consistently.

Cultural differences are evident. For example, many Muslim children are taught to memorize all 80,000 words of the Quran, so they learn strategies to remember long passages—strategies unknown to non-Muslim children. A very different example is the ability to draw a face, an ability admired by U.S. children. They learn strategies to improve their drawing, such as remembering the ratios of distance involving forehead, eyes, mouth, and chin. (Few spontaneously draw the eyes mid-face, rather than at the top, but most learn to do so.)

345

long-term memory The component of the information-processing system in which virtually limitless amounts of information can be stored indefinitely.

Finally, information from working memory may be transferred long-term memory, where it is stored for minutes, hours, days, months, or years. The capacity of long-term memory—how much can be crammed into one brain—is huge by the end of middle childhood. Together with sensory memory and working memory, long-term memory organizes ideas and reactions, with more effective brain functioning over the years (Wendelken et al., 2011).

Especially for Teachers How might your understanding of memory help you teach a 2,000-word vocabulary list to a class of fourth-graders?

Response for Teachers: Children this age can be taught strategies for remembering by forming links between working memory and long-term memory. You might break down the vocabulary list into word clusters, grouped according to root words, connections to the children’s existing knowledge, applications, or (as a last resort) first letters or rhymes. Active, social learning is useful; perhaps in groups the students could write a story each day that incorporates 15 new words. Each group could read its story aloud to the class.

Crucial to long-term memory is not merely storage (how much material has been deposited) but also retrieval (how readily past learning can be brought into working memory). For everyone, at every age, retrieval is easier for some memories (especially memories of vivid, emotional experiences) than for others. And for everyone, long-term memory is imperfect: We all forget and distort memories, with strategies needed for accurate recall. [Lifespan Link: See Chapter 24 for discussion of memory and memory strategies in late adulthood.]

Knowledge

knowledge base A body of knowledge in a particular area that makes it easier to master new information in that area.

Research on information processing finds that the more people already know, the more information they can learn. Having an extensive knowledge base, or a broad body of knowledge in a particular subject, makes it easier to remember and understand related new information. As children gain knowledge during the school years, they become better able to understand what is true or not, what is worth remembering, and what is insignificant (Woolley & Ghossainy, 2013)

Three factors facilitate increases in the knowledge base: past experience, current opportunity, and personal motivation. The last item in this list explains why children’s knowledge base is not what their parents or teachers prefer. Some schoolchildren memorize words and rhythms of hit songs, know plots and characters of television programs, or can recite the names and histories of basketball players, and yet do not know whether World War I was in the nineteenth or twentieth century, or whether Pakistan is in Asia or Africa.

346

Motivation provides a clue for teachers: New concepts are learned best if they are connected to personal and emotional experiences. Children who themselves are from South Asia, or who have friends who are, learn the boundaries of Pakistan more readily.

Control Processes

control processes Mechanisms (including selective attention, metacognition, and emotional regulation) that combine memory, processing speed, and knowledge to regulate the analysis flow of information within the information-processing system. (Also called executive processes.)

The mechanisms that put memory, processing speed, and the knowledge base together are control processes; they regulate the analysis and flow of information within the system. Control processes include emotional regulation and s elective attention (explained in Chapters 10 and 11, respectively).

metacognition “Thinking about thinking,” or the ability to evaluate a cognitive task in order to determine how best to accomplish it, and then to monitor and adjust one’s performance on that task.

Equally important is metacognition, sometimes defined as “thinking about thinking.” Metacognition is the ultimate control process because it allows a person to evaluate a cognitive task, determine how to accomplish it, monitor performance, and then make adjustments. According to scholars of cognition, “Middle childhood may be crucial for the development of metacognitive monitoring and study of control processes” (Metcalfe & Finn, 2013, p. 19).

Control processes require the brain to organize, prioritize, and direct mental operations, much as the CEO (chief executive officer) of a business organizes, prioritizes, and directs business operations. For that reason, control processes are also called executive processes. These processes are evident whenever people concentrate on only the relevant parts of a task, using their knowledge base to connect new information or to apply memory strategies.

Executive processes are more evident among 10-year-olds than among 4- or 6-year-olds (Bjorklund et al., 2009). Fourth-grade students can listen to the teacher talk about the river Nile, ignoring classmates who are chewing gum or passing notes. That deliberate selectivity is control.

Both metacognition and control processes improve with age and experience. For instance, in one study, children took a fill-in-the-blanks test and indicated how confident they were of each answer. Then they were allowed to delete some questions, with the remaining ones counting more. Already by age 9, they were able to estimate correctness; by age 11, they were skilled at knowing what to delete (Roebers et al., 2009).

Sometimes experience is not directly related, but it nonetheless has an impact. This seems to be true for fluently bilingual children, who must learn to inhibit one language while using another. They are advanced in control processes, obviously in language but also in more abstract measures of control (Bialystok, 2010).

Such processes develop spontaneously, as the prefrontal cortex matures, but they can be taught. Sometimes teaching is explicit, more so in some nations (e.g., Germany) than in others (e.g., the United States) (Bjorklund et al., 2009). Examples that may be familiar include spelling rules (“i before e except after c”) and ways to remember how to turn a lightbulb (lefty, loosey, righty, tighty). Preschoolers ignore such rules or use them only on command, 7-year-olds begin to use them, and 9-year-olds can create and master more complicated rules.

Many factors beyond specific instruction affect learning. For example, if children do not master emotional control in early childhood, their school achievement is likely to suffer for years (Bornstein et al., 2013).

Given the complexity of factors and goals, educators disagree as to what should be deliberately taught versus what is best discovered by the child. However, understanding the early steps that lead to later knowledge, as information processing seeks to do, may guide instruction and hence improve learning. That is one possible conclusion from an interesting experiment (see a View from Science).

347

A VIEW FROM SCIENCE

Balls Rolling Down

Should metacognition be taught, or should children develop it spontaneously when they are old enough? This question has been the focus of decades of research (Lee & Anderson, 2013; Orlich et al., 2009; Pressley & Hilden, 2006). Scholars have considered both “discovery” learning (inspired by Piaget) and explicit teaching (from an information-processing perspective), always with awareness of cultural differences (as Vygotsky stressed).

The answer depends partly on cultural goals and methods. Some cultures value single-minded concentration, others multitasking; some stress direct instruction, others self-exploration, and still others social learning.

Most U. S. schools now focus on achievement as measured by tests. That has led to emphasis on instruction and research to support the strategy of explicit teaching. In one study, researchers sought to teach children that a scientific experiment must measure variables one at a time in order to be valid (Klahr & Nigam, 2004). The researchers showed 112 third- and fourth-graders two balls that could roll down several ramps (see Figure 12.2). There were four variables: golf or rubber ball, steep or shallow slope, smooth or rough ramp, long or short downhill run.

Confounded Experiment On each of these two ramps, children could vary the steepness, surface, and length of the ramp as well as the type of ball. The confounded experiment depicted here contrasts (a) a golf ball on a steep, smooth, short ramp with (b) a rubber ball on a shallow, rough, long ramp.
Source: Klahr & Nigam, 2004.

First, the children were asked to design four experiments on their own: two to determine the effect of distance and two to determine the effect of steepness. Only 8 of the 112 children designed experiments that controlled the variables. For example, in an uncontrolled experiment, a child might use a golf ball on a long ramp and a rubber ball on a short ramp. With the variables uncontrolled, the results would be confounded (inappropriately combined).

The 104 children who did not spontaneously control the variables were then divided into two groups. Half were told to continue to create their own experiments; the other half received explicit instruction by watching an experimenter create pairs of demonstrations. For that half, the experimenter asked the children whether a demonstrated pair allowed them to “tell for sure” how a particular variable affected the distance traveled by the ball. After each response, the experimenter provided the correct answer and explained it, emphasizing the importance of testing one variable at a time.

Then all 104 children were asked to design four experiments, as before. Far more children who received direct instruction (40 of 52) correctly isolated the variables than did children who explored on their own (12 of 52).

A week later, those children who seemed to understand (the 40 and the 12) were asked to examine two science posters ostensibly created by 11-year-olds. The researcher asked the children for suggestions that would make the posters “good enough to enter in a state science fair.” The 40 children who had been instructed were virtually as perceptive in their critiques as the 12 who had learned through discovery. This study suggests that strategy can be taught—if the teacher actively engages the students. That is exactly what information-processing theory would predict.

Of course, scientific understanding is about more than understanding variables: It is about questioning conclusions and realizing that answers can and do change. How children develop this ability—whether by discovery and personal exploration as Piaget might expect, or whether by explicit instruction as information-processing theory suggests—is a matter of intense concern to educators.

348

A recent experiment involving computer simulation with several classes of German 13-year-olds found that some explicit instruction helped, but that “‘too much’ instructional support can constrain knowledge acquisition” (Eckhardt et al., 2013, p. 120). The proper balance between self-propelled discovery and teacher-provided instruction may depend on exactly what is being taught by whom and to whom.

Many educators fear that the current political climate stresses specific facts, taught explicitly, over broader concepts, grasped by discovery. Psychologists see merit in both strategies, as well as in learning by observation (Lee & Anderson, 2013; Tsethlikai & Rogoff, 2013). What do you think? And, as Vygotsky would ask, “How does your cultural heritage and political persuasion affect your answer?”

SUMMING UP

Every theory of cognitive development recognizes that school-age children are avid learners who actively build on the knowledge they already have. Piaget emphasized children’s own logic, with maturation and experience allowing them to reach the stage he called concrete operational. Research inspired by Vygotsky and the sociocultural perspective reveals that cultural differences can be powerful: Both what is learned and how it is learned are influenced by the context of instruction and everyday experience.

An information-processing analysis highlights the many components of thinking that advance, step-by-step, during middle childhood. Although sensory and long-term memory do not change much during these years, the speed and efficiency of working memory improve dramatically, making school-age children better thinkers as well as more strategic learners as they grow older. With every passing year children expand their knowledge base, which makes new material easier to connect with past learning and thus easier to learn. As control processes and metacognition advance, children are better able to direct their minds toward whatever they want to learn.