Responding to all these data, developmentalists are now looking closely at patterns of cognitive gain and loss. They contend that, because virtually every pattern is possible, it is misleading to ask whether intelligence either increases or decreases; it does not move in lockstep. There may be “vast domains of cognitive performance … that may not follow a common, age-
Many psychologists describe distinct intellectual abilities, each of which independently rises and falls. Math slowdowns are apparent even by age 40, but verbal ability keeps rising until the 60s (Schaie, 2013). Most current researchers agree that there are many forms of intelligence (Roberts & Lipnevich, 2012).
We consider here only two proposals, one that posits two distinct abilities and the other, three. [Lifespan Link: Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences describes nine abilities, an idea with implications for childhood education, as explained in Chapter 11.]
In the 1960s, a leading personality researcher, Raymond Cattell, teamed up with a promising graduate student, John Horn, to study intelligence tests. They concluded that adult intelligence is best understood by grouping various measures into two categories, which they called fluid and crystallized.
fluid intelligence Those types of basic intelligence that make learning of all sorts quick and thorough. Abilities such as short-
As its name implies, fluid intelligence is like water, flowing to its own level no matter where it happens to be. Fluid intelligence is quick and flexible, enabling people to learn anything, even things that are unfamiliar and unconnected to what they already know. Curiosity, learning for the joy of it, solving abstract puzzles, and the thrill at discovering something new are marks of fluid intelligence (Silvia & Sanders, 2010).
People high in fluid abilities can draw inferences, understand relationships between concepts, and readily process new ideas and facts in part because their working memory is large and flexible. Someone high in fluid intelligence is quick and creative with words and numbers and enjoys intellectual puzzles. The kinds of questions that test fluid intelligence among Western adults might be:
What comes next in each of these two series
4 9 1 6 2 5 3
V X Z B D
The correct answers are 6 and F. The clue is to think of multiplication (squares) and the alphabet: Some series are much more difficult to complete.
609
Puzzles are often used to measure fluid intelligence, with speedy solutions given bonus points (as on many IQ tests). Immediate recall—
Since fluid intelligence appears to be disconnected from past learning, it may seem impractical—
The ability to detoxify stress may be one reason that high fluid intelligence in emerging adulthood leads to longer life and higher IQ later on. Fluid intelligence is associated with openness to new experiences and overall brain health (Batterham et al., 2009; Silvia & Sanders, 2010). (Ways to cope with stress are discussed later in this chapter, when you will see that adaptive cognition provides the best defense against the problems of life.)
crystallized intelligence Those types of intellectual ability that reflect accumulated learning. Vocabulary and general information are examples. Some developmental psychologists think crystallized intelligence increases with age, while fluid intelligence declines.
The accumulation of facts, information, and knowledge as a result of education and experience is called crystallized intelligence. The size of a person’s vocabulary, the knowledge of chemical formulas, and the long-
What is the meaning of the word eleemosynary?
Who was Descartes?
Explain the difference between a tangent and a triangle.
Why does the city of Peking no longer exist?
Although such questions seem to measure achievement more than aptitude, these two are connected, especially in adulthood. Intelligent adults read widely, think deeply, and remember what they learn, so their achievement reflects their aptitude. Thus crystallized intelligence is an outgrowth of fluid intelligence (Nisbett et al., 2012).
Vocabulary, for example, improves with reading. Using the words joy, ecstasy, bliss, and delight—each appropriately, with distinct nuances (quite apart from the drugs, perfumes, or yogurts that use these names)—is a sign of intelligence. Remember the knowledge base (Chapter 12): As people know more, they learn more. That explains why a person’s extent of education is considered a rough indicator of adult IQ (Nisbett et al., 2012).
610
To reflect the total picture of a person’s intellectual aptitude, both fluid and crystallized intelligence must be measured (Hunt, 2011). Age complicates the IQ calculation because scores on items measuring fluid intelligence decrease with age, whereas scores on items measuring crystallized intelligence increase.
Scores on subtests tend to increase or decrease over time. Typically, speed and verbal ability change in opposite ways. The combination makes IQ fairly steady from ages 30 to 70. Although brain slowdown begins at age 20 or so, it is rarely apparent until massive declines in fluid intelligence affect crystallized intelligence. Only then do overall IQ scores fall.
Horn and Cattell (1967) wrote that they had:
shown intelligence to both increase and decrease with age—
[Horn & Cattell, p. 124]
In other words, it may be foolish to try to measure g, a single omnibus intelligence, because components need to be measured separately. In testing for g, real developmental changes will be masked because fluid and crystallized abilities cancel each other out.
Robert Sternberg (1988, 2003, 2011) agrees that a single intelligence score is misleading. As first mentioned in Chapter 11, Sternberg proposed three fundamental forms of intelligence: analytic, creative, and practical, each of which can be tested. (See Table 21.1.)
Related Issues |
Analytic Intelligence |
Creative Intelligence |
Practical Intelligence |
---|---|---|---|
Mental processes involved |
|
|
|
Valued for |
|
|
|
Indicated by |
|
|
|
Source: Based on Sternberg, 1988, 2003, 2011. |
analytic intelligence A form of intelligence that involves such mental processes as abstract planning, strategy selection, focused attention, and information processing, as well as verbal and logical skills.
Analytic intelligence includes all the mental processes that foster academic proficiency by making efficient learning, remembering, and thinking possible. Thus, it draws on abstract planning, strategy selection, focused attention, and information processing, as well as on verbal and logical skills.
Strengths in those areas are valuable in emerging adulthood, particularly in college, and in graduate school. Multiple-
creative intelligence A form of intelligence that involves the capacity to be intellectually flexible and innovative.
Creative intelligence involves the capacity to be intellectually flexible and innovative. Creative thinking is divergent rather than convergent, valuing the unexpected, imaginative, and unusual, rather than standard and conventional answers. Sternberg developed tests of creative intelligence that include writing a short story titled “The Octopus’s Sneakers” or planning an advertising campaign for a new doorknob. Those with many unusual ideas earn high scores.
611
practical intelligence The intellectual skills used in everyday problem solving. (Sometimes called tacit intelligence.)
Practical intelligence involves the capacity to adapt one’s behavior to the demands of a given situation. This capacity includes an accurate grasp of the expectations and needs of the people involved and an awareness of the particular skills that are called for, along with the ability to use these insights effectively. Practical intelligence is sometimes called tacit intelligence because it is not obvious on tests. Instead it comes from “the school of hard knocks” and is sometimes called “street smarts,” not “book smarts.”
The benefits of practical intelligence in adult life are obvious once we recall the cognitive tasks of adulthood. Few adults need to define obscure words or deduce the next element in a number sequence (analytic intelligence); and few need to write a new type of music, restructure local government, or invent a new gadget (creative intelligence). Ideally, those few have already found a niche for themselves and have learned to rely on people with practical intelligence to implement their analytic or creative ideas.
Almost every adult, however, must solve real-
Practical intelligence is very useful. Without it, a solution found by analytic intelligence will fail because people resist academic brilliance as unrealistic and elite, as the term ivory tower implies. Similarly, a stunningly creative idea may be rejected as ridiculous and weird rather than serious and sensible—
For example, imagine a business manager, a school principal, a political leader, or a parent without practical intelligence trying to change routine practices—
612
Flexibility is needed for practical intelligence (K. Sloan, 2009). As you remember from Chapter 18, this develops during emerging adulthood. Ideally, practical intelligence continues to be refined throughout adulthood as each new experience provides practice. Failures can be either defeats or learning opportunities.
No abstract test can assess practical intelligence because context is crucial. Instead, to measure this kind of intelligence, adults need to be observed coping with daily life. In hiring a new employee, the hiring committee might describe an actual situation and ask how the applicant would handle it. Many companies use such situational tests to hire a manager (Salter & Highhouse, 2009). For instance, the prospective employee is asked:
You assign a new project to one of your subordinates, who protests, saying it cannot be done without more resources and time. Rank your possible responses, from best to worst. Explain your reasoning.
Situational tests can be used in many fields. They are often used in the medical professions: review books have been made for would-
Sternberg (2011) emphasizes that analytic intelligence is neither the only one nor always the best, and that everyone should deploy the strengths and guard against the limitations of each type. Choosing which intelligence to use takes wisdom, which Sternberg considers a fourth ingredient of successful intelligence:
One needs creativity to generate novel ideas, analytical intelligence to ascertain whether they are good ideas, practical intelligence to implement the ideas and persuade others of their value, and wisdom to ensure that the ideas help reach a common goal.
[Sternberg, 2012, p. 21]
[Lifespan Link: Wisdom is discussed in Chapter 24.]
Which kind of intelligence is most needed and valued depends partly on age and partly on culture. Think about Sternberg’s three types. Analytic intelligence is usually valued in high school and college, as students try to remember and analyze various ideas. However, although people considered “smart” usually have analytic intelligence, that is not enough in adulthood. As Sternberg argues: “many very smart people turn a blind eye to the wars, poverty, government atrocities, starvation, and disease that affect others around them” (Sternberg, 2013, p. 188).
Creative intelligence is prized if life circumstances change and new challenges arise; it is much more valued in some cultures and countries than in others (Kaufman & Sternberg, 2006). In times of social upheaval, or in certain professions (such as the arts), creativity is a better predictor of accomplishment than is IQ. However, creativity can be so innovative and out of touch that creative people are scorned, ignored, or even killed. The contributions of many creative geniuses went unrecognized until years after their death.
Think about these three intelligences cross-
613
cognitive artifacts Intellectual tools passed down from generation to generation which may assist in learning within societies.
Difficult as it is to determine who is truly smart, it is even more difficult to judge which nation is smartest, in part because each culture has its own standards to determine the combination of abilities that comprises intelligence (Sternberg, 2013). A controversial idea from Earl Hunt, a psychologist who studies intelligence, is that the nations with the most advanced economies and greatest national wealth are those that make best use of cognitive artifacts—that is, ways to amplify and extend general cognitive ability (Hunt, 2012).
Especially for Prospective Parents In terms of the intellectual challenge, what type of intelligence is most needed for effective parenthood?
Response for Prospective Parents: Because parenthood demands flexibility and patience, Sternberg’s practical intelligence is probably most needed. Anything that involves finding a single correct answer, such as analytic intelligence or number ability, would not be much help.
Historically, written language, the number system, universities, and the scientific method were cognitive artifacts. Each of these artifacts extended human intellectual abilities by helping people interact to learn more. Sheer survival (in earlier centuries, more newborns died than lived) and longer life (few people survived to age 50) resulted from cognitive artifacts. The germ theory of disease, for instance, was developed because doctors were able to do research and then learn from each other (Hunt, 2011).
In more recent times, universal education and preventive health care, clean water, electricity, global travel, and the Internet have resulted in advanced societies. According to this idea, smart people are better able to use the cognitive artifacts of their society to advance their own intelligence. Then they develop more cognitive artifacts, and the whole community benefits.
For instance, developed nations provide preschool and kindergarten to all children. That increases the IQ of the new generation, which eventually advances the nation. By contrast, some nations value fertility more than health, expose children to lead and other toxins, and censure information. All those practices undercut the full intellectual development of the people (Hunt, 2011). In thinking about adult intellectual growth, does the specific context of any particular man or woman limit or expand their mind?
“Are pacifiers bad for babies?”
“How much should a 4-
“When do you tell children about sex?”
My students ask me such questions often. I answer based on my knowledge of human development and my experience raising my children. But I wonder how much my answers reflect my culture, and not research. Similar doubts can be raised about almost anything adults know. Our perspective may not be adaptive to some cultural contexts. One definition of intelligence is that it is the ability to adapt to current conditions. Might my intelligence be far more limited than my students assume?
Tests of good infant care have been developed, based primarily on analytic, not practical, child rearing (e.g., McCall et al., 2010). One of the most common scales is the Knowledge of Infant Development Inventory (KIDI) (MacPhee, 1981). KIDI measures how much caregivers know about infant senses, motor skills, and communication—
Such knowledge seems helpful. For example, mothers and fathers who score higher on the KIDI are less depressed and more likely to provide responsive baby care (Howard, 2010; Zolotor et al., 2008). Many researchers believe that knowledge of infant development causes (not merely correlates with) good care.
Should we worry when mothers do not know about their babies’ growth? Perhaps. For instance, in one study only 29 percent of immigrant mothers knew that 2-
The opposing perspective suggests that knowledge of infant development does not matter in caregiving. A study supporting this view found that an immigrant child’s later cognitive development was best predicted not by KIDI scores or other measures of parenting, but by parents’ SES and language use. In this longitudinal study, the mothers’ KIDI scores did not predict later school success for Asian American or Latino children; it did so only for European American children (Han et al., 2012). That suggests that culture was crucial to the outcome.
In another study, researchers provided supportive, encouraging visitors to low-
614
A significant impact of this intervention was its effect on the mothers’ ability to create home environments more suitable for the needs of their infants … despite lack of measurable change in mothers’ knowledge of infant development.
[Katz et al., 2011, p. S81]
In other words, advances in practical skills, not analytic ones, made a difference.
Knowledge may not improve parenting. Instead, warmth and patience, responsiveness (without expecting an infant to reciprocate), mental health, or social support networks may be more critical than knowledge. IQ correlates with positive personal attributes, but it does not determine them (Dunkel, 2013).
Part of the underlying reason why tests of knowledge do not always predict good parenting is that cultures vary in what they believe about infant development. Such beliefs may be beneficial to babies in some contexts but not in others. For example, an anthropologist from the United States studied the Ache in Paraguay. The Ache tribal people were respectful and deferential to her on repeated visits, until she and her husband
arrived at their study site in the forest of Paraguay with their infant daughter in tow. The Ache greeted her in a whole new way. They took her aside and in friendly and intimate but no-
[Small, 1998, p. 213]
How important to quality care is accurate knowledge of child development? I am not neutral on this question; I have devoted much of my life to teaching about development. I believe knowledge is power, and I am grateful that I know what I know.
Yet the most important aspect of good parenting may not be information. Similarly, the sign of an intelligent adult may be something other than analytic intelligence. This box raises these issues. Education is a cultural artifact; I think everyone benefits from it. If I were convinced that responsiveness is more important than knowledge, I would teach responsiveness. Is that a contradiction?
Intelligence may be not a single entity (g) but rather a combination of different abilities, sometimes categorized as fluid and crystallized or as analytic, creative, and practical. These abilities rise and fall partly because of events in each person’s life, partly because of culture and cohort, and partly because of age. The overall picture of adult intelligence, as measured by various tests, is complex. In general, verbal intelligence increases and timed tests of intelligence show declines as the decades of adult life go by.
One controversy underlying all the psychometric approaches to intelligence regards the definition of intelligence, with scholars disagreeing about what to measure, how, and why. It is clear that the specific intellectual abilities needed for success depend, at least in part, on context and culture.
615