As explained in Chapter 2, Piaget’s sweeping overview of four periods of cognition contrasts with information-
For infants, output might be moving a hand to uncover a toy (object permanence), saying a word to signify recognition (e.g., mama), or looking at one photo longer than another (habituation). Some recent studies examine changes in brain waves when infants see a picture (Koulder et al., 2013), research that both confirms and refutes Piaget’s theory.
163
Especially for Computer Experts In what way is the human mind not like a computer?
Response for Computer Experts: In dozens of ways, including speed of calculation, ability to network across the world, and vulnerability to viruses. In at least one crucial way, the human mind is better: Computers become obsolete or fail within a few years, while human minds keep advancing for decades.
To understand the many aspects of information processing in infancy, consider the baby’s reaction to feeling hungry. A newborn simply cries with hunger pangs as a reflex, but an older hungry infant hears its mother’s voice, looks for her, reaches to be picked up, and then nuzzles at her breast, or, at an even older age, signs or says something to indicate hunger. Each step of this process requires information to be processed. Older infants are much more thoughtful and effective than newborns because of more advanced information processing. Advances occur week by week or even day by day in the first year, contrary to Piaget’s notion of six discrete stages (Cohen & Cashon, 2006).
The information-
Consider the concept of number. Habituation and brain scans reveal that 6-
The information-
In earlier decades, infant intelligence was measured via age of sitting up, grasping, and so on, but we now know that, unless a child is severely impaired, age of achieving motor skills does not correlate with later intellectual achievement. However, information-
Now let us look at two specific aspects of infant cognition that illustrate the information-
Perception, remember, is the processing of information that arrives at the brain from the sense organs. Decades of thought and research led Eleanor and James Gibson to conclude that perception is far from automatic (E. J. Gibson, 1969; J. J. Gibson, 1979). Perception—
affordance An opportunity for perception and interaction that is offered by a person, place, or object in the environment.
The environment (people, places, and objects) affords, or offers, many opportunities to interact with whatever is perceived (E. J. Gibson, 1997). Each of these opportunities is called an affordance. Which particular affordance is perceived and acted on depends on four factors: sensory awareness, immediate motivation, current level of development, and past experience.
164
As an example, imagine that you are lost in an unfamiliar city. You need to ask directions. Of whom? Not the first person you see. You want someone knowledgeable and approachable. Affordance is what you seek, and you scan facial expression, body language, gender, dress, and more of the passersby (Miles, 2009).
Age of the perceiver affects what affordances are perceived. For example, since toddlers enjoy running as soon as their legs allow it, every open space affords running: a meadow, a building’s long hall, a highway. To adults, affordance of running is much more limited, as they notice a bull grazing in the meadow, neighbors behind the hallway doors, or traffic on the road. Furthermore, because motivation is pivotal in affordances, toddlers start moving when most adults prefer to stay put.
Selective perception of affordances depends not only on age, motivation, and context but also on culture. Just as a baby might be oblivious to something adults consider crucial—
Variation in affordance is also apparent within cultures. City-
visual cliff An experimental apparatus that gives the illusion of a sudden drop-
Experience affects which affordances are perceived. This is obvious in studies of depth perception. Research demonstrating this began with an apparatus called the visual cliff, designed to provide the illusion of a sudden drop-
Especially for Parents of Infants When should you be particularly worried that your baby will fall off the bed or down the stairs?
Response for Parents of Infants: Constant vigilance is necessary for the first few years of a child’s life, but the most dangerous age is from about 4 to 8 months, when infants can move but do not yet fear falling over an edge.
Scientists once thought that a visual deficit—
This awareness of the visual cliff hazard depends on experience. The difference is in processing, not input; in affordance, not sensory ability. Further research on affordances of the visual cliff includes the social context, with the tone of the mother’s encouragement indicating whether or not the cliff affords crawling (Kim et al., 2010).
A similar sequence happens with fear. By 9 months, infants attend to snakes and spiders more readily than to other similar images, but they do not yet fear them. A few months later, perhaps because they have learned from others, they are afraid of such creatures. Thus perception is a prerequisite, but it does not always lead to affordance (LoBue, 2013).
Despite all the variations from one person to another in affordances perceived, all babies are attracted to things that move. They stare at passing cars, flickering images on a screen, mobiles—
165
Other creatures that move, especially an infant’s own caregivers, afford pleasure to every infant. It’s almost impossible to teach a baby not to chase and grab moving creatures, including dogs, cats, or even bugs. Universally, moving objects are more attractive than static ones.
The infant’s interest in motion was the inspiration for another experiment (van Hof et al., 2008). A ball was moved at various speeds in front of infants aged 3 to 9 months. Most tried to touch or catch the ball as it passed within reach. However, they differed in their perception of whether the balls afforded catching.
Sometimes younger infants did not reach for slow-
The term infant amnesia refers to the belief that infants remember nothing until about age 2, an idea once accepted by most adults. Information processing has revealed otherwise; memory is evident in very young babies.
However, memory is fragile in the first months of life. Both a certain amount of experience and a certain amount of brain maturation are required in order to process and remember anything (Bauer et al., 2010).
Infants have difficulty storing new memories in their first year, and older children are often unable to describe events that occurred when they were younger. One reason is linguistic: People use words to store (and sometimes distort) memories, so preverbal children have difficulty with recall (Richardson & Hayne, 2007), while adults cannot access early memories because they did not have words to solidify them.
166
A series of experiments, however, reveals that very young infants can remember, even if they cannot later put memories into words. Memories are particularly evident if:
The most dramatic proof of infant memory comes from innovative experiments in which 3-
When some infants had the mobile-
reminder session A perceptual experience that helps a person recollect an idea, a thing, or an experience.
The lead researcher, Carolyn Rovee-
In this particular reminder session, two weeks after the initial training, the infants watched the mobile move but were not tied to it and were positioned so that they could not kick. The next day, when they were again connected to the mobile and positioned so that they could move their legs, they kicked as they had learned to do two weeks earlier. Apparently, watching the mobile move on the previous day had revived their faded memory. The information about making the mobile move was stored in their brains, but they needed processing time to retrieve it. The reminder session provided that time.
Other research finds that repeated reminders are more powerful than single reminders and that context is crucial, especially for infants younger than 9 months old: Being tested in the same room as the initial experience aids memory (Rovee-
After about 6 months of age, infants retain information for a longer time than younger babies do, with less training or reminding. Toward the end of the first year, many kinds of memory are apparent. For example, suppose a 9-
Many experiments show that toddlers can transfer learning from one object or experience to another and that they learn from various people and events—
167
Note that these experiments are further evidence of several facts already mentioned: Babies observe affordances carefully, they are especially attuned to movement, and deferred imitation is possible before Piaget’s stage six begins.
implicit memory Unconscious or automatic memory that is usually stored via habits, emotional responses, routine procedures, and various sensations.
explicit memory Memory that is easy to retrieve on demand (as in a specific test). Most explicit memory involves consciously learned words, data, and concepts.
One reason earlier scientists underestimated memory is that they failed to distinguish between implicit memory and explicit memory. Implicit memory is memory that remains hidden until a particular stimulus brings it to mind (like the mobile reminder session), whereas explicit memory is memory that can be recalled on demand. Explicit memories are usually verbal, and therefore “although explicit memory emerges sometime between 6 and 12 months, it is far from fully developed” (Nelson, de Haan et al., 2006, p. 23).
The particular part of the brain on which explicit memory depends is the hippocampus, present at birth but very immature until about age 5 or 6. [Lifespan Link: The function of the hippocampus is explained in Chapter 8.] It is no surprise that this timing coincides with the beginning of formal education, because children are much better at memorizing at that age.
Implicit memories, by contrast, begin before birth. Implicit memories are evident in all the examples just mentioned, when evidence of memory comes from the situation, not from the answer to a spoken question.
Especially for Teachers People of every age remember best when they are active learners. If you had to teach fractions to a class of 8-
Response for Teachers: Remember the three principles of infant memory: real life, motivation, and repetition. Find something children already enjoy that involves fractions—
For instance, adults who knew a language in childhood often have no explicit memory of it: They claim to have forgotten all the Spanish, French, Chinese, or whatever. When asked the word for a common object in that language, they honestly reply that they do not know (Bowers et al., 2009). Moreover, when first tested, such adults are no better at comprehension than those who never heard the language.
However, repeated exposure uncovers implicit memories from infancy. Thus, a student who has forgotten childhood Spanish catches on more quickly in Spanish class than does the student who never knew Spanish as an infant. Apparently, the first weeks of class serve as a reminder session. Reminders may also explain the phenomenon of déjà vu; people, places, and smells sometimes seem familiar or emotionally evocative, even if they were never experienced before, because something very similar occurred in infancy and was stored implicitly.
Infants probably store in their brains many emotions and sensations that they cannot readily retrieve. The information-
The crucial insight from information processing is that the brain is a very active organ, even in early infancy. Therefore, the particulars of experiences and memory are critically important in determining what a child knows or does not know. Soon generalization is possible. In one study, after 6-
Many studies show that infants remember not only specific events and objects but also patterns and general goals (Keil, 2011). Some examples come from research, such as memory of what syllables and rhythms are heard and how objects move in relation to other objects. Additional examples arise from close observations of babies at home, such as what they expect from Mommy as compared to Daddy or what details indicate bedtime. Every day of their young lives, infants are processing information and storing conclusions.
168
Information processing analyzes each component of how thoughts begin; how they are organized, remembered, and expressed; and how cognition builds, day by day. Infants’ perception is powerfully influenced by particular experiences and motivation; affordances perceived by one infant differ from those perceived by another. Memory depends on brain maturation and on experience. For that reason, memory is fragile in the first year (though it can be triggered by reminders) and becomes more evident, although still fragile, in the second year.