Chapter 7 What Have You Learned?

  1. Question 7.1

    YHunuXJoLfrnYSmIXwLJtjtizSq8KNyVb+sYdKViBa9jxYijAsO4P1Luybr0R25UU3k+9TyV/kQ=
    Crying and contentment are present from birth. The social smile appears around 6 weeks of age. Infants express social joy and laughter between 2 and 4 months.
  2. Question 7.2

    spo20/HMASN6qJdolJE0uzuDEeQmpcTziac73Iisj+bTmE9FrAgdpJzN8Lsp8IKWTW8uTof9FvgYsLyQ
    Anger is evident at 6 months, usually triggered by frustration. Sadness usually indicates withdrawal and is accompanied by an increase in the body’s production of cortisol, the primary stress hormone.
  3. Question 7.3

    siB7xjxNgaaSheFZ2u3LX0TsUAHNFpKxD5LqjeWEyXanCxdOK/6BTgvTB434tIwmpZoGAzv+nTIf2MqAJZvC5KTWOQ63/seeW87M6uKLXU5DmpAbKt710qNNOGlrUt/QCzASHPmgKjwLGWP/zXSCqUZ6O9Z2JZd/0GV9JK8c6+rfcn/K
    Typical 1-year-old children fear strangers and separation from their caregivers. Many also fear anything unexpected, from the flush of the toilet to the pop of a jack-in-the-box.
  4. Question 7.4

    L5Ytd6pwxvXZCFHHD0rKl1HJ2nyxvAVcrj3TTyegpC64QcTClRt2N/1y2DOVVVfOZsKoysyDw5Y8TPORmL/wPlL+OeYsP5SG
    Emotions take on new strength during toddlerhood. For example, anger and fear become less frequent but more focused, targeted toward especially infuriating or terrifying experiences. Similarly, laughing and crying are louder and more discriminating. Social awareness develops, ushering in the new emotions of pride, shame, embarrassment, disgust, and guilt.
  5. Question 7.5

    n7IdwdY1cKmWucJNDEyyeI16XEMRVZ5yY7xtxg5y5o1qrdrrMhJX9JYMIZV8pvdRgu0YI6R/3qyU2Ohmc8Y3mIyO4lAU0mhB
    The expression of pride, shame, embarrassment, disgust, and guilt require social awareness and self- awareness. Social awareness and self-awareness emerge from family interactions and are shaped by the culture. For example, North American parents encourage toddlers to feel proud of their accomplishments, but Asian families typically discourage pride and instead cultivate modesty and shame.
  6. Question 7.6

    rDbzF8HU8ZyxeE3NvI4l8/xtUnr1r3cILqqeuqzfHo7FoP63rtrdgPeA/fwXoeD5hr9CZX18iNqq6e/R0ra4rfc9WTBy2eptweJBH4pW4sc=
    The social smile, laughter, fear, self-awareness, and anger appear as the cortex matures. The maturation of the anterior cingulate gyrus (a part of the cortex) is directly connected to emotional self-regulation, allowing a child to hide or express his or her feelings. What is unknown is how infant brains are molded by their environment and culture and how this affects their expression of emotions.
  7. Question 7.7

    VxJ+P0BdUFH7GCw+ClJYr0c8CND97gOMMyqRfLOMgCbjSQqemCAt9aeeQDM=
    All emotional reactions, particularly those connected to self-awareness, depend partly on memory. Memory is fragile at first and gradually improves over the first year. This explains why toddlers are more quickly angered than younger babies when teased by an older sibling. Unlike young infants, toddlers have vivid memories of the previous time a sibling frustrated them.
  8. Question 7.8

    /lTOJ040cc7GCLqzT0g+EEpfSTEfoN+EuIkLr6FCTb8vZE1pZ0DkCUi9Cn32pZeZaKLRog==
    Excessive stress harms the developing brain. The hypothalamus, in particular, grows more slowly if an infant is often frightened.
  9. Question 7.9

    LeLtlU37bzlBsTydE8B8l52DvO7UJQpxD+xNzZjjxGSG8XQdJZUm3OxSasjfbfIvDwhDeA6r9pihdVXbjv66mvlN0UJWbFeipJBiY1mI0ns=
    Although temperament originates with genes, the expression of emotions over the life span is modified by experience—the result of child-rearing methods, culture, and learning. This means that in some people temperament is more apparent.
  10. Question 7.10

    zqq2fZIBVVkx6MlKO+qV2GkUV205kdRtxGoRZzWzT9Nc7o5v3ntcNfrqPsAJnCeurymp8X8tok4C1nFi
    Parental responsiveness during synchrony aids psychosocial and biological development by providing powerful learning experiences. In every episode, infants learn to read another’s facial expression, body language, and emotions, thereby laying the foundation for the development of social skills.
  11. Question 7.11

    e7qNeAGJ4jyZ7/bAZFMG3Zl19+1o+Ej4Tq6+NJbF+tew89bJtbz3oVij7TetFIN0OikcA7FGh3zBpVHh7RqesUawcki5eQGkMKBET7tzES7Ugg6d6cG5/v1yVHiSi23EvZ0J77qfnf72m5zzD+aCBNR5xQRD9CqADLRSabjVH5NjchVypmbjGDQpnJcwAlZRJm0nWdtjP6mYNYkXhB454E0F0f6y75CFYelpa+W+/OpKyJbM+H1P5kEeZ0s5TxOnFtvbuvkdD5qonBhTjQbKJg==
    Examples of proximity-seeking behavior include a baby crying when the mother leaves the room (if only for a moment) or if a back-facing car seat prevents the baby from seeing the parent. Examples of contact-maintaining behaviors include caregivers absentmindedly smoothing their toddler’s hair or caressing their child’s hands.
  12. Question 7.12

    ZTNFd0mEQoOq7KZwdMaJKCSIUKcZalAKBNgdsL8h0eMkL0v+51uZMeGA3Im/MmsCuB0tQtSV/HEDlAyTmZI7TPisi2YhYteDtc3aIw==
    A) Secure attachment: The infant feels comfortable and confident. The caregiver is a base for exploration, providing assurance and enabling exploration.B) Insecure-avoidant attachment: The infant feels fear, anxiety, anger, or indifference. Infants play independently without maintaining contact with the caregiver.C) Insecure-resistant/ambivalent attachment: The infant feels fear, anxiety, anger, or indifference. Infants may be unwilling to leave the caregiver’s lap or are angry at being left.D) Disorganized attachment: The infant may shift suddenly from hitting to kissing the caregiver, from staring blankly to crying hysterically, from pinching himself or herself to freezing in place.Securely attached infants are more likely to become secure toddlers, socially competent preschoolers, high-achieving schoolchildren, and capable adult partners and parents. Insecure attachment correlates with many problems in later relationships.
  13. Question 7.13

    AjCprqeJbeP37thysIJU0MDKu2hP6zhaWe3XzjKQtvlvTK/S1PtldXgz2YcZCP2pmVE/R+s/FbzMaRwzVSGHhkPxr+nKmuXtCAwXp/46sk96VxBMsVNbQEycS3zlAFMNntIZM9zxujF+iZBudW4qtoWQB2x1/hLlL2gUKFEfRkrWDm7zb+9ighUmMnjNyxU+OBjoZF3Fi75/0l/vQEyO8tsdHOsoOAJBs8Z6DQ==
    High-risk parents (particularly those who were abused as children, are socially isolated, or are very young) can benefit from early support offered by skilled professionals. This may include counseling, parenting classes, and one-on-one education about childcare.
  14. Question 7.14

    6NTj8hqWzYtvFSSqEBRINf5HTFS4KUVzctl0QB0WnIl4ZDYKYw5bOVYKNZNCa4mtSQj19w==
    After age 1, when infants can walk and can act like “little scientists,” their need to consult others becomes urgent for safety reasons. Toddlers use social referencing for clues in the faces and body position of trusted others, paying close attention to emotions and intentions in order to learn how to interpret the world. If the trusted other looks calm, then the child knows the situation is not dangerous.
  15. Question 7.15

    Q1wSr8cirNZDe88pyxD0SNSkI24cpxN2vjLv0OA1J1j46m2uoccKYtIvmpQyH2bB
    Fathers enhance their children’s social and emotional development in many ways, and synchrony, attachment, and social referencing are all apparent with fathers. Close father–infant relationships can teach infants appropriate expressions of emotion, particularly anger. Fathers also elicit more smiles and laughter from infants than mothers do, engaging more often in exciting and active play.
  16. Question 7.16

    fA/TXpfVe4rACyfi2Z0f8kO3l60OJWImLBxaZzqZVKVaDCKsrPhP/3jb4dLxqAFkp6K8JioS48BuQErLsq4Mxyy163S5/N+mlcL+YkvrMg4vTUe+
    According to Freud, a person who is stuck in the oral stage may eat or drink to excess, chew tobacco, bite their nails, or talk excessively. A person who is stuck in the anal stage would develop an anal personality, seeking self-control and regularity in all aspects of life.
  17. Question 7.17

    Am4dqsnWgLxPK8t7VSpGf+TOvBVswqo1oeg4EHnKz3JvMEnrFLV6iojqC5BMfnmauOLjyGloyQLfgcM2iBGjOCOIwNCdMbjb5JWMTg==
    If positive social interactions in infancy inspire trust, later in life the child or adult confidently explores the social world. The inverse would be true for infants who develop mistrust at this stage; they would lack confidence in their engagement with the world and assume that their basic needs cannot be met.
  18. Question 7.18

    Sscs8xOJxBeTsKTg4GTqQWik8/IXIMQqKErr9epW+HFlmP7SNOzTDPZFhfA67gyZbYgleN8+V7sR2yoJFBUL/PFzpuV3ThTXkg0SpMXqBLkwnQOk
    Toddlers want autonomy, or self-rule, over their own actions and bodies. If this does not develop, then they feel ashamed and doubtful, and this may result in an adult who is suspicious and pessimistic or easily shamed. Children who develop autonomy will likely develop into confident adults, secure in their own efficacy.
  19. Question 7.19

    cUCQhlJIgzX1chIitOIkRUVurofmebpfCOGWX+ZWr/IbGaL40ran8/+6zNuWn/Xr70s/LgCFJzIVfjFTIe+IE5rkF8DQTCSucFvcvg==
    Behaviorists believe a child’s emotions and personality are molded as parents reinforce or punish a child. In addition, behaviorists believe that infants absorb information from the people around them through social learning.
  20. Question 7.20

    8mPYq+ccTcG3kowwex4xva4HEpy6KiMpYsp/ARgQoj20kEo/GHDrY0M+VCa8eve/vAZbbfD5wQ2e43Dl+LqRkvkvbI0uYkOSFK7siA0rkN4=
    A working model is formed from early experiences, and it is a set of assumptions that become a frame of reference for later life based on beliefs, perceptions, and memories; cognitive theorists believe that these very things—beliefs, perceptions, memories—determine a person’s perspective in life. In contrast, psychoanalytic theory suggests that early events are buried in the unconscious, and behaviorists propose that they simply provide reinforcements or models for behavior.
  21. Question 7.21

    9qHsz6bslOIIRZHlZ2I03esA29Xjn//ZVRd+JGQTsnj9AuhGUNhWnu+6DiGlo7/3s4J19eCWlh1AfVhtX+/7Fvfz5hqig1DE9FXvOyrggO9arYZImOOjTOaJrgREkPU/
    Caregivers clearly have their own needs, and those needs influence how they respond to infants. Self-actualized people no longer have their own unmet needs, so they can nurture an infant well. But most parents are not at the highest level on Maslow’s hierarchy and still seek love or respect. In addition, the early experiences of caregivers often include unmet needs, and these can interfere with their ability to nurture.
  22. Question 7.22

    Kz8n3s04lR5x8VGYzA0DfLYXQBklg3EG8Rr1ThOag+iXARDsBugtb2fQMQ2XfyNVQe2BJOeUnVWlhpuCYCcmYnwKytWarWerSmgSOi13YXZL5w2ipZryrnSD/Dn3T7awYna6fDw070g=
    Evolutionary theory stresses two needs: survival and reproduction. Newborns are extraordinarily dependent, unable to walk or talk or even sit up and feed themselves for months after birth. Thus, they must attract adult devotion—and they do. The parent–child bond ensures the survival of infants.
  23. Question 7.23

    kMYSgr9P9MuSbobqJ06ZENvKNRmweNRs4kRT8MdZt2/Qb/2oJO7j1mwKVq32XFTGw6/tWHYEc7+p1yZUJz8hlg==
    Evolutionary social scientists note that if mothers were the exclusive caregivers of each child until children were adults, a given woman could rear only one or two offspring—not enough for the species to survive. Allocare, the care of children by people other than the biological mother, is necessary so that mothers can have more children than they can care for on their own.
  24. Question 7.24

    ZllDAvMSh3imvXU9DORw+xE7t5MDUDIHgntEGfqsKUaELnB+UBpbUC2agZh8WLbqtXLO40l1HK3e3JykXSlMwcsNxnmIYAzCuyUtGw==
    Advantages of nonmaternal care include that it allows mothers to return to work, and provides (in good environments) safe spaces, appropriate learning and playing equipment, and trained providers to interact with children. The advantages appear most obvious around preschool age; debate remains about its value for younger children. Disadvantages vary greatly by type of care, and the range of quality in care is certainly an issue. There is some correlation between later aggression and early nonmaternal care. Family income level, culture, religion, education levels, and even the sex and temperament of the child all play a role in determining what type of care is best for a child.
  25. Question 7.25

    wjFciW8MXCi8CLdh9eM5+73lga+D3HkLYVZKqN/VJjm8+yH5FBc3LIRwJ58POQH9/EnFx603Kq4QK/HqqNeFcJKe5cU5UnImctEM4KWqh/qIMqj/7NtOqw==
    Care by relatives may help the infant and the budget, but when it is the parents who are splitting the care, the arrangement may interfere with their ability to spend time together. Center day care is more expensive and varies greatly in quality; high-quality programs feature licensed professionals specially educated to care for infants and children, but often at a substantial financial cost. Grandmother care has been beneficial in most times and societies. Overall, consistent caregivers seem to be the most important factor, whether the caregiver is a family member or a professional.
  26. Question 7.26

    DVpFvpZv94yvowrYef9UbrjBnUl1avGuvnyzNj58iHAh4KeVuBI+tpmrnb+b8K8xG8LsZP2B69SVOITVNZDcx8ps2d0=
    Variations in day care arrangements are vast, and not all—particularly those that rely on family assistance—have records that make study and evaluation possible. The quality of infant day care varies a great deal, and some babies seem far more affected than others by the quality of the care they receive. In addition, there continues to be disagreement about the wisdom of nonmaternal child care for the very young.
  27. Question 7.27

    h1cwg3XavB+0KT9uebXPw3Wcc78fIj3ZqJm9tBTd2u7ElSGasTcsO831B+YF7nhTt8a3pfToBJz6eyfuaYB+jBTHLjVD5AAAi0yDyboaz6vHRuMH
    Research from the United States indicates that children generally benefit if their mothers are employed. The most likely reasons are that maternal income reduces parental depression and increases family wealth, both of which correlate with happier and more successful children. In addition, studies indicate that preschool education benefits children. That said, debate continues about the value of nonmaternal care for the very young, with some studies indicating that young children in day care may develop aggression issues and that nonmaternal care may pose particular problems for boys.