10.3 Mood

How are you feeling right now? Decide which of the following words describes your current feelings. Are you feeling:

Relaxed?

Calm?

Gloomy?

Jittery?

Sleepy?

Depressed?

Excited?

Peppy?

Tense?

Contented?

Energetic?

Sluggish?

These words describe different aspects of mood (Russell, 2003; Thayer, 1996). Mood is your feeling state; the term refers to the feelings—of sluggishness, peppiness, contentment, depression, and so on—that are an ever-present part of your conscious experience. To be “in” a certain mood is to experience a consistent type of feeling for a prolonged period.

Sometimes you barely notice your mood; it’s in the “background” of your mental life, unnoticed until someone asks, “How are you feeling?” Sometimes, though, your mood dominates your consciousness. If you’re in a depressed mood, it can be hard to stop ruminating about how bad you’re feeling.

The Structure of Mood

Preview Question

Question

What does it mean to say that we can describe any mood with two simple structures: valence and arousal?

Look back for a moment at the list of mood-related words above. If you think about them, you’ll notice two things:

Low and high arousal One major dimension of variation in mood is arousal, as illustrated by the low (left) and high (right) states of arousal of former U.S. Secretary of State Hillary Clinton.

MOOD DIMENSIONS. The similarities and opposites have an important implication. They suggest that the 12 mood terms in the list above may not refer to a dozen distinct, unrelated psychological states. Rather, there may be some underlying dimensions of mood. Dimensions of mood (or mood dimensions) are universal variations in feeling states, that is, variations that can describe the mood of any and all people.

An analogy will make the idea of mood dimensions clear. Instead of psychological mood, consider six terms we can use to describe the physical body: “skinny,” “chubby,” “thin,” “plump,” “emaciated,” and “rotund.” Although these six terms are separate words, they clearly do not refer to six separate, distinct physical qualities. Rather, they refer to variations in one underlying dimension: weight.

Similarly, although a large number of words can refer to mood, there may be a small number of basic mood dimensions. Mood terms that are opposites—relaxed/tense; peppy/sluggish—exist at different ends of the mood dimensions, just as “skinny” and “chubby” are at different ends of a bodily dimension of weight.

In the psychology of mood, the complete set of dimensions required to describe variations in mood experiences is called the structure of mood.

IDENTIFYING THE STRUCTURE OF MOOD. To identify the structure of mood, researchers rely on statistical tools. They statistically analyze survey responses in which people describe their moods on simple ratings scales. Research findings converge on a simple conclusion: There are two dimensions of mood (Russell, 2003; Watson & Tellegen, 1985). Although not all psychologists agree on the best labels for these dimensions, a popular approach identifies the dimensions arousal and valence.

Psychologists combine these two dimensions to obtain a structure that can describe the spectrum of moods people experience (Figure 10.11). The structure functions like a map. No matter where you are on Earth, your location can be identified on a map defined by lines of longitude and latitude. No matter what your mood at any given time, it can be located somewhere in the “mood map,” defined by lines of valence and arousal (Russell, 2003).

figure 10.11 The structure of mood Everyday moods differ on two main dimensions: (1) arousal and (2) valence (the positivity or negativity of the mood). The variety of terms that we use to describe mood (“upset,” “tense,” “excited,” etc.) can be located within this two-dimensional space.

432

Where are you currently located on the “mood map”?

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 9

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
Sluggish: negative, low arousal; peppy: positive, high arousal; depressed: negative, low arousal; energetic: positive, high arousal

Improving Your Mood

Preview Question

Question

What activities have been demonstrated to improve mood?

Just as a map describes your location but doesn’t tell you how to get somewhere new, the two-dimensional structure of mood describes your mood but doesn’t tell you how to change it. But change often is what you want. You’re tired of feeling grouchy and sluggish. How can you move toward contented and peppy?

THINK ABOUT IT

The two-dimensional model of mood says that, at any one time, you are located in one place in the mood map. But what about “mixed” feelings, of the sort you might experience if you won a competition but, simultaneously, your best friend lost? When experiencing mixed feelings, are you in two locations in the mood map at once?

Many factors affect your mood. Some are life stresses—relationship problems, professional setbacks, death of a loved one—that make you tense and can affect your health, as we discuss later in this chapter. Others are unexpected events that brighten your day: An old friend calls to say hello; people compliment you on your appearance; you win a contest. But you can’t count on these unexpected events to occur, and their effects on mood may be short lived. Fortunately, you’ve got more options; some types of activities have been shown to reliably improve mood states.

433

PHYSICAL ACTIVITIES. One such activity is exercise (Thayer, 1996). Regular exercise can improve mood. Evidence of this comes from experimental studies that randomly assign people to experimental conditions in which they do or do not exercise regularly (Blumenthal et al., 1999). In a study of adults suffering from chronically depressed mood, one group of participants took 45-minute exercise classes 3 times a week for 4 months. In a second experimental condition, participants did not regularly exercise but took antidepressant medications. Participants in the exercise group experienced more positive mood—that is, they were much less depressed—after the four months of exercise than before. Furthermore, exercise improved depressed mood as effectively as did the antidepressant drugs. When researchers checked in with participants six months later, most of the participants were still exercising and remained in a more positive mood (Babyak et al., 2000).

The change in mood that resulted from exercise primarily was a shift along one of the two dimensions of the two-dimensional model of mood (see Figure 10.11): the positivity–negativity dimension. What about the other dimension, arousal? Some activities reliably lower high-arousal states of tension and stress. One is yoga.

In yoga, practitioners learn physical poses that build bodily strength and flexibility, while simultaneously focusing one’s attention in a calm and concentrated manner. The skills of yoga may improve mood. To find out, researchers (Streeter et al., 2010) assigned participants to one of two groups. In one, participants engaged in 60-minute yoga sessions 3 times a week for 12 weeks. In the other, participants walked for 60 minutes 3 times a week for 12 weeks. Both groups thus got a similar amount of exercise. But yoga had a greater effect on mood. Yoga participants reported higher levels of positive mood and lower levels of anxious arousal; that is, they improved on both mood dimensions. This study also involved brain-level analyses. Through brain-imaging methods, the researchers found evidence that yoga may increase levels of a brain chemical that contributes to calm, low-arousal states of mind (Streeter et al., 2010). Other studies confirm that training in yoga can substantially reduce levels of stress (Michalsen et al., 2005).

Does your current exercise program benefit your mood?

It’s as relaxing as it looks Research indicates that yoga reduces anxious moods.

Other physical interventions also have the power to reduce anxiety. In massage therapy, pressure is applied to the body in order to manipulate, and relax, muscles and other soft tissues of the body. A statistical analysis of more than three dozen studies shows that massage therapy influences mood (Moyer, Rounds, & Hannum, 2004). Even single sessions of massage therapy reduce feelings of anxious tension.

The effects of exercise, yoga, and massage therapy teach a general lesson about mood. Your mood reflects the overall state of your body (Thayer, 2003). Changes in the condition of the body, then, directly influence mood.

MUSIC. Another activity that can affect your mood is music. Intuitively, you know that music can alter your feelings. An upbeat song peps you up. Hearing your favorite band can brighten a dull or depressed mood. Research backs up your intuitions.

Laboratory experiments reveal that music and mood are systematically linked (Krumhansl, 2002). In this research, participants heard passages of symphonic music that varied in tempo, rhythm, and tone. They reported experiencing different emotions during the different musical passages. Furthermore, physiological recordings (heart rate, blood pressure) showed that their internal bodily states varied across musical passages, just as you would expect if the sound of music was altering their mood (Krumhansl, 1997).

434

Brain imaging confirms the effect of music on inner states of the body. Pleasant and inspiring music activates brain regions associated with the experience of pleasurable, rewarding stimuli (Blood & Zatorre, 2001; Koelsch et al., 2006).

Finally, singing may have unique effects on mood as well. In a survey of college choir members, the large majority reported that singing improves their mood (Clift & Hancox, 2001). In research comparing the effects of singing with merely listening to choral music, singing produced greater increases in positive mood, in addition to increased levels of a protein that is part of the body’s immune system (Kreutz et al., 2004).

The effects of music on mood are puzzling, though. Why should a series of tones, patterned across time, affect people’s feelings? Psychological science cannot answer this question definitively. However, theorists suggest an interesting possibility (Scherer, 2004). Music may automatically activate body rhythms and movements that, in turn, influence your mood. When you hear some upbeat music, the rhythm and melody tend to induce body movement (such as bobbing your head). These movements are associated with positive mood; your body tends to move more quickly and vigorously when you’re happy than when you’re depressed. The body movement induced by music, then, may create a more positive mood (Scherer, 2004)—just as you might expect from research reviewed earlier on mood and physical activities.

Do you find it hard to resist moving your body when you hear music? If so, what is the effect on your mood?

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 10

Which of the following statements are true about factors that can change mood?

  • Ht3PDiGzt/UDciE23/CPZtZnvwpgOx7qZoaOScBypxc/W6YXV8QWpKcYoN8aUFhGHYAtUUSNdtP05J01GEO2BHv0qAM5gIRBUdcobY78GHLLEp46fXTzxMoZGm4af0ah9H3pFDqwCJ2kXdEXLP0swi1DiiJiqJjZkTTZ3zwoGQkJkLn8NtgY3vic8VFFtsefvVQH57DeVbOR/2Id6deZw2zxV8Tq0LTPxc468bQXwAQXWRKu
  • NnRD/N38hfWC8iX9lI5Hjuhys4j7CtZjTtWSPURtkqeHUaOTbj1ylAs5qAD3qKwsmBhzarofH9pRyOqkra0i1PRfiNib+9L7mDdYpzKm9H1fPaWDHaVtuP+X1zxPNAqQ5tq9OjVyRUZuExC0gy+3QjVxwO1mH+QG
  • sfvFU8SyBKSEGseRWcIrpmeB8A69NaMnlMdWekewWoNxYlUyfSwZ6pi/rotS3iMazSeYPFn++QtLMy4QGUl/B6vYS4bm5XhJrVXG9/O3pM22m17iaTV9JRJwuEfLzz1AXtkR45Q6eJU=
  • 2tZXtz/qeJzBgO9n6vsxFmuB6kW7WSGFw8fG0GUAE5sd4fFTGlXt9oCjcq9vzQ4CmBBG/uAVXQm9TOshhDMqCLk7hfvCb/1E+wpYoZT9/hUWrMtaUV6bja3rMhJkrqplfPaQUll62jhjX+7ZJOVwouWOyKDu8H3oL+F2WbY/Hhg+hHZiCgGkVkjHhyemiv9nG4SK+g==

Mood, Thought, and Behavior

Preview Questions

Question

Can today’s weather influence how satisfied we are with life in general?

Are people more likely to help others when in a positive mood or negative mood?

Mood is inherently interesting. Variations in mood state intrigue psychologists and nonpsychologists alike. Yet, to the psychological scientist, mood is intriguing for a second reason, namely, because it systematically affects thinking and behavior. Let’s first look at the influence of mood on thought.

MOOD AS INFORMATION. Consider the following questions:

  1. How much do you like the college in which you are enrolled?

  2. How good is your car?

  3. How satisfied are you with your life in general?

435

We posed those questions in order to ask you a different one: How did you go about answering them?

In theory, you might have answered each question by contemplating a long list of facts and then systematically adding the facts together. To answer the life satisfaction question, you could have written down a long list of every person, place, and thing associated with your life; weighed the good with the bad; averaged all the pieces of information together; and computed a level of life satisfaction. But, in reality, that’s probably not what you did.

When evaluating a target of judgment—a school, a car, a life—people rarely enumerate and compile long lists of facts. Instead, they rely on something else: their mood, specifically, the mood they are in when making the evaluation.

The mood-as-information hypothesis (Schwarz & Clore, 2007) proposes that when people evaluate a target of judgment, their moods directly inform their evaluations. In the psychological process of evaluation, mood acts as if it is a source of information. The idea behind the theory is that, when confronted with a question such as “How satisfied are you with your life?” people answer by consulting their feelings. If a person thinks about her life, is feeling good when thinking about it, and then formulates an evaluation of her life in general, she’ll tend to say, “I’m very satisfied.” Her feelings inform her evaluation.

The mood-as-information hypothesis thus has a fascinating implication: Irrelevant factors may affect people’s evaluations. People’s evaluations of a target—their college, their car, their life—may be influenced by extraneous factors that happen to affect their mood, but that otherwise have nothing to do with the object being evaluated. One such factor is the weather.

On a day like this, she may not like her city. Or her clothes. Or her life in general. Foul weather can put you in a bad mood that affects your judgment through mood-as-information processes.

A study by the social psychologists Norbert Schwarz and Gerald Clore (1983) vividly illustrates mood-as-information processes. In a telephone interview, they asked people to rate their level of satisfaction with their life as a whole. Schwarz and Clore conducted interviews on both damp, rainy days and warm, sunny days. You might think that weather would have no effect on interview responses because people were rating their overall life, not the day’s weather. But that’s not what Schwarz and Clore thought; they predicted that weather would influence mood, and mood, in turn, would affect evaluations of life. As the researchers predicted, those interviewed on a sunny day evaluated their life more positively than those interviewed on a rainy day (Figure 10.12).

figure 10.12 Life satisfaction and weather If someone asked how satisfied you are with your life as a whole, your answer might be influenced by on two factors: (1) the weather, and (2) whether the person first asked you about the weather. Weather can affect your mood and your judgments, if you don’t stop to think about this effect.

Interestingly, if the researchers first asked participants, “How’s the weather?” their life evaluations on sunny and rainy days did not differ (Schwarz & Clore, 1983). The “how’s the weather” question reminded people that something irrelevant, the weather that day, might bias their judgments. Once reminded, participants no longer used their mood as a source of information. There’s a practical lesson here! When making an important life decision, stop and think—or your conclusions could be biased by trivial factors that affect your mood.

MOOD AND HELPING. Mood can also influence behavior. How you feel affects what you do. One type of social behavior that is strongly influenced by mood is helping others.

People often have opportunities to help. Salvation Army workers ask for donations. Community service organizations seek volunteers. Indigent and homeless citizens ask for assistance. Do you help them? Research suggests that you are much more likely to do so if you are in a good mood.

436

Keep smiling! Research shows that putting people in a good mood increases the chances that they will help you out.

Even simple, subtle influences on mood can affect willingness to help. Isen and Levin (1972) conducted a study in which they left a dime in the coin return slot of telephones in a phone booth. (This was well before the advent of cell phones, at a time when the value of a dime was equivalent to about 50 cents in today’s money.) After participants found the dime and left the booth, a researcher nearby would drop a folder full of papers on the ground. The dependent measure of the study was whether participants helped pick up the papers. Finding a dime—an event that temporarily induced a positive mood—increased helping behavior. Most people who found a dime helped pick up the papers. Most people in a second experimental condition, in which there was no dime, did not help.

Can any of your acts of generosity have been influenced by something as small as a dime?

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 11

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
If the company wants honest feedback, it would be better off giving the gift after people fill out the questionnaire. That way, the positive mood elicited by the gift cannot influence their judgments.