13.5 Trait Theory

Preview Question

Question

What characterizes the measurement of personality in trait theory?

In personality psychology, a trait is a person’s typical style of behavior and emotion. (“Trait” contrasts with the concept of psychological state— a person’s current thoughts and feelings, which may vary from moment to moment.) Trait theories are theoretical approaches that try to identify, describe, and measure people’s personality traits. A key word in that last sentence was “measure” (discussed in Chapter 2); reliable measurement is the foundation on which trait theory is built (Cattell, 1965).

Trait theorists recognize that good measurement benefits not only basic science, but also practical applications. Suppose you are hiring people to fill jobs at a company and want a sociable salesperson, an honest accountant, and a manager with a calm leadership style. There will be many applicants. Which would be best for each job? To find out, you need good measurement tools: efficient and reliable measures of the traits of interest. This is exactly what trait theory provides.

By way of background, trait theory has a long history. Early in the twentieth century, psychologists recognized that measurement principles used in the study of intelligence (see Chapter 8) could be applied to personality. By asking people a large number of questions and scoring their responses, personality could be measured. In the latter half of the twentieth century, research on genes and individual differences indicated that variations in personality trait scores were, to a significant degree, based on inherited biology (see Chapter 4). By the century’s close, the combination of quantitative measures and genetic findings made trait psychology a major force in the science of personality.

Structure: Stable Individual Differences

Preview Questions

Question

What scientific method do trait theorists use to reduce the thousands of traits in our language into the Big Five (or Six)?

What does Eysenck’s research suggest is the biological basis of the trait extraversion?

The major structure in personality trait theory is—no surprise here—the personality trait. As noted, a trait is a person’s typical style of behavior and emotion. More specifically, personality traits have three defining qualities that refer to consistent ways in which people differ in their average tendencies:

  1. Consistency: According to trait theory, personality traits express themselves consistently, across time and situations (Allport, 1937; Epstein, 1979). A person who is high in the trait conscientiousness, for example, would be expected to display conscientious behavior in a consistent manner (e.g., the person might be a reliable employee, a hardworking student, and a law-abiding citizen).

  2. Individual differences: Personality traits refer to differences among people. Most trait psychologists try to identify a small number of traits that can be used to describe individual differences in any population of people.

  3. On average: Traits refer to what people typically are like; in other words, what they are like on average. Suppose you are very friendly and pleasant with everyone except one particular person, whom you dislike. A trait measure of friendliness would average your behavior across all these relationships. Because you are friendly with most people, you would get a high score on friendliness. The score reflects your typical behavior.

572

In addition to these three defining qualities, the word “trait” has two extra meanings to most trait theorists. One is that it refers to a dimension, not a category. This means that there are gradual variations in levels of the trait. In this way, personality traits are like physical traits such as height. Although we sometimes talk about “tall people” and “short people,” we know that these are not distinct categories of people; height varies along a dimension. Similarly, although we may talk about “agreeable people” and “disagreement,” the trait of agreeableness varies dimensionally. For any given trait dimension, most people are near the middle of the dimension, and small numbers of people are at the very high or low ends (Figure 13.4).

figure 13.4 Trait dimensions Trait theorists view personality traits as dimensions. For a trait dimension such as introversion–extraversion, most people are in the middle of the dimension; relatively few have extremely low or high levels of the trait.

Second, theorists see traits as causes of behavior (McCrae & Costa, 1995, 1996). Again, an analogy with height is useful. If someone buys shirts with extra-long sleeves and pants with extra-long legs, and he sometimes bumps his head on the top of doorways, the cause is his high standing on the physical dimension of height. Analogously, if someone helps people with errands, avoids getting into arguments, and cooperates cheerfully with team members on work projects, his behavior, according to trait theory, would be caused by his high standing on the personality dimension of agreeableness—an overall tendency to be pleasant, helpful, and accommodating (Graziano, Jensen-Campbell, & Finch, 1997; but see This Just In).

If an extraterrestrial were to come to Earth and ask, “What are the most important ways in which individuals differ from one another?” what would your answer be?

With the meaning of “trait” established, the next question to ask is what are the most important personality traits? There are two approaches to answering this question; we’ll look at both in the sections ahead. The first is to examine words in everyday language (Ashton & Lee, 2007; Goldberg, 1981). The lexical approach to trait theory presumes that people will notice, and invent words for, all significant differences among people, and thus that words in the language contain clues about the most significant personality traits. The other approach is to examine biology (Stelmack & Rammsayer, 2008). Neural or biochemical differences between people may produce variations in personality.

PERSONALITY TRAITS IN THE LANGUAGE. Imagine opening a dictionary and listing all the words that describe personality traits. You start with “abrasive,” move to “absent-minded” and “abstemious,” and—if you can keep yourself awake long enough—eventually arrive, appropriately enough, at “zonked.”

Incredibly, two personality psychologists once did just that: They listed every trait word in English—more than 4000 of them (Allport & Odbert, 1936).

The length of that list poses a problem: Measuring 4000 traits is impractical. It’s too much work to develop 4000 tests, and even if you did develop them, who would ever take them all? Trait theorists need to identify a small set of traits that correspond to the most significant individual differences in personality. They do so by using factor analysis.

Factor analysis is a statistical technique that identifies patterns in a large set of correlations (correlations are described in Chapter 2). Suppose you measure numerous personality characteristics—for instance, reliable, emotional, insecure, generous, hardworking, nervous, punctual, self-conscious, ambitious, and creative—by asking many people to rate, on a 7-point scale, whether each characteristic describes them. For any two characteristics (e.g., insecure and nervous), the correlation between the two measures indicates how strongly the characteristics go together (i.e., whether people who say they are insecure also tend to say they are nervous).

573

Now suppose you want to know how, in general, the traits correlated with one another. You suddenly are faced with a lot of numbers: If 10 traits were measured, there are 45 pairs of traits and thus 45 correlations. If 100 traits are measured, there are 4950 correlations—way too many to keep track of without a mathematical tool. Factor analysis is that tool. It enables psychologists to sort through the correlations to find groups of measures that are correlated strongly. Trait psychologists reason that each of these strongly correlated groups represents one trait. In our example, the group including reliable, hardworking, punctual, and ambitious would represent the trait of conscientiousness.

In the study of personality traits, factor analysis usually yields a simple result. Only five or six traits are needed to describe the main individual differences in personality. One group of five traits is found so commonly that it is called the “Big Five” (Goldberg, 1993; John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008). The Big Five personality traits are found consistently when people’s descriptions of personality are analyzed with factor analysis. The same five traits are found when analyzing people’s descriptions of their own personality and people’s descriptions of others’ personalities (e.g., close friends or one’s spouse; McCrae & Costa, 1987). The Big Five traits—extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness to experience —are described in Table 13.4. (A hint for remembering them is that the first letters of the traits can be arranged to spell “ocean.”)

Table :

13.4

The Big Five Traits

Big Five Trait

Definition

Characteristics

Extraversion

A tendency to approach the social and material world in an energetic manner

Sociable, active, assertive

Agreeableness

An orientation toward positive, prosocial feelings and behaviors when interacting with others

Altruistic, trusting, modest

Conscientiousness

A tendency to control inappropriate emotions and impulses and to follow social rules

Reliable, hardworking, organized

Neuroticism

A tendency to experience negative emotions

Anxious, nervous, sad

Openness to experience

An orientation toward a complex mental and behavioral life, and a diversity of experiences

Creative, artistic, liberal

Research from John, Naumann, & Soto (2008)

The Big Five can be assessed easily. Table 13.5 contains a simple 10-item measure of the five personality factors (Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003).

In this measure of the Big Five trait variables, two items tap each of the factors: Extraversion (1, 6*), Agreeableness (2*, 7); Conscientiousness (3, 8*), Emotional Stability (4*, 9), and Openness to Experiences: (5, 10*). The asterisk indicates that the item is scored in reverse; strongly agreeing with the item gives one a lower score on the trait.

Table :

13.5

A Simple Big Five Measure

Disagree strongly

Disagree moderately

Disagree a little

Neither agree nor disagree

Agree a little

Agree moderately

Agree strongly

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

I see myself as:

1. _____ Extraverted, enthusiastic

2. _____ Critical, quarrelsome

3. _____ Dependable, self-disciplined

4. _____ Anxious, easily upset

5. _____ Open to new experiences, complex

6. _____ Reserved, quiet

7. _____ Sympathetic, warm

8. _____ Disorganized, careless

9. _____ Calm, emotionally stable

10. Conventional, uncreative

Reprinted from Journal of Research in Personality, 37: 504–528, Gosling et al., A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains, © 2003, with permission from Elsevier

Some trait theorists think that the Big Five trait model is missing something—a sixth trait called honesty/humility (Ashton & Lee, 2007). People with high scores on this trait display sincerity and a lack of pretentiousness. Evidence of this sixth factor came from a multinational study. Native speakers of each of seven different languages—Dutch, French, German, Hungarian, Italian, Korean, and Polish—rated themselves on a list of about 400 personality trait adjectives, some of which had not been included in prior Big Five studies. In each language, factor analysis revealed that six, not five, factors were needed to describe individual differences, the sixth being honesty/humility (Ashton et al., 2004).

THINK ABOUT IT

Do you think that everyone has the same five traits? Or might you have some distinctive personality traits that make you unique, yet that are not represented within the Big Five approach?

574

In sum, the lexical approach summarizes personality traits found in everyday language. The big question, though, is whether that is good enough. Ultimately, psychologists don’t just want to know about the language of personality; they want to know the “flesh-and-bones” of personality: the biological structures underlying differences between people.

On what trait did they differ? Both men were smart, ambitious, and hardworking. Neither was highly extraverted. Both may have been somewhat high on neuroticism. But they differed dramatically on the personality trait of honesty/humility. The humble Abe Lincoln was known as Honest Abe. Richard Nixon, known for saying that “I always do more than I say. I always produce more than I promise,” resigned from office due to the dishonesty of the Watergate scandal.

575

THIS JUST IN

Traits as Networks

When researchers give people personality tests, groups of test items correlate with one another. For example, people who say that they are (1) self-conscious also tend to report that they tend to (2) worry, (3) feel anxious, and (4) experience frightening thoughts, which creates positive correlations among the items. Why do the items correlate?

The cause, according to trait theory, is the influence of traits. In our example, the trait of neuroticism is said to cause the items to correlate with one another (Figure 13.5).

figure 13.5 Trait theory view

Although trait psychologists believe this, there are other ways of thinking about it. A research team in the Netherlands recently developed an alternative conception: not traits as causes of behavior, but traits as descriptions of “networks” of thoughts and actions (Cramer et al., 2012). In this view, networks arise because the psychological qualities measured by test items causally influence each other (Figure 13.6). Consider the four qualities we just mentioned. Self-consciousness might cause one to worry; worrying can cause anxiety; anxiety could cause frightening thoughts to enter the mind; and the frightening thoughts could feed back into the network, causing one to become more anxious—which, in turn, might result in greater self-consciousness.

figure 13.6 Network analysis view

Researchers use a statistical technique called network analysis to depict the relations among individual test items. The technique is complex, but the idea behind it is simple. Instead of adding up test items to give people a single score, network analysis analyzes the items one by one. A network graph depicts the ways in which items are linked (i.e., correlated with one another). As you can see in Figure 13.7, when researchers analyze personality test items this way, they find overlapping clusters of items that roughly correspond to the Big Five traits (Cramer et al., 2012). The findings are consistent with the theoretical analysis in Figure 13.7the network analysis.

figure 13.7 Is a trait a “thing”—a single structure in the mind or brain that causes consistent patterns of behavior? Maybe not. Trait terms might just be words that describe patterns of correlations among personality test items that are inherently interconnected. That is an implication of network analysis. In this network analysis of the correlations among items designed to measure the Big Five personality traits, items (indicated by numbers) that are closer in the network are more highly correlated. Traits (neuroticism, extraversion, etc.) do not cause the correlations to occur; they merely describe naturally occurring patterns in the data. The inset diagram in the upper right is what the network would look like if the five traits were independent.

576

Why is network analysis so important? There’s a critical difference between Figures 13.6 and 13.7: In Figure 13.7, there is no single “neuroticism” variable. In the network analysis, neuroticism is not a real “thing”—that is, not a structure in the mind that causes consistent patterns of behavior. Neuroticism is merely a term that describes networks of thoughts, feelings, and actions that go together inevitably. The same holds true for the other Big Five traits as well; each is reconceptualized as a description of networks of experiences and actions, rather than as a psychological structure that causes behavior.

The picture reveals something interesting. Items measuring the Big Five traits do not fall into five separate clumps; they do not appear as five distinct traits. Rather, items representing the supposedly different traits are intertwined. Network analysis thus suggests that personality structure consists not of five separate, discrete traits, but of an interconnected system of thoughts, feelings, and actions.

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 17

True or False?

  • /1demM1xIlY6ceiyMvdZG3vCbaQTJ1FO9BfTBy4P2w2S7/vQ5edXdLgDLGdKVULO0yAdAdJyJ1TiQUBFSsCulFeBh6o1yYBkhqe5FT93WPTtWSeW72TpE6HxpukBtvMe3ph3BJPlf7JeXiZUKNRvb5viXwowPBYwJ5JhCB8q171s3qCLE/qW7HjsZa9pDbqdcjG25EdwVvJ4z9GGysKQPdPPpoOwrl5yboCfEQbcd541NeExlz/GWg==
  • B+JCGGOn5+Jsfqw7Sgz204/h1Bmh5hn7gFxDY7X9EC5bqzqeq5BZoWxeWW4XWDunilVnX7Q8E/LVYB2pfAgiTNKfS0Ud6Fy8Y046i5qGyVwA1wWyILqyBxHvrWuggIV4L5JKXl4p0OWBOgLtYTtNTZvL7w1i5bOK+gE0HPKttnm6XCEmdX1fja1qWRdnVFjjWxR43Csn69TPQpYJzZMkrZEESPRxPjof0a5ypgcF9M3+q9vZMFVUdpgiHBI=
  • 4YweVEaI98OYGkPcOvqP82RlbkeH9j+qGMbiJLki7dM8r4mVqdJyLBb41tZTuQx6DcD7Uw9+BCSWPqU1DuOxQ3jKhPi4o8IUdrSANtY90t0gPEMhDMrdKaY56HjbPTICegAhAlU1voqTt7I76lO33FpxQtPm59hqHKVvRupB0usEWMrZ24Dh6VDvlCy0wOi0ImaSdTpXX03yK6WD4sYWes9jrXhC3WOSkSZDO5JRQb+RYWIVFwSc4W52pq8vaHpFNnu6kPTau+ugbMnkgFyYzYPYaik=

PERSONALITY TRAITS IN THE NERVOUS SYSTEM. People differ biologically in many ways (height, weight, blood pressure, bone density, etc.). Might some of the biological differences produce individual differences in personality traits? Many trait psychologists have pursued this possibility (Stelmack & Rammsayer, 2008). One of the first was the British psychologist Hans Eysenck.

Hans Eysenck, who identified extraversion and neuroticism as the two primary traits of personality.

577

Eysenck (1970) searched for the biological basis of extraversion. He hypothesized that extraverts (high-energy people who like lots of activity and social interaction) and introverts (reserved individuals who are happy sitting quietly, reading a book) should differ in brain functioning. Specifically, Eysenck predicted they would differ in cortical arousal, that is, activation in the surface regions of the brain (the cortex; see Chapter 3). People with naturally low levels of arousal should compensate for this by pursuing arousing activities that raise their arousal level. People with high levels of cortical arousal should avoid simulating environments, which make them overly aroused. Arousal level therefore causes people to become extraverts (low arousal) or introverts (high arousal).

Some evidence supports Eysenck’s idea. In electroencephalographic (EEG) research (see Chapter 2), investigators record the brain activity of introverts and extraverts while presenting simple stimuli, such as flashes of light or moderately loud noises. Just as Eysenck predicted, the brains of introverts react more strongly than those of extraverts to this physical stimulation (Stelmack & Rammsayer, 2008). Outside of the lab, in everyday life, the differences in brain reactivity may create differences in introverted versus extraverted behavior.

Eysenck also sought the biological basis of neuroticism, that is, the tendency to feel anxious and worried. Here, he predicted that individual differences in neuroticism would be linked to variations in the reactivity of the autonomic nervous system (ANS), which responds to environmental threats (see Chapter 3). People with high scores on neuroticism should have “jumpy” nervous systems. This prediction, however, has received less support (Stelmack & Rammsayer, 2008). Individual differences in self-reported neuroticism and ANS activity are related only weakly. Factors not anticipated by Eysenck, such as people’s subjective interpretations of events, contribute to anxious, neurotic response styles (M. Eysenck, 2013). This weak result has motivated some trait psychologists to seek an alternative to Eysenck’s theory.

One alternative was advanced by the British psychologist Jeffrey Gray and his colleagues (Corr, 2004; Gray & McNaughton, 2000; Pickering & Corr, 2008). Gray identified a problem in Eysenck’s theory of neuroticism: It failed to distinguish between two fundamentally different emotions, namely, fear and anxiety. In Gray’s theory of personality and motivation, different brain systems are said to underlie these different emotional responses.

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 18

Trait refers to the major ways in which people FTIZvvOu0bD4DF0c from each other in their average tendencies. They are Lwhn5mjnw4BqUErfLh9fKw== versus categorical and are said to be the jkZ5Ck7Br6hwyF2P of behavior. The statistical tool of sFwrC8RjfSlDvXjPDhnBcKItMn0= is used to identify the major individual differences in personality. Hans Eysenck hypothesized that differences between introverts and extraverts could be tied to differences in K2vS8WozSwB6aSVArSeD0g== arousal. His hypothesis that differences in neuroticism could be tied to differences in reactivity of the zPifQ5brCURiYGiU6ARqUA== nervous system has received little support.

Does it take an extravert to lead a social movement? The Indian spiritual and political leader Mohandas Gandhi led millions of Indian citizens in a struggle for human rights and political independence. Yet he was introverted. In his youth, he avoided contact with others. “My books and my lessons were my sole companions,” Gandhi reported. “I literally ran back [home from school] because I could not bear to talk to anybody” (Easwaran, 1997, p. 13).

Process: From Traits to Behavior

Preview Question

Question

How can Eysenck’s work link structures to processes in trait theory?

Traits are the personality structures of trait theory. What about personality processes?

Trait theorists have devoted less attention to dynamically shifting personality processes than to stable personality structures (Hampson, 2012; McCrae & Costa, 1996). The lexical approach to personality traits, for example, analyzes personality structure exclusively.

578

Biological research, however, does shed light on personality processes. Consider again the work of Eysenck. His analysis of cortical arousal and extraversion identifies brain processes that come into play as people encounter situations featuring low and high levels of stimulation. Other researchers have extended Eysenck’s analyses by showing how extraversion may be linked to bodily arousal, which, in turn, affects thinking processes (Humphreys & Revelle, 1984). Nonetheless, the analysis of personality processes is not the strong point of trait theories.

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 19

Research that links extraversion to bodily 95PO9DVPejW0k3sk is a promising way for trait theorists to link structure to 3pX8UQIUx3G8sPc1.

Assessment: Measuring Individual Differences

Preview Question

Question

How do trait theorists assess personality and what outcomes can be predicted by these assessments?

We’ve seen that analysis of personality processes is not a strength of trait theory. Personality assessment, however, is. Trait theorists have developed tests that reliably measure individual differences in personality traits.

COMPREHENSIVE SELF-REPORT QUESTIONNAIRES. The most common personality tests are self-report questionnaires in which people indicate their personal preferences and tendencies. Some of these tests aim for comprehensiveness; test creators seek to measure all the major personality traits, to provide a complete psychological portrait of the individual. Let’s look at one such test in detail.

The NEO-Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992; McCrae & Costa, 2010) measures individual differences in each of the Big Five personality traits: neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, conscientiousness, and agreeableness. (Through a historical quirk, it gets its name, NEO, from only the first three of those traits. It exists in different forms, called the NEO-PI-R and NEO-PI-3.)

The NEO includes about 50 test items that measure each of the five traits. Why so many? No one question can measure a broad personality trait. But a large set of questions can. For example, conscientiousness (see Table 13.4) test items ask about keeping one’s possessions in neat order, paying bills on time, working efficiently, finishing jobs, and so on. Adding together responses across the items provides an accurate measure of the trait.

As noted earlier, there are shorter tests (Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003; Rammstedt & John, 2007). Brevity sacrifices accuracy, however. When psychologists want highly accurate measures of Big Five traits, they prefer to use longer tests, such as the NEO.

PREDICTING BEHAVIOR. Do these personality tests predict people’s behavior? Often they do. Let’s see two examples, the first of which involves personality and personal space.

Most people have one or two personal spaces, that is, areas they decorate themselves. In your bedroom, for instance, you might arrange the furniture and choose art for the walls. Personality predicts decoration preferences. Researchers sent participants to individuals’ office spaces and bedrooms and asked them to judge the personality traits of the inhabitants. They made the judgments without ever meeting the occupants. Separately, the inhabitants of the rooms completed questionnaires measuring their personality traits. The ratings of conscientiousness, extraversion, and openness to experience that participants gave to occupants based on their rooms correlated significantly with the occupants’ self-ratings on those traits (Gosling et al., 2002).

579

Guess what types of people live in these rooms? Trait theory research indicates that people can judge the personality of the inhabitants merely by viewing the rooms, without even meeting the inhabitants themselves.

Personality traits also predict a more serious outcome: mortality. Researchers (Terracciano et al., 2008) measured personality traits in a large group of adults, and then followed their life course over decades. During this time, some research participants died. Remarkably, personality traits predicted length of life (Figure 13.8). People with more emotionally stable (low neuroticism) personalities, and those with higher levels of conscientiousness, lived longer (Terracciano et al., 2008).

figure 13.8 Personality traits predict longevity The graph shows longevity (age at death) among people with low, average, and high scores on the personality traits of emotional stability and conscientiousness. People with high scores on both emotional stability and conscientiousness lived longer (Terraciano et al., 2008).

Why would personality predict mortality? People with more emotional personalities may experience more life stress, which can impair health (Chapter 10). More conscientious people may maintain healthier lifestyles and have regular medical check-ups, which can prolong life.

THINK ABOUT IT

Conscientiousness predicts the neatness and organization of people’s living spaces. Does this mean that the trait of conscientiousness causally affects people’s behavior? Or might it be the opposite: Behavior affects self-reported conscientiousness? Self-report measures of conscientiousness ask people if they are “careless” and “disorganized.” People might base answers to these questions on knowledge of their everyday behavior, such as the care of their living spaces.

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 20

True or False?

  • RWMeTZYl0MXJMEsQ7Haq9NvQThgwd/vpClRsfE2zEc2FjmfwbfWrY3Yf4Tjw3zDvHvgv2xBev/mW+GeeyCRsXtqVWFVtRImpJyU0fG7CBywk2xmO6LOLUjxec0qD0/HCcGPK8+KcEqePLp+pjRgIvo/rm1Hecpbiezzu1ddxT8AD9hWSGD2IEvckW5s=
  • USlRO7Vw8WmDlRTbUgOfjBTxDcgAkIJFBU1scZsFAkXA4sP1BBfBmicBjYJ8XEpMd5CEHFuq38SXwkFUHdoqbRhdLn00zuPeow/sb/7dIdDwKONLV3Tnu/Q/vXJv5JnucLaQSLF/kOjis53ltu/20WW0Z6dZt47txY5FdYU4WrB2jYNqlucSLQUuntbFi03TwPo2UShXnVRlf17+bjNtzjiKpj4APrb/QkqF7Jn39vBGqCBkYjIj66OHpFuRW0YttddBD6AhKZT7w79suwyX+PmEB0bSAhiaVuvEFkRF6Mva8jSr
  • B5bU+Yr8YjdJhABpR/ZnUf1P9ulOfP/00YJeULq7n6vvXi09EUrzOmDtQLSb/NMyyhPHacPSyxSGs/ulBGlFhZ66qbNIKDerwzzlFmaixiD7DT7UpFlR1AWemBrCACPwgX9F0qKoiVU9Fald4wgvgD6CizhuywRO

Evaluation

Preview Question

Question

What are some strengths and weaknesses of trait theory?

Trait theories have three major strengths. They provide simple methods for assessing individual differences. They have spurred research into the connection between personality traits and biology. Their use of sophisticated statistical techniques brings scientific rigor that was sometimes lacking in prior theories of personality.

However, trait theories have limitations, too. One is that, like Rogers’s humanistic theory, they are insufficiently comprehensive. As we’ve noted, trait theory says little about personality processes—the dynamic interactions among thoughts and feelings that were highlighted, for example, by Freud. Trait theory also says little about inconsistencies in behavior. When asked to describe themselves, people often report behavior that is inconsistent; sometimes they act one way, sometimes another: “I am very shy, but I talk a lot once I get to know someone”; “I am organized for the most part, but I’m very rushed and messy at home”; “I have a very welcoming personality, but another aspect of my personality is my mean side” (Orom & Cervone, 2009). If you observe people’s behavior, you see these inconsistencies; most individuals sometimes look like they’re extraverts and other times look like they’re introverts (Fleeson, 2001). Because trait variables refer to average levels of behavior, they don’t explain such variations around the average. The psychologists who developed the fourth, and last, personality theory you’ll learn about have tried to overcome these two limitations of the trait approach.

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 21

Which of the following statements about the strengths and weaknesses of trait theories are true?

  • zogNSW9SFf0y/x3yh5mtby4HE1djT1AsRfLPnRQDIkuO4txnW51vYpxAI/u8Yu7yAWcEJNdU2de43b5C+QDyTCkNKE/08C0uTeZYE2hICCWICzqklRfGeWpj8flsSOu3N99jC+vA2Fhm/oFwTRYeDeYzvUda6qPO6dOjhpoZ0uYJrREs0dv94A==
  • y2QskcTp+IPKtopRFbP6gS7kxwHg/ox5jQbGwFOoGwL09A13tFedeLydbnaFaMj2laU1H1eMNLSgAqiwMpLzvzGHtTqZaWVbT9kD82WPlCF4MyllOM57NZnordEGiGmrlDLPHJqfsU7VJnZvKY37IAvZbwB8gpaMQM/spYTfb95ixU6l
  • GYbYOLKgjcvF54l5h9GQMsqyb5n18tO7mkgpACxqZY2zI+hEqFlac1+wQpQeAFE2fiarhpHw+8UL2l06SnWR6++y488GmaVS/UrvgTvc31R9V/mOmEPH3GykFFU4dHpQE+jiaGqGCKDCOJe/N4RuK3Op9aYDVcijOQnef0H+Py4o3Y6CELaGeqSOGfXHdFeLsvOyN8auUxqazddX4XeECMx4DekZVjdXLUqBUeJVQCvUB6rSbIevVqNz/8zaviuzWHJLst08hknwcHsiljL1zvzfbZ8ZyGl5
  • cOVEeEB1WPOmotYFYdWENA2tlAwxM4KScdz4qlNlamX2nH9h1Hi413TsyU9S1SgOmfyASwZpdcNitQ45Xqdr6Ngg95rMRua5DaWT3gAzxwRFunVtebngCQjAXCwgKnypEyLShkCLZu2RtT3TTLVeITKWsgIGFQHSiTGUVJ+BsgkEyWbiiuh1rg3zc91bschkGWSvsqpwp9QjdoYzS4IzDSNMha+NkdOTgcSoxFHcAu7+cksXypHUkWHBL5tO6jOnxZi3rYEBrRs=
  • qfehEF4DIWJUxvciK/Cnn4WOJvPWjVytv8yco1YCFmiK5fjt9jjIf3VIRoajwDFGYEO04+XD6TKh608m+wtSkPHopDIW0F4ezWucVeTKeWw2ZhHcewVS9QKbiZP063gaKoi08u40OXGqLdsDwhc652pCBhCGEUTnu+39Ep9DlnVnRwQ/RQMnHZyZ6Mw=