Internet reports tell of a Chinese man whose girlfriend needed treatments for cancer that the couple could not afford. Desperate, the man stole money to pay for the treatment. He was caught and jailed for 4 years—
Was it wrong for the man to steal money to save the woman’s life? Why or why not?
We ask this question not to determine the right answer but to note that there is no “right answer”—at least not in the same sense that there is a right answer to “What is 2 + 2?” or “Does the amount of water change when you pour it from a wide container into a narrow one?” The question about stealing money is not a factual question, but a moral question. The moral domain is a domain of reasoning that concerns personal rights, responsibilities, and obligations, especially with regard to the welfare of others. We’ll conclude this chapter by examining moral development, that is, the development of thinking about the moral domain.
647
How do we develop our morality?
Psychologist Lawrence Kohlberg established key foundations for the study of moral development. Inspired by Piaget, Kohlberg suggested that moral development occurs in stages. A moral stage is a period of development during which a person’s moral reasoning is consistently organized around a single way of thinking (Kohlberg & Hersh, 1977).
To identify moral stages, Kohlberg interviewed boys living in the Chicago area at 4-
Preconventional level: Children’s thinking is organized around ideas about rewards and punishments. In the first stage, “good” behavior consists of actions that avoid punishment. In the second stage, children recognize that actions that bring personal rewards also are good.
Conventional level: Children evaluate actions in terms of social conventions and norms. In the first stage of the conventional level, children assess whether behavior is consistent with being a “good girl” or “good boy.” This is an advance over the preconventional level, in that the child recognizes that a behavior might be wrong even if it is not punished. At the second stage, children have a “law and order” orientation. They think about their duty to uphold rules established by authorities.
Postconventional level: Individuals can transcend the rules of authority figures and base their reasoning in abstract principles. At the first stage of the post-
According to Kohlberg, social experiences affect people’s rate of progress through these stages and whether they ever reach the highest stage. Kohlberg called for moral education to increase the complexity of students’ moral reasoning (Kohlberg & Hersh, 1977).
Kohlberg’s work, although enormously influential, has been criticized, however. One criticism involves domains of reasoning. Kohlberg (like Piaget) assumed children would reason in a similar manner across different domains. But, evidence reveals domain specificity. For example, when thinking about the personal domain (i.e., actions that involve only oneself, such as hair style or food preferences), children employ principles involving personal choices and rights to privacy that are distinct to that domain (Nucci & Turiel, 2009).
A second criticism, which we will consider in detail after This Just In, involves gender.
648
THIS JUST IN
Do babies think of behavior as being “right” or “wrong,” “good” or “bad”? Kohlberg did not think so. As you learned, he said people did not achieve that type of thinking until the conventional level of moral development, which might not be reached until adolescence.
Research suggests, however, that babies are more moral than Kohlberg thought. Remarkable findings come from research in which babies 9 months of age and younger watched a puppet show (Hamlin & Wynn, 2011; Figure 14.20). In the show, one puppet tried to open a plastic box. Two other puppets either (1) helped the first to open the box or (2) jumped up and landed on the box’s lid, slamming it closed. The researchers then showed the babies both puppets—
649
No preference. The infants might have had no understanding of the moral content of the puppet show (i.e., the fact that one puppet’s actions were “good” and the other’s “bad”), and thus have shown no preference for one or the other puppet.
Prefer the “mean” puppet. The mean puppet was active and powerful; it jumped up in the air and slammed the lid. Babies might be attracted to action and power.
Prefer the “good” puppet. If babies understand good and bad behavior, they would prefer the helpful puppet, that is, the one whose actions were morally good.
Results confirmed possibility #3: Babies preferred the “good” puppet. Nearly 75% of them reached out to touch the puppet that, in the show, had been helpful. This means that babies are aware of good and bad behavior—
How does women’s moral reasoning differ from men’s?
You may have noticed something unusual about Kohlberg’s research, reviewed earlier: All the participants were male. The psychologist and feminist scholar Carol Gilligan (1977) noticed this and something more: None of the questions Kohlberg posed to participants dealt specifically with concerns unique to women’s lives. Furthermore, the style of moral thinking Kohlberg deemed most advanced—
Gilligan (1977) suggested that women’s moral reasoning differs from men’s. Women, she proposed, focus on personal relationships rather than abstract principles. They value obligations to others and try to solve problems without hurting others physically or psychologically.
In research, Gilligan posed moral questions pertinent to women’s lives (e.g., questions about pregnancy and a prospective abortion). She found that women progress through moral stages distinct from the ones identified by Kohlberg in research with men. The highest stage for women, Gilligan found, involved reasoning according to a principle of nonviolence; rather than adhering to abstract principles of justice, women adhered to the concrete human principle of not harming others (Gilligan, 1977).
Gilligan’s critique of Kohlberg’s research was powerful. But research conducted after she formulated her approach indicates that gender differences in moral reasoning are smaller than she expected. A meta-
650
This result, in which men and women were more similar than many anticipated, is not unusual. In many aspects of cognitive and social development, gender differences are smaller than people expect (Hyde, 2005). The relative lack of differences is important. Scientific findings do not support social stereotypes about stark differences between men’s and women’s tendencies and potentials. Instead, they are consistent with the idea that society should give all people, of both genders, equal opportunities for personal and professional success.
Carol Gilligan argued that because all of Kohlberg’s research participants were ScpYII3/BpqcUWHmj0Rseg==, his procedures were biased in their favor. In her research, she found that women’s highest moral stage was characterized by a principle of AeXwFtS0oRwlMNyyoNbM7g== and obligation to others, rather than adherence to abstract values of /FePT9UWaYdVZqCX. A later meta-