16.3 Dissociative and Conversion Disorders

Let’s hear once again from some people on an Internet discussion forum.

These reports seem so different from those written by people with schizophrenia, which you read earlier in this chapter. These people don’t report that God is speaking directly to them, that everyone else is a robot, or other such delusional beliefs. You might wonder what these stories about Megan, Skip, Kyle, and the others are even doing in a chapter on psychological disorders.

Here’s a hint: Megan doesn’t exist. Neither does Skip. The same goes for Kyle, Hera, and their friends. They don’t exist anywhere except within the minds of the writers (from http://www.psychforums.com/dissociative-identity). All these characters are alters —alternative personalities—invented by people with dissociative identity disorder.

In this section of the chapter, we’ll learn about the dissociative disorders and will then complete our coverage of psychological disorders by reviewing a puzzling syndrome known as conversion disorder.

Dissociative Disorders

Preview Questions

Question

Do people with dissociative identity disorder truly have multiple independent personalities?

What causes dissociative identity disorder?

What defines depersonalization/derealization disorder?

Can people completely lose their memory for who they are?

Dissociative disorders are a category of psychological disorders in which people experience profound alterations in their sense of personal identity, their conscious experiences of events, or their memory of their own past. Clinicians have identified a range of dissociative disorders; they include dissociative identity disorder, depersonalization/derealization disorder, and dissociative amnesia and fugue.

DISSOCIATIVE IDENTITY DISORDER. In dissociative identity disorder, people experience more than one self; that is, more than one personality seems to inhabit their mind. (The disorder was formerly known as multiple personality disorder.) According to the DSM, people qualify as having the disorder when they experience two or more personalities that are distinct. The different personalities are experienced as possessing different behavioral styles, emotional experiences, and personal goals. An additional factor in diagnosis involves memory. People with dissociative identity disorder experience memory disruptions; when in one personality state, they report being unable to remember what they were doing when in another.

Have you seen dramatic depictions of dissociative identity disorder on TV or film?

The disorder became well known in American society more than half a century ago thanks to a movie. The film The Three Faces of Eve (1957) told the story of Eve White, a modest woman with a wild alternative personality, as well as a third personality that was able to discuss the other two.

734

There can be no doubt that dissociative identity disorder severely disrupts people’s lives. Yet substantial questions about the disorder are unresolved (Gillig, 2009). One concerns the alternative personalities. Are they really separate psychological beings, each with its own, independent mental life? Or are they just ways of talking about normal variations in emotion and behavior (Merckelbach, Devilly, & Rassin, 2002)? Everyone is in a good mood sometimes and a bad mood other times. Everyone is fun-loving and outgoing in some settings, and quiet and shy in others. These variations do not indicate that everyone has multiple personalities, but merely that people’s experiences and behaviors naturally vary from one situation to another. It can be difficult, then, to discern whether people with dissociative identity disorder truly have multiple personalities or simply observe variations in their own behavior, try to understand themselves, and, in so doing, create stories in which they describe themselves in terms of different characters.

A second question is the cause of dissociative identity disorder. Many clinicians have suggested that its causes lie in childhood (Gillig, 2009). Children who experience mistreatment, including sexual abuse, may cope with the trauma by convincing themselves that it happened “to somebody else”—an alternative personality. This was the explanation given in the famous case of Sybil (Schreiber, 1973), a woman whose multiple personalities reportedly were caused by physical abuse suffered at the hands of her mother (but see This Just In). Others, while recognizing the reality of the disorder, question whether evidence truly supports trauma-based explanations. They suggest, instead, that the disorder may develop through social processes in which people adopt different social roles that call for different types of behavior and gradually come to think of these roles as “selves” or “personalities” (Lilienfeld et al., 1999).

The Three Faces of Eve, a 1957 movie starring Joanne Woodward as Eve, brought multiple personality disorder (now known as dissociative identity disorder) to the attention of the general public.

THIS JUST IN

Sybil

Dissociative disorder burst into the public awareness in the 1970s, thanks to psychology’s most famous case study of the past half-century. The case of Sybil (a pseudonym, to protect her identity) was bizarre and gripping. Sybil displayed multiple personalities—16 of them! There emerged from her mind a toddler, a grandmother, a couple of promiscuous girls, a couple of boys, and more, each with distinctive names, voices, and characteristics.

Sybil’s therapist, a well-known psychoanalyst named Connie Wilbur, worked with Sybil to uncover the origins of her multiple personalities. Together, they searched Sybil’s mind for buried memories of childhood trauma—and found them. While in therapy with Dr. Wilbur, Sybil recalled horrific instances of child abuse perpetrated by her parents, while she was being raised in a small rural town in the Midwest.

Dr. Wilbur and Sybil made her case public. They collaborated with a professional writer, Flora Rheta Schreiber, on Sybil: The True Story of a Woman Possessed by Sixteen Separate Personalities (Schreiber, 1973), a book detailing the childhood horrors that presumably gave birth to Sybil’s multiple personalities.

It sold like hotcakes. Although many professional books on mental health sell only a few thousand copies, Schreiber’s sold 6 million! Sybil the book was then turned into Sybil the TV miniseries, which was seen by 40 million Americans.

The case of Sybil affected society, including the mental health professions. Sybil made therapists and patients more aware of dissociative disorder. As a result, far more cases of the disorder were diagnosed after its publication than before. The case contributed to the inclusion of the disorder in the DSM. Wilbur and Schreiber appeared to have done a great service to mental health by uncovering facts about multiple personality … until the following news came in.

735

Much of the content of Wilbur’s and Schreiber’s report was a hoax. That was the conclusion reached by author Debbie Nathan, whose painstaking journalistic research has punched huge holes in the Sybil story. When she traveled to Sybil’s hometown and spoke with people who had known her family well, none reported any sign of child abuse. When she read correspondence between Sybil and Dr. Wilbur, she found a confession; Sybil had informed her analyst that she was “none of the things I pretended to be. … I do not have any multiple personalities. … I have been essentially lying. … I was trying to show you I felt I needed help.” Sybil had become emotionally dependent on her doctor, made up stories she thought her doctor wanted to hear, and explained to Wilbur that she had been afraid that if she told the truth, “you would be angry … [and] would not let me come to talk with you anymore” (Nathan, 2011, p. 106).

Sybil Sally Field (with glasses) played Sybil in a 1976 TV movie based on the book Sybil: The True Story of a Woman Possessed by Sixteen Separate Personalities. Joanne Woodward, who starred in The Three Faces of Eve, played her therapist.

Incredibly, despite the confession, Wilbur remained certain that her original diagnosis of Sybil was correct! She stuck to her diagnosis of multiple personality disorder and worked to convince Schreiber that Sybil’s symptoms were real and that their causes were childhood abuse. Nathan’s journalistic detective work suggests that this book involved blatant deception, including the creation of a fake diary that told of Sybil’s mental distress in the years before she knew Wilbur.

Sybil truly was mentally distressed. But the bizarre details—the 16 personalities, the stories of childhood abuse—apparently were fictions. Ultimately, what the famed case primarily illustrates is how patients may try to please their therapists, how therapists may be overconfident in their diagnoses, and how everybody is prone to bending the truth when trying to sell a lot of books.

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 13

kgPpDOSRdxhs9Dg/oexwQJ3nLxrXiFCEdvnJHeBdyKuC0nsY8mbvCT6V6FC2jGZqdA3cbRUde6gRLG62x2RJo0m6Pc54uhC9LW9PWDKnYq9dSd5FOvMHOvPX8vxETCOpyHyDbrYWgZUxmJTbaBlDBQ==
Sybil lied because she had become emotionally dependent on her therapist and wanted to please her in order to continue their relationship.

There is no single, standard therapy strategy for treating dissociative identity disorder. Some therapists employ cognitive therapy designed to teach clients ways of coping with stress that are more adaptive than switching personalities (Gillig, 2009). Others use hypnosis (see Chapter 9) in an effort to help clients remain calm while focusing their attention on traumatic memories or feelings that may underlie their disorder (Kluft, 2012).

DEPERSONALIZATION/DEREALIZATION DISORDER. A second dissociative disorder is depersonalization/derealization disorder, which is defined by a change in people’s conscious experiences. In depersonalization/derealization disorder, people do not feel fully involved in their own experiences. They go through their day with strange detachment, as if they are merely observing the day’s events rather than taking part in them (Sierra, 2009). According to DSM-5 criteria, a person is classified as having depersonalization/derealization disorder (previously known as depersonalization disorder) based on the persistent presence of either (or both) of these two key symptoms:

Adam Duritz, lead singer of the band Counting Crows, describes—from personal experience with depersonalization/derealization disorder—what it’s like to have this particular dissociative disorder. It “makes the world seem like it’s not real, as if things aren’t taking place. It’s hard to explain, but you feel untethered. … Because nothing seems real, it’s hard to connect with the world or the people in it because they’re not there. You’re not there … you feel like you don’t exist” (Duritz, 2008).

Adam Duritz, lead singer of the band Counting Crows, suffers from depersonalization/derealization disorder.

Not all cases of depersonalization are alike; there is a range of severity (Sierra, 2009). For some people, feelings of detachment occur only rarely and do not substantially disrupt their lives. For others, depersonalization/derealization experiences are nearly continual, and personal well-being is severely impaired.

DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA AND FUGUE. The third dissociative disorder we’ll review disrupts memory. In dissociative amnesia, people are unable to remember significant personal information—facts and experiences that normally would be highly memorable. Some people with dissociative amnesia forget specific personal experiences that were highly stressful or traumatic. Others’ memory loss is more widespread; they may forget major aspects of their own life history.

On rare occasions, individuals with dissociative amnesia experience dissociative fugue, which is a complete loss of memory for one’s own personal identity. Fugue states, when they occur, commonly prompt unexpected travel (the word fugue, in Italian, means “escape” or “take flight”). People move to a new location and may establish a new identity. When they recover memory of their original, true identity, they may have no memory of the fugue period. Cases of dissociative fugue are rare but have been documented, including among well-known people.

Case study evidence shows what it’s like to experience dissociative amnesia and fugue. Consider these two cases:

These two cases are particularly interesting in that they were reported by neuroscientists who used brain-imaging techniques to identify the biological bases of the men’s psychological problems. The men were found to have lower than normal activity in the hippocampus, a brain region known to be involved in the formation of new memories (Kikuchi et al., 2010).

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Match the disorder on the left with three facts about it on the right:

Question 14

1. Dissociative identity disorder

a. May involve the feeling that things are dreamlike or unreal.

2. Depersonalization/derealization disorder

b. May have its origin in childhood trauma or could simply be an extreme manifestation of the different social roles a person adopts.

3. Dissociative amnesia

c. In rare cases, individuals experience fugue states that prompt unexpected travel.

d. Characterized by a feeling of detachment from one’s own experiences

e. Characterized by loss of memory for significant personal information

f. Was once known as multiple personality disorder

g. People question whether individuals with this disorder truly experience separate personalities

h. Characterized by the feeling that objects and other people are not real

i. Associated with decreased functioning of the hippocampus

Answer: 1bfg, 2adh, 3cei

737

Agatha Christie, author of Murder on the Orient Express and other mystery novels, became the world’s best-selling fiction writer. She also provided the world with a nonfiction mystery: One winter’s night she disappeared without telling anyone—including her 7-year-old daughter—of her plans or whereabouts. Days later, people took notice of an outgoing, intellectual woman calling herself “Mrs. Theresa Neale” who had checked into a hotel about 250 miles from Christie’s home. Mrs. Neale turned out to be Christie. Some suggest that the author suffered from a dissociative fugue in which she forgot her own identity, took up a new one, and began traveling. She later regained her identity and said she’d experienced a “nervous breakdown” (Elliot, 2009).

CULTURAL OPPORTUNITIES

Latah

Boo!

If you sneak up on people and yell, “Boo,” they will startle. The response is usually brief: a sudden wide-eyed facial expression, a quick bodily reaction, and a temporary increase in breathing and heart rate. Then psychological life returns to normal.

In the Southeast Asian nation of Malaysia, however, some people display a response to startle, called latah (Simons, 1996), that might strike you as abnormal. A nineteenth-century British traveler named O’Brien was the first Westerner to describe it.

O’Brien observed that startling stimuli—a loud noise, a snake in the woods—initially produced the same reactions in Malaysians as in people from the West: surprise, jumping back, and often an “involuntary exclamation … characterized by … obscenity.” But some Malaysians would then enter a trance state in which they lost voluntary control of their behavior. Their actions appeared to be controlled by other people and environmental events rather than by themselves. “Without encouragement,” O’Brien wrote, people would “involuntarily imitate the words, sounds or gestures of those around them … [they would] completely abandon themselves to my will and powers of direction” (in Tanner & Chamberland, 2001, p. 528). Latah is this “mindless” imitation of others and obedience to their commands.

Evidence suggests that, here in the twenty-first century, latah still exists. Researchers (Tanner & Chamberland, 2001) interviewed 15 Malaysian women known, in their communities, to experience latah. They found that, when startled, the women repeated sounds, imitated gestures, and obeyed commands in a manner that appeared largely out of their control.

Latah can be understood, in part, in terms of biology. Throughout the world, some people experience larger startle reactions than others due to differences in the brain systems that produce the startle response (Dreissen et al., 2012). Within Malaysia, it’s likely such people are the individuals prone to experiencing latah. But culture is also critical. Latah is a culturally linked psychological syndrome; in other words, people only experience it if they grow up in cultures where it is an established and socially accepted pattern of response (Tseng, 2006).

What do you think of latah? It resembles dissociative disorders, in that conscious experience is altered and people’s normal personal identity escapes them. But is this a mental disorder?

This is the sort of question mental health professionals face when working with people from a culture unfamiliar to them. If people’s behavior seems strange, is it safe to conclude that they have a psychological disorder? In general, no. The behavior might be a normal reaction within the culture in which it originated. As one psychiatrist explained, “From a diagnostic point of view, it is necessary to be careful in labeling ‘peculiar behavior’ as a ‘disorder’ simply because it is unfamiliar. A good example is provided by … latah. (Many) behavioral scientists favor the view that latah is a social behavior and not a ‘disorder’ … even though some psychiatrists have considered it a psychopathological condition” (Tseng, 2006, p. 559).

738

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 15

In Malaysia, many individuals experience latah, a trance state following a QlVhZBt7XsTrx1Hw response during which they imitate and obey those around them. Latah resembles UbQ2SiRp9/mEQHLSG4ajkMAlMtc= disorders, but prior to concluding that it is a disorder, we should consider the context or 2ZHcAiXwK5/Kih0r in which it occurs.

Dissociative Disorders and the Law

Preview Question

Question

Are people with dissociative identity disorder responsible for crimes committed by their alternative personalities?

Dissociative identity disorder raises not only psychological questions, but also moral and legal concerns.

Sometimes we condemn behavior as immoral. We do this when the behavior violates a moral rule. What about behavior that violates a rule, but is performed by an alter —an alternative personality that seizes control of the actions of someone suffering from dissociative identity disorder? One sufferer writes, “One of my alters (a 17-year-old sexual deviant) went and had sex with someone a few times over multiple years. … Logically I understand that my body went and had sex with someone that was not my husband. … Logically he understands that the person that had the affair was not his wife. … But emotionally we’re a bit of a mess” (http://www.psychforums.com/dissociative-identity/topic24140.html).

A similar question arises in legal settings (Farrell, 2011). Suppose a crime is committed, but the criminal, a person with dissociative identity disorder, reports the following: “I would never do that. I don’t even remember any of the criminal behavior. The crime wasn’t done by me. It was done by my alter.” Does this individual, who suffers from a severe mental illness, have the same legal responsibility as someone without a mental illness who planned and intentionally committed a crime?

Legal scholars have addressed this issue. Elyn Saks (featured in this chapter’s opening vignette) explains that a standard legal principle is the following: People are found innocent of acts if they “did not have the capacity … not to act” (Saks, 1991, p. 431; emphasis added). In other words, people with no control over their behavior cannot be found guilty. Imagine someone who is sleepwalking. If the person sleepwalks into a store and walks out without paying, she can’t be found guilty of shoplifting. Because she was asleep, she had no control over her behavior—she was not able “not to act.” Lawyers defending a person with dissociative identity disorder may argue, similarly, that their client had no control over the actions of an alter.

Hillside Strangler Kenneth Bianchi was one of two men charged in the Hillside Strangler case, involving a series of kidnappings and murders in Southern California in the late 1970s. His defense? Multiple personality disorder. Bianchi claimed that the crimes had not been committed by him, but by an alter—one of his alternative personalities. The legal strategy failed; Bianchi received a sentence of life imprisonment.

The psychologist John Kihlstrom (2005) notes a different legal circumstance. Suppose a person insists on his innocence in a criminal case, but one day an alter confesses to the crime. Should the legal system convict the person based on the confession of his alter?

People charged with crimes have the legal option of pleading not guilty by reason of insanity. In the insanity defense, people admit to committing the crime—to being the physical agent who caused the crime to occur—but claim that, due to mental illness, their physical behavior was out of their control. For example, people might claim that they suffer from dissociative identity disorder and that, at the time of the crime, their behavior was controlled not by themselves, but by an alter.

739

How successful is this defense? Not very. The claim of insanity due to dissociative disorder rarely has produced not-guilty verdicts (Farrell, 2011). Sometimes courts are not convinced that the defendant really is mentally ill. Other times they accept the claim of mental illness, but judge that it is not an excuse; for instance, a court may decide that whatever the defendant’s personality at the time of a crime, that personality had the responsibility to tell right from wrong. Either way, the insanity defense fails (Fouche & Klesty, 2011).

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 16

XfTmjuT/RWXic6bmZqwqiCdpn1TOKuhx34EpWdNfQWw8bF/z6jHUcp5zKlheRr/Qp8J1hqoBF+halrv77+ryJ+P4KCvFHz5r6HRhbxNMKb9D44S3nEHv2VE5iExZ/kPgyPTvyAj6hFoEysB3zLsBVXtK66DSnqyShY5xv7LMYYpX9HSpeYJUrOzdWf1iAaGJfvU+ZO/X2ku8g/Tnja5qc2ZCmV2WcRZ4mDp9GIZNf10d3A5LJ5oZF30sMXC7Ch8vTFW/WY/HaniNBrqCxGqZxZICQe5wdhi/hUgj001hFG21q0Kh96WR/AoLZGFrFlEFllECPD8MZYvaOEj3PBT4cgoDg5IozK1jjIf64Kd+SLC4BA6o+8SRq9twRiDX1nFU
This statement is incorrect because the insanity defense rarely works in the case of dissociative disorder. It is also incorrect because juries rarely believe the defendant is mentally ill, and even when they do, such jurors do not agree that this diagnosis provides a reasonable excuse.

Conversion Disorder

Preview Question

Question

What could cause an individual to experience paralysis in the absence of a medical condition?

The last psychological disorder we will review, conversion disorder, is among the first to be studied systematically. Freud saw patients with the disorder in the late nineteenth century and based much of his psychoanalytic theory of personality on ideas about how conversion disorder develops and how it can be cured.

Jean Charcot, a nineteenth-century French physician, is shown demonstrating to colleagues a case of conversion disorder (called hysteria in Charcot’s time): a female patient exhibiting a physical symptom, fainting, in the absence of any biomedical condition that could explain the symptom. Early in his career, Sigmund Freud studied under Charcot and was greatly influenced by his theories on the psychological causes of physical symptoms.

PHYSICAL SYMPTOMS AND EMOTIONAL DISTRESS. Conversion disorder is a psychological disorder in which people experience physical symptoms that cannot be explained by any medical condition. A person might report paralysis of a hand or arm, or difficulty seeing or hearing, without there being any detectable biological cause. Most editions of the DSM, including DSM-5, have suggested that the physical symptoms may have an emotional basis, with psychological stress or trauma triggering the disorder. Treatment of the disorder is particularly difficult because patients commonly believe they are afflicted by a malady that is physical, not psychological, and thus may resist psychological therapy (Krull & Schifferdecker, 1990).

Freud explained such cases through his theory of the unconscious. The unconscious mind, according to the founder of psychoanalysis, holds repressed ideas that the physical symptoms symbolically represent. A repressed traumatic memory that involved the use of one’s hand, for example, might cause a hand paralysis.

Today, most psychologists do not endorse Freud’s explanation of the disorder. In fact, some question the entire existence of conversion disorder. They say patients might be faking their symptoms to get attention from others, or might be overly sensitive to minor physical symptoms with ordinary medical causes. Recent brain-imaging evidence, however, may quiet the skeptics.

CONVERSION DISORDER AND BRAIN RESEARCH. Researchers (Voon et al., 2010) studied patients with motor conversion disorder, in which the symptoms are motor movement disorders (e.g., muscular shaking) with no medical explanation. They compared them with a control group of psychologically healthy individuals lacking any sign of conversion disorder. The researchers took brain images while participants responded to a series of pictures displaying either fearful or happy human faces. Afterward, they conducted analyses to determine not only which brain regions were active during the task, but also how different regions of the brain interacted with each other during task performance.

740

The results provided remarkable brain-level insight into conversion disorder. Compared with psychologically healthy participants, in the brains of people with conversion disorder there was more communication between two regions of the brain: (1) the amygdala, which is involved in the detection of emotionally relevant stimuli (see Chapter 10), and (2) the motor cortex, which controls the planning and execution of muscular movements (Voon et al., 2010). These links between emotion and motor-movement centers of the brain may be a biological basis for the unusual physical symptoms experienced by emotionally distressed people with conversion disorder.

OTHER PSYCHOLOGICAL SOMATIC DISORDERS. In DSM-5, conversion disorder is part of a broader category of disorders in which psychological factors substantially affect the experience of one’s body and potential physical illness. This broader category also includes somatic symptom disorder and illness anxiety disorder.

In somatic symptom disorder, people are excessively distressed about a physical symptom. Unlike conversion disorder, the cause of the physical symptom may be known; it may have a clear medical basis. The psychological problem is the person’s degree of reaction to the symptom. For example, a heart attack victim who has a medical prognosis predicting full recovery, but who obsessively worries about having another heart attack and substantially restricts work and social activities to avoid one, would be exhibiting somatic symptom disorder.

In illness anxiety disorder, a person is excessively distressed about his or her physical well-being even in the complete absence of physical symptoms. The disorder is present when people are highly anxious about their health and frequently check themselves for signs of illness, despite being physically fit.

WHAT DO YOU KNOW?…

Question 17

wJbNA1JaZceaeJEVBaHEJkXvkr5u9CNb8rWyZpFNqq9ARS67ZMqlydnU9UmL4UaLY9Lwvehne947RB0lkw/TljVWKe6rxS+gkqPHejhoL42ZoWgtxoBsIIDS2EK/NohbscHmgm61Vo0bpEik9CBuVTZra7w0QYV0goEiAXQBF63OIMkxtS4VxCfYeEnfzqgMj9MWThpIfIiCxRPDIwKFOPNZaj4=
This statement is incorrect according to brain research indicating that, among people diagnosed with conversion disorder, there is greater communication between the amygdala (which is active during the detection of emotional stimuli) and the motor cortex. Research at the brain level, then, confirms the reality of a psychological experience.

Question 18

In somatic symptom disorder and illness anxiety disorder, individuals are excessively stressed about physical symptoms; however, it is only in rN5vX07vaOtcijpYdGI8QhSgUGM= disorder that the individual has an actual physical symptom to worry about.