Psychological Needs as Motivators

331

KEY THEME

According to the motivation theories of Maslow and of Deci and Ryan, psychological needs must be fulfilled for optimal human functioning.

KEY QUESTIONS

Why did you enroll in college? What motivates you to study long hours for an important exam? To push yourself to achieve a new “personal best” at a favorite sport? Rather than being motivated by a biological need or drive like hunger, such behaviors are more likely motivated by the urge to satisfy psychological needs.

In this section, we’ll first consider two theories that attempt to explain psychological motivation: Abraham Maslow’s famous hierarchy of needs and the more recently developed self-determination theory of Edward L. Deci and Richard M. Ryan.

Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs

A major turning point in the discussion of human needs occurred when humanistic psychologist Abraham Maslow developed his model of human motivation in the 1940s and 1950s. Maslow acknowledged the importance of biological needs as motivators. But once basic biological needs are satisfied, he believed, “higher” psychological needs emerge to motivate human behavior.

The centerpiece of Maslow’s (1954, 1968) model of motivation was his famous hierarchy of needs, summarized in Figure 8.3. Maslow believed that people are motivated to satisfy the needs at each level of the hierarchy before moving up to the next level. As people progressively move up the hierarchy, they are ultimately motivated by the desire to achieve self-actualization. The lowest levels of Maslow’s hierarchy emphasize fundamental biological and safety needs. At the higher levels, the needs become more social and psychologically growth-oriented, culminating in the need to achieve self-actualization.

image
FIGURE 8.3 Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs Abraham Maslow believed that people are innately motivated to satisfy a progression of needs, beginning with the most basic physiological needs. Once the needs at a particular level are satisfied, the individual is motivated to satisfy the needs at the next level, steadily progressing upward. The ultimate goal is self-actualization, the realization of personal potential.
Source: Research from Maslow (1970).

332

What exactly is self-actualization? Maslow (1970) himself had trouble defining the term, saying that self-actualization is “a difficult syndrome to describe accurately.” Nonetheless, Maslow defined self-actualization in the following way:

MYTH SCIENCE

Is it true that people need to satisfy basic needs before they can try to achieve higher needs, like artistic expression?

It may be loosely described as the full use and exploitation of talents, capacities, potentialities, etc. Such people seem to be fulfilling themselves and to be doing the best that they are capable of doing. . . . They are people who have developed or are developing to the full stature of which they are capable.

Maslow identified several characteristics of self-actualized people, which are summarized in Table 8.2.

image
SOURCE: Research from Maslow (1970).

image
Courtesy of Robert D. Farber University Archives at Brandeis University

It is quite true that man lives by bread alone—where there is no bread. But what happens to man’s desires when there is plenty of bread and when his belly is chronically filled? At once other (and “higher”) needs emerge and these, rather than physiological hungers, dominate the organism. And when these in turn are satisfied, again new (and still “higher”) needs emerge, and so on. That is what we mean by saying that the basic human needs are organized into a hierarchy of relative prepotency.

—Abraham Maslow (1943)

Maslow’s model of motivation generated considerable research, especially during the 1970s and 1980s. Some researchers found support for Maslow’s ideas (see Graham & Balloun, 1973). Others, however, criticized his model on several points (see Fox, 1982; Neher, 1991; Wahba & Bridwell, 1976).

First, Maslow’s notion that we must satisfy needs at one level before moving to the next level has not been supported by empirical research (Sheldon & others, 2001). Second, Maslow’s concept of self-actualization is very vague and almost impossible to define in a way that would allow it to be tested scientifically. And, Maslow’s initial studies on self-actualization were based on limited samples with questionable reliability. For example, Maslow (1970) often relied on the life stories of acquaintances whose identities were never revealed. He also studied the biographies and autobiographies of famous historical figures he believed had achieved self-actualization, such as Eleanor Roosevelt, Abraham Lincoln, and Albert Einstein.

There is a more important criticism. Despite the claim that self-actualization is an inborn motivational goal toward which all people supposedly strive, most people do not experience or achieve self-actualization. Maslow (1970) himself wrote that self-actualization “can seem like a miracle, so improbable an event as to be aweinspiring.” Maslow explained this basic contradiction in a number of different ways. For instance, he suggested that few people experience the supportive environment that is required to achieve self-actualization.

333

Perhaps Maslow’s most important contribution was to encourage psychology to focus on the motivation and development of psychologically healthy people (King, 2008). In advocating that idea, he helped focus attention on psychological needs as motivators.

Deci and Ryan’s Self-Determination Theory

Self-determination theory, abbreviated SDT, is a contemporary theory of motivation developed by University of Rochester psychologists Edward L. Deci and Richard M. Ryan (2000, 2012a, 2012b). Much like Maslow’s theory, SDT’s premise is that people are actively growth oriented and that they move toward a unified sense of self and integration with others. To realize optimal psychological functioning and growth throughout the life span, Ryan and Deci contend that three innate and universal psychological needs must be satisfied:

Like Maslow, Deci and Ryan view the need for social relationships as a fundamental psychological motive. The benefits of having strong, positive social relationships are well documented (Leary & Allen, 2011). Another well-established psychological need is having a sense of competence or mastery (Bandura, 1997; White, 1959).

One subtle difference in Maslow’s views compared to those of Deci and Ryan has to do with the definition of autonomy. Deci and Ryan’s definition of autonomy emphasizes the need to feel that your activities are self-chosen and self-endorsed (Ryan & Deci, 2011; Niemiec & others, 2010). This reflects the importance of selfdetermination in Deci and Ryan’s theory. In contrast, Maslow’s view of autonomy stressed the need to feel independent and focused on your own potential (see Table 8.2 on the previous page).

How does a person satisfy the needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness? In a supportive social, psychological, and physical environment, an individual will pursue interests, goals, and relationships that tend to satisfy these psychological needs. In turn, this enhances the person’s psychological growth and intrinsic motivation (Sheldon & Ryan, 2011). Intrinsic motivation is the desire to engage in tasks that the person finds inherently satisfying and enjoyable, novel, or optimally challenging. The doctors, nurses, and other volunteers who traveled to Nepal in the Prologue story displayed intrinsic motivation, taking time away from work and family to contribute their efforts to helping others in a distant land.

image
Roz Chast The New Yorker Collection/The Cartoon Bank

In contrast, extrinsic motivation consists of external influences on behavior, such as rewards, social evaluations, rules, and responsibilities. Of course, much of our behavior in daily life is driven by extrinsic motivation (Ryan & La Guardia, 2000). According to SDT, the person who has satisfied the needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness actively internalizes and integrates different external motivators as part of his or her identity and values (Ryan & Deci, 2012). In effect, the person incorporates societal expectations, rules, and regulations as values or rules that he or she personally endorses.

What if one or more of the psychological needs are thwarted by an unfavorable environment, one that is overly challenging, controlling, rejecting, punishing, or even abusive? According to SDT, the person may compensate with substitute needs, defensive behaviors, or maladaptive behaviors. For example, if someone is frustrated in satisfying the need for relatedness, he or she may compensate by chronically seeking the approval of others or by pursuing substitute goals, such as accumulating money or material possessions.

334

In support of self-determination theory, Deci and Ryan have compiled an impressive array of studies, including cross-cultural studies (Deci & Ryan, 2000, 2012a, 2012b). Taking the evolutionary perspective, they also argue that the needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness have adaptive advantages. For example, the need for relatedness promotes resource sharing, mutual protection, and the division of work, increasing the likelihood that both the individual and the group will survive.

Competence and Achievement Motivation

KEY THEME

Competence and achievement motivation are important psychological motives.

KEY QUESTIONS

For more on the historic climb by Pasang and her teammates, visit http://www.k2expedition2014.org/

In self-determination theory, Deci and Ryan identified competence as a universal motive. You are displaying competence motivation when you strive to use your cognitive, social, and behavioral skills to be capable and exercise control in a situation (White, 1959). Competence motivation provides much of the motivational “push” to prove to yourself that you can successfully tackle new challenges, such as striving to do well in this class or making it to the top of a steep trail.

A step beyond competence motivation is achievement motivation—the drive to excel, succeed, or outperform others at some task. For example, in the chapter Prologue, Pasang clearly displayed a high level of achievement motivation. Climbing Everest and becoming an internationally certified mountaineering guide are just two examples of Pasang’s drive to achieve. In fact, in late July 2014, Pasang summited K-2, the second-highest and, some say, most dangerous mountain in the world. Pasang and her two Nepali teammates were the first all-female team to successfully conquer K-2. Their mission’s avowed purpose was to bring attention to the importance of climate change and to encourage sustainable development in the Himalayas.

In the 1930s, Henry Murray identified 20 fundamental human needs or motives, including achievement motivation. Murray (1938) defined the “need to achieve” as the tendency “to overcome obstacles, to exercise power, [and] to strive to do something difficult as well and as quickly as possible.” Also in the 1930s, Christiana Morgan and Henry Murray (1935) developed a test to measure human motives called the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT). The TAT consists of a series of ambiguous pictures. The person being tested is asked to make up a story about each picture, and the story is then coded for different motivational themes, including achievement. In Chapter 10, on personality, we’ll look at the TAT in more detail.

image
Extraordinary Achievement Motivation Thousands of students from 120 countries compete each year in the Google Science Fair. Showing off their trophies—and their grins—are the 2013 winners: 14-year-old Viney Kumar, from Australia, who invented a signalling system for emergency vehicles; 15-year-old Ann Makosinski, from Canada, who designed a flashlight without batteries or moving parts; 16-year-old Elif Bilgin, from Turkey, who developed a method to create bioplastics from banana peels; and 17-year-old Eric Chen, from the U.S., who discovered a new approach to finding better anti-flu medications.
© Andrew >Federman/federman.photography@gmail.com

In the 1950s, David McClelland, John Atkinson, and their colleagues (1953) developed a specific TAT scoring system to measure the need for achievement, often abbreviated nAch. Other researchers developed questionnaire measures of achievement motivation (Spangler, 1992; Ziegler & others, 2010).

Over the next four decades, McClelland and his associates investigated many different aspects of achievement motivation, especially its application in work settings. In cross-cultural studies, McClelland explored how differences in achievement motivation at the national level have influenced economic development (McClelland, 1961, 1976; McClelland & Winter, 1971). He also studied organizational leadership and power motivation—the urge to control or influence the behavior of other people or groups (McClelland, 1975, 1989).

335

Hundreds of studies have shown that measures of achievement motivation generally correlate well with various areas of success, such as school grades, job performance, and worker output (Senko & others, 2008). This is understandable, since people who score high in achievement motivation expend their greatest efforts when faced with moderately challenging tasks. In striving to achieve the task, they often choose to work long hours and have the capacity to delay gratification and focus on the goal. They also tend to display original thinking, seek expert advice, and value feedback about their performance (McClelland, 1985).

image
Celebrating Achievement in a Collectivistic Culture Does achievement motivation look the same in every culture? Comparing statements by Japanese and American athletes during the Olympics, Hazel Rose Markus found that Japanese emphasized the importance of their supportive relationships, but Americans tended to see their wins as an individual achievement (Markus & others, 2006). Here, Japanese track star Satomi Kubokura celebrates after winning a gold medal in the 19th Asian Athletics Championship in Kobe, Japan.
Kiyoshi Ota/Getty Images

ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION AND CULTURE

When it is broadly defined as “the desire for excellence,” achievement motivation is found in many, if not all, cultures. In individualistic cultures, like those that characterize North American and European countries, the need to achieve emphasizes personal, individual success rather than the success of the group. In these cultures, achievement motivation is also closely linked with succeeding in competitive tasks (Markus & others, 2006; Morling & Kitayama, 2008).

In collectivistic cultures, like those of many Asian countries, achievement motivation tends to have a different focus. Instead of being oriented toward the individual, achievement orientation is more socially oriented (Bond, 1986; Kitayama & Park, 2007). For example, students in China felt that it was unacceptable to express pride for personal achievements but that it was acceptable to feel proud of achievements that benefited others (Stipek, 1998). The person strives to achieve not to promote himself or herself but to promote the status or well-being of other members of the relevant social group, such as family members (Matsumoto & Juang, 2008).

Individuals in collectivistic cultures may persevere or aspire to do well in order to fulfill the expectations of family members and to fit into the larger group. For example, the Japanese student who strives to do well academically is typically not motivated by the desire for personal recognition. Rather, the student’s behavior is more likely to be motivated by the desire to enhance the social standing of his or her family by gaining admission to a top university (Kitayama & Park, 2007).

Test your understanding of Psychological Needs as Motivators with image .