13.2 Contemporary Perspectives on Personality

Trait Theories

image
© The New Yorker Collection, 2014, William Haefeli from cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved.

13-13 How do psychologists use traits to describe personality?

trait a characteristic pattern of behavior or a disposition to feel and act, as assessed by self-report inventories and peer reports.

Rather than focusing on unconscious forces and thwarted growth opportunities, some researchers attempt to define personality in terms of stable and enduring behavior patterns, such as Lady Gaga’s openness to new experiences and her self-discipline. This perspective can be traced in part to a remarkable meeting in 1919, when Gordon Allport, a curious 22-year-old psychology student, interviewed Sigmund Freud in Vienna. Allport soon discovered just how preoccupied the founder of psychoanalysis was with finding hidden motives, even in Allport’s own behavior during the interview. That experience ultimately led Allport to do what Freud did not do—to describe personality in terms of fundamental traits—people’s characteristic behaviors and conscious motives (such as the curiosity that actually motivated Allport to see Freud). Meeting Freud, said Allport, “taught me that [psychoanalysis], for all its merits, may plunge too deep, and that psychologists would do well to give full recognition to manifest motives before probing the unconscious.” Allport came to define personality in terms of identifiable behavior patterns. He was concerned less with explaining individual traits than with describing them.

Exploring Traits

506

Classifying people as one or another distinct personality type fails to capture their full individuality. We are each a unique complex of multiple traits. So how else could we describe our personalities? We might describe an apple by placing it along several trait dimensions—relatively large or small, red or green, sweet or sour. By placing people on several trait dimensions simultaneously, psychologists can describe countless individual personality variations. (Remember from Chapter 6 that variations on just three color dimensions—hue, saturation, and brightness—create many thousands of colors.)

What trait dimensions describe personality? If you had an upcoming blind date, what personality traits might give you an accurate sense of the person? Allport and his associate H. S. Odbert (1936) counted all the words in an unabridged dictionary with which one could describe people. There were almost 18,000! How, then, could psychologists condense the list to a manageable number of basic traits?

FACTOR ANALYSIS One technique is factor analysis, a statistical procedure that has been used to identify clusters (factors) of test items that tap basic components of a trait, such as intelligence (spatial ability or verbal skill). Imagine that people who describe themselves as outgoing also tend to say that they like excitement and practical jokes and dislike quiet reading. Such a statistically correlated cluster of behaviors reflects a basic factor, or trait—in this case, extraversion.

British psychologists Hans Eysenck and Sybil Eysenck [EYE-zink] believed that we can reduce many of our normal individual variations to two or three dimensions, including extraversion-introversion and emotional stability–-instability (FIGURE 13.3). People in 35 countries around the world, from China to Uganda to Russia, have taken the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. When their answers were analyzed, the extraversion and emotionality factors inevitably emerged as basic personality dimensions (Eysenck, 1990, 1992). The Eysencks believed, and research confirms, that these factors are genetically influenced.

image
Figure 13.3: FIGURE 13.3 Two personality dimensions Mapmakers can tell us a lot by using two axes (north–south and east–west). Two primary personality factors (extraversion–introversion and stability–instability) are similarly useful as axes for describing personality variation. Varying combinations define other, more specific traits (from Eysenck & Eysenck, 1963). Those who are naturally introverted, such as primatologist Jane Goodall, may be particularly gifted in field studies. Successful politicians, including former U.S. President Bill Clinton, are often natural extraverts.
Jean-Marc Bouju/AP Photo; Andrew Innerarity/Reuters/Landov
image
Erik Lam/Shutterstock

BIOLOGY AND PERSONALITY Brain-activity scans of extraverts add to the growing list of traits and mental states now being explored with brain-imaging procedures. Such studies indicate that extraverts seek stimulation because their normal brain arousal is relatively low. For example, PET scans have shown that a frontal lobe area involved in behavior inhibition is less active in extraverts than in introverts (Johnson et al., 1999). Dopamine and dopamine-related neural activity tend to be higher in extraverts (Kim et al., 2008; Wacker et al., 2006).

507

Our biology influences our personality in other ways as well. As you may recall from the twin and adoption studies in Chapter 2, our genes have much to say about the temperament and behavioral style that help define our personality. Children’s shyness and inhibition may differ as an aspect of autonomic nervous system reactivity: Those with a reactive autonomic nervous system respond to stress with greater anxiety and inhibition (Kagan, 2010) (see Thinking Critically About: The Stigma of Introversion). The fearless, curious child may become the rock-climbing or fast-driving adult.

Personality differences among dogs (in energy, affection, reactivity, and curious intelligence) are as evident, and as consistently judged, as personality differences among humans (Gosling et al., 2003; Jones & Gosling, 2005). Monkeys, chimpanzees, orangutans, and even birds also have stable personalities (Weiss et al., 2006). Among the Great Tit (a European relative of the American chickadee), bold birds more quickly inspect new objects and explore trees (Groothuis & Carere, 2005; Verbeek et al., 1994). By selective breeding, researchers can produce bold or shy birds. Both have their place in natural history: In lean years, bold birds are more likely to find food; in abundant years, shy birds feed with less risk.

THINKING CRITICALLY ABOUT

The Stigma of Introversion

13-14 What are some common misunderstandings about introversion? Does extraversion lead to greater success than introversion?

Psychologists describe and measure personality, but they don’t advise which traits people should have. Society does this. Western cultures, for example, prize extraversion. In one study, 87 percent of people wanted to be more extraverted (Hudson & Roberts, 2014). Being introverted seems to imply that you don’t have the “right stuff” (Cain, 2012).

Just look at our superheroes. Extraverted Superman is bold and energetic. His introverted alter ego, Clark Kent, is mild-mannered and bumbling. Take-charge Elastigirl saves the day in The Incredibles. The message is clear: It’s the extraverts who are the superheroes.

TV shows also portray heartthrobs and examples of success as extraverts. Many consider Don Draper, the highly successful, attractive advertising executive in the show Mad Men to be a classic extravert. He is dominant and charismatic. Women clamor for his attention. His quiet secretary, Peggy Olson, gains respect and career advancement as the series progresses and she becomes more outspoken. Here again, extraversion equals success.

Why do we celebrate extraversion and belittle introversion? Many people may not understand what introversion really is. Introversion is not shyness. Introverted people seek low levels of stimulation from their environment because they’re sensitive. One classic study suggested that introverted people even have greater taste sensitivity. When given lemon juice, introverted people salivated more than extraverted people (Corcoran, 1964). Shy people, in contrast, remain quiet because they fear others will evaluate them negatively.

image
AMC/The Kobal Collection/Art Resource

We also tend to believe that introversion acts as a barrier to success, yet introversion actually has many benefits. As supervisors, introverts show greater receptiveness when their employees voice their ideas, challenge existing norms, and take charge. Under these circumstances, introverted leaders outperform extraverted ones (Grant et al., 2011). One striking analysis of 35 studies showed no correlation between extraversion and sales performance (Barrick et al., 2001). Many introverts prosper. Consider the American presidency, which may offer the best example of the misperception that introversion hinders career success. The top-rated U.S. president of all time was introverted. His name was Abraham Lincoln.

So, introversion should not be considered a sign of weakness. Those who need a quiet break from a loud party are not social rejects nor are they incapable of great things. They simply need a less stimulating environment in order to thrive. It’s important for extraverts to understand that not everyone feels driven by high levels of stimulation. It’s not a crime to unwind.

image
John Parrot/Stocktrek Images/Getty Images

508

RETRIEVE IT

Question

9yS2dJ1/tnp7jeU1BXqUIehJS2YOD5HPtjw9cQebCm453zefL2hp0tPsUDilIA1XTICtCMhXRNlMWqiG1aGLedSUk2p1F3fNTEn86v7B1qNu0fBhT5fUDvIU6Y8DIPk3ZHjpF+f6w/jJP4kUtfzpbh0mmNyemY4H6a1F8w+Dvq0UvkmYQ3f++hd+5oP3qT7caD8jm8R284F4fmgh
ANSWER: introversion–extraversion and emotional stability–instability

Assessing Traits

13-15 What are personality inventories, and what are their strengths and weaknesses as trait-assessment tools?

personality inventory a questionnaire (often with true-false or agree-disagree items) on which people respond to items designed to gauge a wide range of feelings and behaviors; used to assess selected personality traits.

If stable and enduring traits guide our actions, can we devise valid and reliable tests of them? Several trait-assessment techniques exist—some more valid than others. Some provide quick assessments of a single trait, such as extraversion, anxiety, or self-esteem. Personality inventories—longer questionnaires covering a wide range of feelings and behaviors—assess several traits at once.

image IMMERSIVE LEARNING Might astrology hold the secret to our personality traits? To consider this question, visit LaunchPad’s How Would You Know If Astrologers Can Describe People’s Personality?

People have had fun spoofing the MMPI with their own mock items: “Weeping brings tears to my eyes,” “Frantic screams make me nervous,” and “I stay in the bathtub until I look like a raisin” (Frankel et al., 1983).

Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) the most widely researched and clinically used of all personality tests. Originally developed to identify emotional disorders (still considered its most appropriate use), this test is now used for many other screening purposes.

empirically derived test a test (such as the MMPI) developed by testing a pool of items and then selecting those that discriminate between groups.

The classic personality inventory is the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI). Although the MMPI was originally developed to identify emotional disorders, it also assesses people’s personality traits. One of its creators, Starke Hathaway (1960), compared his effort with that of Alfred Binet. Binet, as you may recall from Chapter 9, developed the first intelligence test by selecting items that identified children who would probably have trouble progressing normally in French schools. Like Binet’s items, the MMPI items were empirically derived: From a large pool of items, Hathaway and his colleagues selected those on which particular diagnostic groups differed. “Nothing in the newspaper interests me except the comics” may seem senseless, but it just so happened that depressed people were more likely to answer True. The researchers grouped the questions into 10 clinical scales, including scales that assess depressive tendencies, masculinity–femininity, and introversion–extraversion. Today’s MMPI-2 has additional scales that assess work attitudes, family problems, and anger.

Whereas most projective tests are scored subjectively, personality inventories are scored objectively. (Software can administer and score these tests, and can also provide descriptions of people who previously responded similarly.) Objectivity does not, however, guarantee validity. Individuals taking the MMPI for employment purposes can give socially desirable answers to create a good impression. But in so doing they may also score high on a lie scale that assesses faking (as when people respond False to a universally true statement, such as “I get angry sometimes”). The objectivity of the MMPI has contributed to its popularity and to its translation into more than 100 languages.

The Big Five Factors

13-16 Which traits seem to provide the most useful information about personality variation?

Today’s trait researchers believe that simple trait factors, such as the Eysencks’ introversion–extraversion and stability–instability dimensions, are important, but they do not tell the whole story. A slightly expanded set of factors—dubbed the Big Five—does a better job (Costa & McCrae, 2011). If a test specifies where you are on the five dimensions (conscientiousness, agreeableness, neuroticism, openness, and extraversion; see TABLE 13.3), it has said much of what there is to say about your personality. Around the world—across 56 nations and 29 languages in one study (Schmitt et al., 2007)—people describe others in terms roughly consistent with this list. The Big Five may not be the last word. Some researchers report that basic personality dimensions can be described by only one or two or three factors (such as conscientiousness, agreeableness, and extraversion) (Block, 2010; De Raad et al., 2010). But for now, at least, five is the winning number in the personality lottery (Heine & Buchtel, 2009; McCrae, 2009). To find out how your personality measures up, try the brief self-assessment in FIGURE 13.4.

image
Figure 13.4: FIGURE 13.4 The Big Five self-assessment

509

image

The Big Five is currently our best approximation of the basic trait dimensions. This “common currency for personality psychology” (Funder, 2001) has been the most active personality research topic since the early 1990s, as researchers have explored these questions and more:

510

image For an 8-minute demonstration of trait research, see LaunchPad’s Video: Trait Theories of Personality below.

By exploring such questions, Big Five research has sustained trait psychology and renewed appreciation for the importance of personality. Traits matter.

RETRIEVE IT

Question

AD2Cg7tCYXE4KXoz8xYFyf83m/m3kYg59gWOoOWc2uT33EDMeZAhFhU/PSzVeLKjeWeo5EJGvDyk6m/z0H9MBwPUMy1RBIs12cGM1X0tb0zEYcv7qZ8e+tBuDTro3Bg2Yv/uqT6Qv3gJs1G7c6LBZzwRUFoYcfHgO7viiutt76I=
ANSWER: The Big Five personality factors are conscientiousness, agreeableness, neuroticism (emotional stability vs. instability), openness, and extraversion (CANOE). These factors may be objectively measured, they are relatively stable over the life span, and they apply to all cultures in which they have been studied.

Evaluating Trait Theories

13-17 Does research support the consistency of personality traits over time and across situations?

Are our personality traits stable and enduring? Or does our behavior depend on where and with whom we find ourselves? In some ways, our personality seems stable. Cheerful, friendly children tend to become cheerful, friendly adults. At a recent college reunion, I [DM] was amazed to find that my jovial former classmates were still jovial, the shy ones still shy, the happy-seeming people still smiling and laughing—50 years later. But it’s also true that a fun-loving jokester can suddenly turn serious and respectful at a job interview. And the personality traits we express can change from one situation to another. Major life events, such as becoming unemployed, can shift our personality from agreeable to slightly rude (Boyce et al., 2015).

511

“There is as much difference between us and ourselves, as between us and others.”

Michel de Montaigne, Essays, 1588

THE PERSON-SITUATION CONTROVERSY Our behavior is influenced by the interaction of our inner disposition with our environment. Still, the question lingers: Which is more important? When we explore this person-situation controversy, we look for genuine personality traits that persist over time and across situations. Are some people dependably conscientious and others unreliable? Some cheerful and others dour? Some friendly and outgoing and others shy? If we are to consider friendliness a trait, friendly people must act friendly at different times and places. Do they?

Roughly speaking, the temporary, external influences on behavior are the focus of social psychology, and the enduring, inner influences are the focus of personality psychology. In actuality, behavior always depends on the interaction of persons with situations.

In considering research that has followed lives through time, some scholars (especially those who study infants) are impressed with personality change; others are struck by personality stability during adulthood. As FIGURE 13.5 illustrates, data from 152 long-term studies reveal that personality trait scores are positively correlated with scores obtained seven years later, and that as people grow older their personality stabilizes. Interests may change—the avid tropical-fish collector may become an avid gardener. Careers may change—the determined salesperson may become a determined social worker. Relationships may change—the hostile spouse may start over with a new partner. But most people recognize their traits as their own, as Robert McCrae and Paul Costa noted (1994), “and it is well that they do. A person’s recognition of the inevitability of his or her one and only personality is . . . the culminating wisdom of a lifetime.”

image
Figure 13.5: FIGURE 13.5 Personality stability With age, personality traits become more stable, as reflected in the stronger correlation of trait scores with follow-up scores 7 years later. (Data from Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000.)

Change and consistency can co-exist. If all people were to become somewhat less shy with age, there would be personality change, but also relative stability and predictability.

So most people—including most psychologists—would probably presume the stability of personality traits. Moreover, our traits are socially significant. They influence our health, our thinking, and our job choices and performance (Deary & Matthews, 1993; Hogan, 1998; Jackson et al., 2012; Sutin et al., 2011). Studies that follow lives through time show that personality traits rival socioeconomic status and cognitive ability as predictors of mortality, divorce, and occupational attainment (Roberts et al., 2007).

Any of these tendencies, taken to an extreme, become maladaptive. Agreeableness ranges from cynical combativeness at its low extreme to gullible subservience at its high extreme. Conscientiousness ranges from irresponsible negligence to workaholic perfectionism (Widiger & Costa, 2012).

image
It’s not just personality that stabilizes with age.
© Mitra Farmand, www.fuffernutter.com

Although our personality traits may be both stable and potent, the consistency of our specific behaviors from one situation to the next is another matter. As Walter Mischel (1968, 2009) has pointed out, people do not act with predictable consistency. Mischel’s studies of college students’ conscientiousness revealed only a modest relationship between a student’s being conscientious on one occasion (say, showing up for class on time) and being similarly conscientious on another occasion (say, turning in assignments on time). If you’ve noticed how outgoing you are in some situations and how reserved you are in others, perhaps you’re not surprised.

512

This inconsistency in behaviors also makes personality test scores weak predictors of behaviors. People’s scores on an extraversion test, for example, do not neatly predict how sociable they actually will be on any given occasion. If we remember this, says Mischel, we will be more cautious about labeling and pigeonholing individuals. Years in advance, science can tell us the phase of the Moon for any given date. A day in advance, meteorologists can often predict the weather. But we are much further from being able to predict how you will feel and act tomorrow.

image
The New Yorker Collection, 2006, Victoria Roberts from cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved.

However, people’s average outgoingness, happiness, or carelessness over many situations is predictable (Epstein, 1983a,b). People who know someone well, therefore, generally agree when rating that person’s shyness or agreeableness (Jackson et al., 2015; Kenrick & Funder, 1988). The predictability of average behavior across many situations was again confirmed when researchers collected snippets of people’s daily experience via body-worn recording devices: Extraverts really do talk more (Mehl et al., 2006). (I [DM] have repeatedly vowed to cut back on my jabbering and joking during my noontime pickup basketball games with friends. Alas, moments later, the irrepressible chatterbox inevitably reoccupies my body. And I [ND] have a similar experience each time I try to stay quiet in taxis. Somehow, I always end up chatting with the driver!) As our best friends can verify, we do have genetically influenced personality traits. And those traits even lurk, report Samuel Gosling and his colleagues in a series of studies, in our

image
Room with a cue Even at “zero acquaintance,” people can catch a glimpse of others’ personality from looking at their website, bedroom, or office. So, what’s your read on this person’s office?
Karan Kapoor/Stone/Getty Images

In unfamiliar, formal situations—perhaps as a guest in the home of a person from another culture—our traits remain hidden as we carefully attend to social cues. In familiar, informal situations—just hanging out with friends—we feel less constrained, allowing our traits to emerge (Buss, 1989). In these informal situations, our expressive styles—our animation, manner of speaking, and gestures—are impressively consistent. Viewing “thin slices” of someone’s behavior—such as seeing a photo for a mere fraction of a second or seeing three, 2-second clips of a teacher in action—can tell us a lot about the person’s basic personality traits (Ambady, 2010; Rule et al., 2009).

To sum up, we can say that at any moment the immediate situation powerfully influences a person’s behavior. Social psychologists have learned that this is especially so when a “strong situation” makes clear demands (Cooper & Withey, 2009). We can better predict drivers’ behavior at traffic lights from knowing the color of the lights than from knowing the drivers’ personalities. Averaging our behavior across many occasions does, however, reveal distinct personality traits. Traits exist. We differ. And our differences matter.

513

RETRIEVE IT

Question

J7jum2GtywAhRKqVLHRpO8RFfjhalLj+Dhb8B8xp5G/p0ywQlNvBWOBZBK5KDNPHMCsiVQFLimVn400B2rJC7ntAwt8cW/BLUUXWwU9YWEymmyqvKn+wbXffPP0FmZ7ZUIFheZ2tu/ODDfNTkzAbrw==
ANSWER: Our scores on personality tests predict our average behavior across many situations much better than they predict our specific behavior in any given situation.

Social-Cognitive Theories

13-18 How do social-cognitive theorists view personality development, and how do they explore behavior?

social-cognitive perspective views behavior as influenced by the interaction between people’s traits (including their thinking) and their social context.

The social-cognitive perspective on personality, proposed by Albert Bandura (1986, 2006, 2008), emphasizes the interaction of our traits with our situations. Much as nature and nurture always work together, so do individuals and their situations.

Social-cognitive theorists believe we learn many of our behaviors either through conditioning or by observing and imitating others. (That’s the “social” part.) They also emphasize the importance of mental processes: What we think about a situation affects our behavior in that situation. (That’s the “cognitive” part.) Instead of focusing solely on how our environment controls us (behaviorism), social-cognitive theorists focus on how we and our environment interact: How do we interpret and respond to external events? How do our schemas, our memories, and our expectations influence our behavior patterns?

Reciprocal Influences

reciprocal determinism the interacting influences of behavior, internal cognition, and environment.

Bandura (1986, 2006) views the person-environment interaction as reciprocal determinism. “Behavior, internal personal factors, and environmental influences,” he said, “all operate as interlocking determinants of each other” (FIGURE 13.6). We can see this interaction in people’s relationships. For example, Rosa’s romantic history (past behavior) influences her attitudes toward new relationships (internal factor), which affects how she now responds to Ryan (environmental factor).

image
Figure 13.6: FIGURE 13.6 Reciprocal determinism
Courtesy of Joslyn Brugh

Consider three specific ways in which individuals and environments interact:

  1. Different people choose different environments. The schools we attend, the reading we do, the movies we watch, the music we listen to, the friends we associate with—all are part of an environment we have chosen, based partly on our dispositions (Funder, 2009; Ickes et al., 1997). We choose our environment and it then shapes us.

  2. Our personalities shape how we interpret and react to events. Anxious people tend to attend and react strongly to relationship threats (Campbell & Marshall, 2011). If we perceive the world as threatening, we will watch for threats and be prepared to defend ourselves.

  3. Our personalities help create situations to which we react. How we view and treat people influences how they then treat us. If we expect that others will not like us, our desperate attempts to seek their approval might cause them to reject us. Depressed people often engage in this excessive reassurance seeking, which may confirm their negative self-views (Coyne, 1976a,b).

514

In addition to the interaction of internal personal factors, the environment, and our behaviors, we also experience gene-environment interaction. Our genetically influenced traits evoke certain responses from others, which may nudge us in one direction or another. In one classic study, those with the interacting factors of (1) having a specific gene associated with aggression and (2) being raised in a difficult environment were most likely to demonstrate adult antisocial behavior (Caspi et al., 2002).

In such ways, we are both the products and the architects of our environments: Behavior emerges from the interplay of external and internal influences. Boiling water turns an egg hard and a potato soft. A threatening environment turns one person into a hero, another into a scoundrel. Extraverts enjoy greater well-being in an extraverted culture than in an introverted one (Fulmer et al., 2010). At every moment, our behavior is influenced by our biology, our social and cultural experiences, and our cognition and dispositions (FIGURE 13.7).

image
Figure 13.7: FIGURE 13.7 The biopsychosocial approach to the study of personality As with other psychological phenomena, personality is fruitfully studied at multiple levels.

RETRIEVE IT

Question

Albert Bandura proposed the kcxsDC5VTGkSiECZ - XkPMXfT7QJJq66nMlxyQ1A== perspective on personality, which emphasizes the interaction of people with their environment. To describe the interacting influences of behavior, thoughts, and environment, he used the term u80iYVqKe01+Bc2nydwvtOUabJtS4y8ectmDiQ== .

image
If you can't stand the heat. . . On the Food Network's Chopped, contestants are pitted against one another in stressful situations. The entertaining episodes illustrate a valid point: A chef's behavior in such job-relevant situations is a valid assessment of that person's ability and can help predict job performance.
Gustavo Caballero/Getty Images

Assessing Behavior in Situations

To predict behavior, social-cognitive psychologists often observe behavior in realistic situations. One ambitious example was the U.S. Army’s World War II strategy for assessing candidates for spy missions. Rather than using paper-and-pencil tests, Army psychologists subjected the candidates to simulated undercover conditions. They tested their ability to handle stress, solve problems, maintain leadership, and withstand intense interrogation without blowing their cover. Although time-consuming and expensive, this assessment of behavior in a realistic situation helped predict later success on actual spy missions (OSS Assessment Staff, 1948).

Military and educational organizations and many Fortune 500 companies have adopted assessment center strategies (Bray et al., 1991, 1997; Eurich et al., 2009). AT&T has observed prospective managers doing simulated managerial work. Many colleges assess students’ potential via internships and student teaching, and assess potential faculty members’ teaching abilities by observing them teach. Most American cities with populations of 50,000 or more have used assessment centers in evaluating police officers and firefighters (Lowry, 1997).

515

Assessment center exercises have some limitations. They are more revealing of visible dimensions, such as communication ability, than of others, such as inner achievement drive (Bowler & Woehr, 2006). Nevertheless, these procedures exploit a valid principle: The best means of predicting future behavior is neither a personality test nor an interviewer’s intuition; rather, it is the person’s past behavior patterns in similar situations (Lyons et al., 2011; Mischel, 1981; Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). As long as the situation and the person remain much the same, the best predictor of future job performance is past job performance; the best predictor of future grades is past grades; the best predictor of future aggressiveness is past aggressiveness. If you can’t check the person’s past behavior, the next best thing is to create an assessment situation that simulates the task so you can see how the person handles it (Lievens et al., 2009; Meriac et al., 2008).

“What’s past is prologue.”

William Shakespeare, The Tempest, 1611

Evaluating Social-Cognitive Theories

13-19 What criticisms have social-cognitive theorists faced?

Social-cognitive theories of personality sensitize researchers to how situations affect, and are affected by, individuals. More than other personality theories (see TABLE 13.4), they build from psychological research on learning and cognition.

Table 13.3: TABLE 13.4
Comparing the Major Personality Theories
Personality Theory Key Proponents Assumptions View of Personality Personality Assessment Methods
Psychoanalytic Freud Emotional disorders spring from unconscious dynamics, such as unresolved sexual and other childhood conflicts, and fixation at various developmental stages. Defense mechanisms fend off anxiety. Personality consists of pleasure-seeking impulses (the id), a reality-oriented executive (the ego), and an internalized set of ideals (the superego). Free association, projective tests, dream analysis
Psychodynamic Adler, Horney, Jung The unconscious and conscious minds interact. Childhood experiences and defense mechanisms are important. The dynamic interplay of conscious and unconscious motives and conflicts shape our personality. Projective tests, therapy sessions
Humanistic Rogers, Maslow Rather than examining the struggles of sick people, it’s better to focus on the ways healthy people strive for self-realization. If our basic human needs are met, people will strive toward self-actualization. In a climate of unconditional positive regard, we can develop self-awareness and a more realistic and positive self-concept. Questionnaires, therapy sessions
Trait Allport, Eysenck, McCrae, Costa We have certain stable and enduring characteristics, influenced by genetic predispositions. Scientific study of traits has isolated important dimensions of personality, such as the Big Five traits (conscientiousness, agreeableness, neuroticism, openness, and extraversion). Personality inventories
Social-Cognitive Bandura Our traits and the social context interact to produce our behaviors. Conditioning and observational learning interact with cognition to create behavior patterns. Our behavior in one situation is best predicted by considering our past behavior in similar situations.

516

Critics charge that social-cognitive theories focus so much on the situation that they fail to appreciate the person’s inner traits. Where is the person in this view of personality, ask the dissenters, and where are human emotions? True, the situation does guide our behavior. But, say the critics, in many instances our unconscious motives, our emotions, and our pervasive traits shine through. Personality traits have been shown to predict behavior at work, love, and play. Our biologically influenced traits really do matter. Consider Percy Ray Pridgen and Charles Gill. Each faced the same situation: They had jointly won a $90 million lottery jackpot (Harriston, 1993). When Pridgen learned of the winning numbers, he began trembling uncontrollably, huddled with a friend behind a bathroom door while confirming the win, then sobbed. When Gill heard the news, he told his wife and then went to sleep.

RETRIEVE IT

Question

y/QME4gfl2r+Nk0Df2o9N6Efpp8Wk8p4NCsEnluPoFRbS44DTbSJxUZlNrRpuUT+1vLgZuWNap0Xo+qDEiQnBwlyOmrGEd4XopuzvbE+BYyi7QoUlj2J3sJhRpRUp1E3nvpn8IB/SFQyJ/Tl
ANSWER: examine the person's past behavior patterns in similar situations

Exploring the Self

13-20 Why has psychology generated so much research on the self? How important is self-esteem to our well-being?

self in contemporary psychology, assumed to be the center of personality, the organizer of our thoughts, feelings, and actions.

Psychology’s concern with people’s sense of self dates back at least to William James, who devoted more than 100 pages of his 1890 Principles of Psychology to the topic. By 1943, Gordon Allport lamented that the self had become “lost to view.” Although humanistic psychology’s later emphasis on the self did not instigate much scientific research, it did help renew the concept of self and keep it alive. Now, more than a century after James, the self is one of Western psychology’s most vigorously researched topics. Every year, new studies galore appear on self-esteem, self-disclosure, self-awareness, self-schemas, self-monitoring, and more. Even neuroscientists have searched for the self, by identifying a central frontal lobe region that activates when people respond to self-reflective questions about their traits and dispositions (Damasio, 2010; Mitchell, 2009; Pauly et al., 2013). Underlying all this research is an assumption that the self, as organizer of our thoughts, feelings, and actions, is the center of personality.

“The first step to better times is to imagine them.”

Chinese fortune cookie

One example of thinking about self is the concept of possible selves (Cross & Markus, 1991; Markus & Nurius, 1986). Your possible selves include your visions of the self you dream of becoming—the rich self, the successful self, the loved and admired self. Your possible selves also include the self you fear becoming—the unemployed self, the academically failed self, the lonely and unpopular self. Possible selves motivate us to lay out specific goals that direct our energy effectively and efficiently (Landau et al, 2014). High school students enrolled in a gifted program for math and science were more likely to become scientists if they had a clear vision of themselves as successful scientists (Buday et al., 2012). Dreams do often give birth to achievements.

spotlight effect overestimating others’ noticing and evaluating our appearance, performance, and blunders (as if we presume a spotlight shines on us).

Our self-focused perspective may motivate us, but it can also lead us to presume too readily that others are noticing and evaluating us. Most of them aren’t. Thomas Gilovich has demonstrated this spotlight effect. He and his colleagues found that fewer people than we presume actually notice our dorky clothes, bad hair, nervousness, or irritation (Gilovich & Savitsky, 1999). Others are also less aware than we suppose of the variability—the ups and downs—of our appearance and performance (Gilovich et al., 2002). Even after a blunder (setting off a library alarm, showing up in the wrong clothes), we stick out like a sore thumb less than we imagine (Savitsky et al., 2001). To turn down the brightness of the spotlight, we can use two strategies. The first is simply knowing about the spotlight effect. Public speakers perform better if they understand that their natural nervousness is not obvious (Savitsky & Gilovich, 2003). The second is to take the audience’s perspective. When we imagine audience members empathizing with our situation, we tend to expect we will not be judged as harshly (Epley et al., 2002).

517

The Benefits of Self-Esteem

self-esteem one’s feelings of high or low self-worth.

self-efficacy one’s sense of competence and effectiveness.

Self-esteem—our feelings of high or low self-worth—is important. So also is self-efficacy, our sense of competence on a task. A person with high self-esteem will strongly agree with self-affirming questionnaire statements such as, “I am fun to be with,” or “I have good ideas.” A person with low self-esteem responds to these statements with qualifying adjectives, such as somewhat or sometimes.

High self-esteem pays dividends. People who feel good about themselves have fewer sleepless nights. They are less likely to give in to pressures to conform. They make more positive Facebook posts, causing others to like them more (Forest & Wood, 2012). They are more persistent at difficult tasks, and they are less shy, anxious, and lonely. They try harder to shake their bad moods because they think they deserve better (Wood et al., 2009). And they are more successful and just plain happier (Greenberg, 2008; Orth & Robins, 2014).

But is high self-esteem the horse or the cart? Is it really “the armor that protects kids” from life’s problems (McKay, 2000)? Some psychologists have had their doubts (Baumeister, 2006, 2015; Dawes, 1994; Leary, 1999; Seligman, 1994, 2002). Children’s academic self-efficacy—their confidence that they can do well in a subject—predicts school achievement, but their general self-image does not (Marsh & Craven, 2006; Swann et al., 2007; Trautwein et al., 2006). Maybe self-esteem simply reflects reality. Maybe it’s a side effect of meeting challenges and surmounting difficulties. Maybe self-esteem is a gauge that reads out the state of our relationships with others. If so, isn’t pushing the gauge artificially higher with empty compliments much like forcing a car’s low fuel gauge to display “full”?

If feeling good follows doing well, then giving praise in the absence of good performance may actually harm people. After receiving weekly self-esteem-boosting messages, struggling students earned lower-than-expected grades (Forsyth et al., 2007). Other research showed that giving people random rewards hurt their productivity. Martin Seligman (2012) reported that “when good things occurred that weren’t earned, like nickels coming out of slot machines, it did not increase people’s well-being. It produced helplessness. People gave up and became passive.”

“When kids increase in self-control, their grades go up later. But when kids increase their self-esteem, there is no effect on their grades.”

Angela Duckworth, In Character interview, 2009

Experiments have revealed an effect of low self-esteem. When researchers temporarily deflated participants’ self-image (by telling them they did poorly on an aptitude test or by disparaging their personality), those participants became more likely to disparage others or to express heightened racial prejudice (vanDellen et al., 2011; van Dijk et al., 2011; Ybarra, 1999). Self-image threat even increases unconscious racial bias (Allen & Sherman, 2011). In other studies, people who were negative about themselves also tended to be oversensitive and judgmental (Baumgardner et al., 1989; Pelham, 1993). Self-esteem threats also lead people to spend more time with their online profiles—safe havens in which to rebuild their self-worth (Toma & Hancock, 2013). Such findings are consistent with humanistic psychology’s presumption that a healthy self-image is essential. Accept yourself and you’ll find it easier to accept others. Disparage yourself and you will be prone to the floccinaucinihilipilification1 of others. Said more simply, people who are down on themselves tend to be down on others. Some people “love their neighbors as themselves”; others loathe their neighbors as themselves.

image

518

Self-Serving Bias

13-21 What evidence reveals self-serving bias, and how do defensive and secure self-esteem differ?

Imagine dashing to class, hoping not to miss the first few minutes. But you arrive five minutes late, huffing and puffing. As you sink into your seat, what sorts of thoughts go through your mind? Do you go through a negative door, with thoughts such as, “I hate myself” and “I’m a loser”? Or do you go through a positive door, saying to yourself, “At least I made it to class” and “I really tried to get here on time”?

self-serving bias a readiness to perceive oneself favorably.

Personality psychologists have found that most people choose the second door, which leads to positive self-thoughts. We have a good reputation with ourselves. We show a self-serving bias—a readiness to perceive ourselves favorably (Myers, 2010). Consider:

People accept more responsibility for good deeds than for bad, and for successes than for failures. Athletes often privately credit their victories to their own prowess, and their losses to bad breaks, lousy officiating, or the other team’s exceptional performance. Most students who receive poor grades on an exam criticize the test, not themselves. Drivers filling out insurance forms explain their accidents in such words as “As I reached an intersection, a hedge sprang up, obscuring my vision, and I did not see the other car” and “A pedestrian hit me and went under my car.” The question “What have I done to deserve this?” is one we usually ask of our troubles, not our successes. Although a self-serving bias can lead us to avoid uncomfortable truths, it can also motivate us to approach difficult tasks with confidence instead of despair (Tomaka et al., 1992; von Hippel & Trivers, 2011).

Most people see themselves as better than average. Compared with most other people, how nice are you? How appealing are you as a friend or romantic partner? Where would you rank yourself from the 1st to the 99th percentile? Most people put themselves well above the 50th percentile. This better-than-average effect appears for nearly any subjectively assessed and socially desirable behavior. Some examples:

519

image
Blindness to one's own incompetence Ironically, people often are most overconfident when most incompetent. That, say Justin Kruger and David Dunning (1999), is because it often takes competence to recognize competence. Our ignorance of what we don't know sustains our self-confidence, leading us to make the same mistakes (Williams et al., 2013).
Universal Press Syndicate

The self-serving bias reflects an overestimation of ourselves as well as a desire to maintain a positive self-view (Brown, 2012; Epley & Dunning, 2000). This phenomenon is less striking in Asia, where people value modesty (Falk et al., 2009; Heine & Hamamura, 2007). Yet self-serving biases have been observed worldwide: In every one of 53 countries surveyed, people expressed self-esteem above the midpoint of the most widely used scale (Schmitt & Allik, 2005).

image
United Features Syndicate, Inc.

Ironically, people even see themselves as more immune than others to self-serving bias (Pronin, 2007). That’s right, people believe they are above average at not believing they are above average. (Isn’t psychology fun?) The world, it seems, is Garrison Keillor’s Lake Wobegon writ large—a place where “all the women are strong, all the men are good-looking, and all the children are above average.”

“If you are like most people, then like most people, you don’t know you’re like most people. Science has given us a lot of facts about the average person, and one of the most reliable of these facts is the average person doesn’t see herself as average.”

Daniel Gilbert, Stumbling on Happiness, 2006

Finding their self-esteem threatened, people with large egos may react violently. Researchers Brad Bushman and Roy Baumeister (1998; Bushman et al., 2009) had undergraduate volunteers write a brief essay, in response to which another supposed student gave them either praise (“Great essay!”) or stinging criticism (“One of the worst essays I have read!”). Then the essay writers played a reaction-time game against the other student. After wins, they could assault their opponent with noise of any intensity for any duration.

Can you anticipate the result? After criticism, those with inflated self-esteem were “exceptionally aggressive.” They delivered three times the auditory torture of those with normal self-esteem. “Encouraging people to feel good about themselves when they haven’t earned it” poses problems, Baumeister (2001) concluded. “Conceited, self-important individuals turn nasty toward those who puncture their bubbles of self-love.”

Are self-serving perceptions on the rise in North America? Some researchers believe they are. From 1980 to 2007, popular song lyrics became more self-focused (DeWall et al., 2011). An analysis of 766,513 American books published between 1960 and 2008 showed a similar result: Self-focused words increased (Twenge et al., 2013). Surveys of over 9 million high school seniors and entering college students between 1966 and 2009 also found increasing interest in gaining money, fame, and prestige, and decreasing concern for others (Twenge et al., 2012).

narcissism excessive self-love and self-absorption.

Psychologist Jean Twenge has reported that narcissism—excessive self-love and self-absorption—has also been rising (2006; Twenge & Foster, 2010). After tracking self-importance across the last several decades, Twenge found that what she calls Generation Me (born in the 1980s and 1990s) is expressing more narcissism by agreeing more often with statements such as, “If I ruled the world, it would be a better place,” or “I think I am a special person.” What gives birth to narcissism? One ingredient is parents who tell their kids they are superior to others (Brummelman et al., 2015). Why does a rise in narcissism matter? Narcissists (more often men [Grijalva et al. 2015]) tend to be materialistic, desire fame, have inflated expectations, hook up more often without commitment, and gamble and cheat more—all of which have been increasing as narcissism has increased.

520

Some critics of the concept of self-serving bias claim that it overlooks those who feel worthless and unlovable: If self-serving bias prevails, why do so many people disparage themselves? For four reasons: (1) Self-directed put-downs can be subtly strategic—they elicit reassuring strokes. Saying “No one likes me” may at least elicit “But not everyone has met you!” (2) Before an important event, such as a game or an exam, self-disparaging comments prepare us for possible failure. The coach who extols the superior strength of the upcoming opponent makes a loss understandable, a victory noteworthy. (3) A self-disparaging “How could I have been so stupid!” can help us learn from our mistakes. (4) Self-disparagement frequently pertains to one’s old self. Asked to remember their really bad behaviors, people recall things from long ago; good behaviors more easily come to mind from their recent past (Escobedo & Adolphs, 2010). People are much more critical of their distant past selves than of their current selves—even when they have not changed (Wilson & Ross, 2001). “At 18, I was a jerk; today I’m more sensitive.” In their own eyes, chumps yesterday, champs today.

image
Shannon Wheeler

Even so, it’s true: All of us some of the time, and some of us much of the time, do feel inferior—especially when we compare ourselves with those who are a step or two higher on the ladder of status, looks, income, or ability. For example, Olympians who win silver medals, barely missing gold, show greater sadness on the awards podium compared with the bronze medal winners (Medvec et al., 1995). The deeper and more frequently we have such feelings, the more unhappy, even depressed, we are. But for most people, thinking has a naturally positive bias.

“The [self-]portraits that we actually believe, when we are given freedom to voice them, are dramatically more positive than reality can sustain.”

Shelley Taylor, Positive Illusions, 1989

While recognizing the dark side of self-serving bias and self-esteem, some researchers prefer isolating the effects of two types of self-esteem—defensive and secure (Kernis, 2003; Lambird & Mann, 2006; Ryan & Deci, 2004). Defensive self-esteem is fragile. It focuses on sustaining itself, which makes failure and criticism feel threatening. Perceived threats feed anger and feelings of vulnerability (Crocker & Park, 2004).

“The enthusiastic claims of the self-esteem movement mostly range from fantasy to hogwash. The effects of self-esteem are small, limited, and not all good.”

Roy Baumeister (1996)

Secure self-esteem is less fragile, because it is less contingent on external evaluations. Feeling accepted for who we are, and not for our looks, wealth, or acclaim, relieves pressures to succeed and enables us to focus beyond ourselves. By losing ourselves in relationships and purposes larger than self, we may achieve a more secure self-esteem, satisfying relationships, and greater quality of life (Crocker & Park, 2004). Authentic pride, rooted in actual achievement, supports self-confidence and leadership (Tracy et al., 2009; Weidman et al., in press; Williams & DeSteno, 2009).

“If you compare yourself with others, you may become vain and bitter; for always there will be greater and lesser persons than yourself.”

Max Ehrmann, “Desiderata,” 1927

RETRIEVE IT

Question

AXfrspfaB28Wszv7wl9XkepQJF4p99Wr8xNp7kwg+MOGRQuS8pR0Pj8lPYwLMNH8grPzlnLueVTI8J4PVsFIKbirtEOi32diWcEFVemdLYwCL0d5qJZBy2wJN/HWh6cspwhSiqwrXkjNknJD+3mu79Zi2hIG7SAbCnm8xmOYquGlXbqdPIlRmS23zCRC9CvGXOREn3A29EPuPDpB
ANSWER: People who feel confident in their abilities are often happier, have greater motivation, and are less susceptible to depression. Inflated self-esteem can lead to self-serving bias, greater aggression, and narcissism.

Question

The tendency to accept responsibility for success and blame circumstances or bad luck for failure is called
So0cdW4ARXdUQ6hCNceYCnY/HVtKUTFF .

Question

t0LbW7nWXGtZWyzS/rkYDw== (Secure/Defensive) self-esteem correlates with more anger and greater feelings of vulnerability. dDlP/fKTyrYuJkUF (Secure/Defensive) self-esteem is a healthier self-image that allows us to focus beyond ourselves and enjoy a higher quality of life.

521

Culture and the Self

13-22 How do individualist and collectivist cultures differ in their values and goals?

Our consideration of personality—of people’s characteristic ways of thinking, feeling, and acting—concludes with a look at cultural variations. Imagine that someone ripped away your social connections, making you a solitary refugee in a foreign land. How much of your identity would remain intact?

individualism giving priority to one’s own goals over group goals and defining one’s identity in terms of personal attributes rather than group identifications.

If you are an individualist, a great deal of your identity would survive. You would have an independent sense of “me,” and an awareness of your unique personal convictions and values. Individualists give higher priority to personal goals. They define their identity mostly in terms of personal traits. They strive for personal control and individual achievement.

Although people within cultures vary, different cultures emphasize either individualism or collectivism (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Individualism is valued in most areas of North America, Western Europe, Australia, and New Zealand. The United States is mostly an individualist culture. Founded by settlers who wanted to differentiate themselves from others, Americans have cherished the “pioneer” spirit (Kitayama et al., 2010). Some 85 percent of Americans say it is possible to “pretty much be who you want to be” (Sampson, 2000).

Individualists share the human need to belong. They join groups. But they are less focused on group harmony and doing their duty to the group (Brewer & Chen, 2007). Being more self-contained, individualists move in and out of social groups more easily. They feel relatively free to switch places of worship, change jobs, or even leave their extended families and migrate to a new place. Marriage is often for as long as they both shall love.

image

Individualists even prefer unusual names, as Jean Twenge noticed while seeking a name for her first child. Over time, the most common American names listed by year on the U.S. Social Security baby names website were becoming less desirable. An analysis of the first names of 325 million American babies born between 1880 and 2007 confirmed this trend (Twenge et al., 2010). As FIGURE 13.8 illustrates, the percentage of boys and girls given one of the 10 most common names for their birth year has plunged, especially in recent years. Even within the United States, parents from more recently settled states (for example, Utah and Arizona) give their children more distinct names compared with parents who live in more established states (for example, New York and Massachusetts) (Varnum & Kitayama, 2011).

image
Figure 13.8: FIGURE 13.8 A child like no other Americans’ individualist tendencies are reflected in their choice of names for their babies. In recent years, the percentage of American babies receiving one of that year’s 10 most common names has plunged. (Data from Twenge et al., 2010.)

collectivism giving priority to the goals of one’s group (often one’s extended family or work group) and defining one’s identity accordingly.

If set adrift in a foreign land as a collectivist, you might experience a greater loss of identity. Cut off from family, groups, and loyal friends, you would lose the connections that have defined who you are. Group identifications provide a sense of belonging, a set of values, and an assurance of security in collectivist cultures. In return, collectivists have deeper, more stable attachments to their groups—their family, clan, or company. Elders receive great respect. In some collectivist cultures, disrespecting family elders violates the law. The Law of the People’s Republic of China on Protection of the Rights and Interests of the Elderly states that parents aged 60 or above can sue their sons and daughters if they fail to provide “for the elderly, taking care of them and comforting them, and cater[ing] to their special needs.”

522

image
Considerate collectivists Japan’s collectivist values, including duty to others and social harmony, were on display after the devastating 2011 earthquake and tsunami. Virtually no looting was reported, and residents remained calm and orderly, as shown here while waiting for drinking water.
Kyodo/Reuters/Landov
image
Collectivist culture Although the United States is largely individualist, many cultural subgroups remain collectivist. This is true for Alaska Natives, who demonstrate respect for tribal elders, and whose identity springs largely from their group affiliations.
Sam Harrel/ZUMAPress/Newscom

Collectivists are like athletes who take more pleasure in their team’s victory than in their own performance. They find satisfaction in advancing their groups’ interests, even at the expense of personal needs. Preserving group spirit and avoiding social embarrassment are important goals. Collectivists therefore avoid direct confrontation, blunt honesty, and uncomfortable topics. They value humility, not self-importance (Bond et al., 2012). Instead of dominating conversations, collectivists hold back and display shyness when meeting strangers (Cheek & Melchoir, 1990). When the priority is “we,” not “me,” that individualized latte—“decaf, single shot, skinny, extra hot”—that feels so good to a North American might sound selfishly demanding in Seoul (Kim & Markus, 1999).

“One needs to cultivate the spirit of sacrificing the little me to achieve the benefits of the big me.”

Chinese saying

Within many countries, there are also distinct subcultures related to one’s religion, economic status, and region (Cohen, 2009). In China, greater collectivist thinking occurs in provinces that produce large amounts of rice, a difficult-to-grow crop that often involves cooperation between groups of people (Talhelm et al., 2014). In collectivist Japan, a spirit of individualism marks the “northern frontier” island of Hokkaido (Kitayama et al., 2006). And even in the most individualist countries, some people have collectivist values. But in general, people (especially men) in competitive, individualist cultures have more personal freedom, are less geographically bound to their families, enjoy more privacy, and take more pride in personal achievements (TABLE 13.5).

523

image

Sources: Information from Thomas Schoeneman (1994) and Harry Triandis (1994).

Individualism’s benefits may come at a cost. There has been more loneliness, divorce, homicide, and stress-related disease in individualist cultures (Popenoe, 1993; Triandis et al., 1988). Demands for more romance and personal fulfillment in marriage can subject relationships to more pressure (Dion & Dion, 1993). In one survey, “keeping romance alive” was rated as important to a good marriage by 78 percent of U.S. women but only 29 percent of Japanese women (American Enterprise, 1992). In China, love songs have often expressed enduring commitment and friendship (Rothbaum & Tsang, 1998): “We will be together from now on . . . I will never change from now to forever.”

What predicts change in one culture over time, or between differing cultures? Social history matters. In Western cultures, individualism and independence have been fostered by voluntary emigration, a capitalist economy, and a sparsely populated, challenging environment (Kitayama et al., 2009, 2010; Varnum et al., 2010). Might biology also play a role? In search of biological underpinnings to such cultural differences, a new subfield, cultural neuroscience, is studying how neurobiology and cultural traits influence each other (Chiao et al., 2013). One study compared collectivists’ and individualists’ brain activity when viewing other people in distress. The brain scans suggested that collectivists experienced greater emotional pain when exposed to others’ distress (Cheon et al., 2011). As we have seen in personality and beyond, we are biopsychosocial creatures.

RETRIEVE IT

Question

dLE6tQjlAvjsjZ4K77NZG29Gv9HO46X1mApgzpmRleKQBXwjqLkpB6iX0bPfxCVht8JPPIGK33aL04DrqrJ7EyIbLX68yFP72C8t+7vPXzh1KA2NvBFfYH48FVhkR2U2b4VXoQ==
ANSWER: Individualists give priority to personal goals over group goals and tend to define their identity in terms of their own personal attributes. Collectivists give priority to group goals over individual goals and tend to define their identity in terms of group identifications.

524

REVIEW Contemporary Perspectives on Personality

Learning Objectives

Test Yourself by taking a moment to answer each of these Learning Objective Questions (repeated here from within the chapter). Research suggests that trying to answer these questions on your own will improve your long-term memory of the concepts (McDaniel et al., 2009).

Question

6XpHPyCgS27BnbDXfdNKEtOJCAw5nXdai8fFzCkEWuzrOhqF41aHZUZOGMl2Qr/JXHv8aXmb+eVjnO2vpN9Wv4B41x+T4Q0K3nPc+JiUDYOZT5oAOdJYB1JVWJ1YZlEj6uM0HbBSLY2owsqgUtUvzv/ONr/mBsSjVCSwDnRfM1S7emtOa8279LQA3J9T8MWV6BKqyeWC5sQ+hh7kEKRP+irgSW/tRUFRbqPCLA==
ANSWER: Trait theorists see personality as a stable and enduring pattern of behavior. They describe our differences rather than trying to explain them. Using factor analysis, they identify clusters of behavior tendencies that occur together. Genetic predispositions influence many traits.

Question

OigW5i8SlmASXMklPzpssFvRIbcraZObgEc/50Zdntpq7ZCHf/fuBe1xzrPvzMlAcLDsa8KeakLd5ph9xmS9zlUhuoHl63K5Pm7tEtyHlpw3n8Iy22ysniIRzxpLVqn5wCW1IF8jO6ujt9qSaZ7/ybog6nZtLoEMlS5gvS06bFBcFW5VeoXYajvVM4pG9qotLy+IKA5Be/dyWXuSJ1l97Ofl39AtADjiKu0Kqg0DpHOXFeZyW+2iswbkiDSt50YdsWrFS2dytmbD1a48DxPQ3+zlnsjb8TIdEvBzfMGts+yg6+9o0VVSa3ZWyRs=
ANSWER: Introversion is often misunderstood as shyness, but introverted people often simply seek low levels of stimulation from their environment. Introversion is also sometimes thought to be a barrier to success, but in fact introverts often experience great achievement, even in sales.

Question

ABbtdCvsHoyGmPn97jvGejWfDpyxCxZ7u7Mob/VEkb98ey0xhObTi9BY2jygaq/5Yd6Bp9f8R+CYxmGEDD/sWEBnHCZRvt6YDrKueeWGqta4qhWSoJpGNerMjJK7GbpYRzOZ233yrDzuiCzI3Kt7+LZlAoCMLSFy7pB04z5cPo+LQyiNhw0+du0N1TNRN481OcBj+Ch+Y4HBT4J5FIEb1Mw0DjiiX8YP3QdsER1X043Tt+BEAnMbdiPCAzq0zofsBE04JT4UUhczFVv+m93ageIl1eEcv2gRnDXxn5T/SrqDMsSwaL9z0hHe7trXkczjDEJqZfb4BeZnlaYveCv8Z5L52pCuRFv+1xu4QQ==
ANSWER: Personality inventories (such as the MMPI) are questionnaires on which people respond to items designed to gauge a wide range of feelings and behaviors. Test items are empirically derived, and the tests are objectively scored. But people can fake their answers to create a good impression, and the ease of computerized testing may lead to misuse of the tests.

Question

NkIf8MnfhqBuRRSCBuxvPnqldsq1qIoSVqnYTiyoKk/uDbvdJg6icqn/DK9CWIV4eH1rN6qCNnDHUQXbq6XOanfUCjbNOUCts6OShFqMsgPnprsNfuZRcbHzT61OtaLD/9KBkhT1etv1aDDijrnJpnTo9FHzbxENl5iKCvZh36EPTH8xF5VygORYGlBj+DMUROfdfNtlGf4Zq0qw/9bWMGvJb+Tnak4t+NWE95UmVDziLUkGCSiboiCO/1k0KuyVfPwylt3Xj3OtRUQp
ANSWER: The Big Five personality factors—conscientiousness, agreeableness, neuroticism, openness, and extraversion (CANOE)—currently offer the clearest picture of personality. These factors are quite stable and appear to be found in all cultures.

Question

L8OwVYOUz4NQl+oguekGdjFoRlxfF8nuUrrriEnQZAIg+P6265M7lNd0P4Hwl47iNVhniCJfcrBM5V87E1K/b9UiQhU0GnuXtSPjYig4e2oUUcCtLa2sygzXXip3zO2oykmkwVhhfniC2j1K9O3najn11iighwi/n2NxYGVvMWfsGIN/4vayNivt+7bXvhdBB9mc5T1Oy5SapYn0wdNuIwXnhW/XOJ55oKMDktY/JKkDv+sSSEgVRz811unYnenRTQsVkGOblqhDnLZw+UDALQ==
ANSWER: A person's average traits persist over time and are predictable over many different situations. But traits cannot predict behavior in any one particular situation.

Question

QKz/EI+7f8lC7JMYfVC1uk4lQBjfl7FEE5Ox2fYxL+vbqjq2RlIPw4XL+hZND0FtdUHW64cNQOAxwW60HfGfJKIXKMw6M58vgcuWgfMcwL4IKLT02xBC+G0/kX1sweb+RF4Bsyk6S2P4HXttlCDwNgxz5gnUwLIK95h2dw0qMvpe+75mYNugt3PeMS/ijqwYlZRt76PVlwRg3oW5lvj65SC0mxAubkL/nR9VnaoeTEwZp3SNYX5oImzGKdra2OLSkyvJZ3xiUtPe2uknaMnem/WiGkbEH6ye80zKJQCgmx+QAvZOD+JOpecaxWjdSocoyYPoNxMYXmuLcP1B4QHoZZeuQSk=
ANSWER: Albert Bandura first proposed the social-cognitive perspective, which emphasizes the interaction of our traits with our situations. Social-cognitive researchers apply principles of learning, cognition, and social behavior to personality. Reciprocal determinism is a term describing the interaction and mutual influence of behavior, internal personal factors, and environmental factors.

Question

V1nm+V9HRFYeniTAQfoXaL2iuoFpfpQjtLZzLXYvLLDW565+oPk7Q0cLmW7n6xCefW0a3u/nOWu3ZS7p/dpj31Wyz43DarwX2tuuSMqNrHw8LpSGZZU/1zlQwuZkSvE6683dgfm+AjsaWUHYR8jhmNM7343wRpgoikqWFa3b5wdiuT9jUAoBmEAijMnrYA5M0+fIbz0N240pOm5SUEJ54C1CYKRTHZy+SWtkoPIY84lWkzcC2xvdm1uhvjJVjXjKAfM3//QSjvVBcrlEKh5XVlUgANKDkY84y9SH+g==
ANSWER: Social-cognitive theorists build on well-established concepts of learning and cognition. They tend to believe that the best way to predict someone's behavior in a given situation is to observe that person's behavior in similar situations. They have been faulted for underemphasizing the importance of unconscious motives, emotions, and biologically influenced traits.

Question

qOMCVJIl1blAUbm01qPxfWRR3kRAcst1pDHQJ9Wt1+OPW4o2ETzSAvYFwOShvMNCm6C0pk+vFVfuRjtTLAjvAY5YsUDMwqi1zktxzsdxlz6Vs6Fz7G3ft+AH/wj9TKlqf+18RyfkfhimBOn7mJjtCnU6VRkkQUyuWyItDKvW6Xch4yQkpGHF3EP0ezcPM4+dsxCdJzhDFvJnzMY063ENpaD7jIvTB84Ui5g31EQhR+7T5Fn9AbreLDyIXerVaw6dZMNdrWS1fNNy6wS/5y/3G1+WrWtm7y/Iq8x1cyMX3plDR9iNDwWJ0lkveEW8P+cnJt+eUcZ4MdhSE3ZtAhDfRL0i6VsbTR36Gkmy87bnnnAXBnM4IzDtmmYdNWni+bbxxjGpyOnIkmbM1gWBCs4ecmlMJerXJhd2xOJQtw==
ANSWER: The self is the center of personality, organizing our thoughts, feelings, and actions. Considering possible selves helps motivate us toward positive development, but focusing too intensely on ourselves can lead to the spotlight effect. Self-esteem is our feeling of self-worth; self-efficacy is our sense of competence on a task. High self-esteem correlates with less pressure to conform, with persistence at difficult tasks, and with happiness. But the direction of the correlation is not clear. Psychologists caution against unrealistically promoting children's feelings of self-worth. It's better to reward their achievements, which leads to feelings of competence.

Question

NG6Z/RoZG43d1Yz8tTg64xSG5frhqL1aT6v5cSU9bwPtY+w8BTTlEuYEZk3wmo5wXLWZF6MI7owlcfS4SWNveQBFE2fjYR0/eDvThRqxKneirn3UsmfIrO7usz+avIOtxDe2kfhjZKvzTQ/7KTivQsyy9A+k9+7b3evr2EhMzUoxBll/A1h40bqYucxyiRyUCOF9v4T3mMQwVLSVlTBxKrodERBZcC7U0MGYTyyJHynXmury9byMBQITiuyqd3ukvxO70p3BVb4VRLBP8HTKpBP2EqJPcsE2/Ogsntr0r4FQkOpeZu825BRkQaGZYaQjxxHeYkp06a3ALuBF7/cf3we7bP1pUfzVMexUEJ8WO4hTEmq5nxay775A4JbETFO4egCo5IZCTVjRF6eswE0CtA==
ANSWER: Self-serving bias is our tendency to perceive ourselves favorably, as when viewing ourselves as better than average or when accepting credit for our successes but not blame for our failures. Narcissism is excessive self-love and self-absorption. Defensive self-esteem is fragile, focuses on sustaining itself, and views failure or criticism as a threat. Secure self-esteem enables us to feel accepted for who we are.

Question

VBnW3pSbF64eQBAJrmLSYApQWeQ0vpRAQcvVef17rlTxVyjxunmTB+H5SzneDfNnzkFVLJLDzSfX2VpBMRMpseaKmv10ig8i/RaxlLOVdcOebJ6YC2DkYuU02pftFXRuE6yi2OgmbtNdlU079jjqrSMaMpDprXMaXC9dvhNTCwTSCW6B4GMj3xtYoDZP/STKV+wfUMDdJv/wrbKyRSRmUheKfcRhKgBxLplzmtshiIj/Tppgh2/yyhrFZN8pkHFr92MJ1Q==
ANSWER: Within any culture, the degree of individualism or collectivism varies from person to person. Cultures based on self-reliant individualism, like those found in North America and Western Europe, tend to value personal independence and individual achievement. They define identity in terms of self-esteem, personal goals and attributes, and personal rights and liberties. Cultures based on socially connected collectivism, like those in many parts of Asia and Africa, tend to value interdependence, tradition, and harmony, and they define identity in terms of group goals, commitments, and belonging to one's group.

Terms and Concepts to Remember

Test yourself on these terms.

Question

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

Experience the Testing Effect

Test yourself repeatedly throughout your studies. This will not only help you figure out what you know and don’t know; the testing itself will help you learn and remember the information more effectively thanks to the testing effect.

Question 13.13

1. dM8qpdrAP83EAXf3 theories of personality focus on describing characteristic behavior patterns, such as agreeableness or extraversion.

Question 13.14

jaiVZguTbigz5eMzv9yA+O25xJ3sPZ0ijK1mv1JVDRRWe3a3C816DiwogA4NPmZv1YnNy4PojVRG7EI+oN2P53iIHobJghuTfAhsruFUMZvTiX48jKfndVOV8b/Z7dYbkQ5O3NUDzI8nGSdGLfCuJM/UQzGH768e74Mezk2UDFnNoEis5jePZ3WfMFGT4zk72JqXdKcGOWPGfvWlXQhmR3A/x0ysNOWgjiqL4zbwuixF7WocrFhWUkOnAcXbzEaiUF6xrnk+RbUKmBtBjWC5oMZ388JVIz0nmRPDRIx/AW6HM+TcE97KSmhZQfIMoV6UA3VQQBy6x7SPK7tqJpYF/KFABgRluSVdpa9ZA2C+hiY2V5FcjkvN8g==

Question 13.15

VwcijawockRopcDngGiHnAtl5FkMOtcCGb7ElWjteaM9U9SmIVghbnQKgqpZhzOe5RXdtFreFfvEK/Fo9lotDDU5bmXrnWZTMedFSiKKJPr/f/3LPbTul4ly2YT1mMHIInM0MKW/1x2++jzIHAAtTNPn9CcrIpTpq0gWiAic4u5rTpNPMTVCfgjoHgd42M0/ZyexBiu/olQUCrzriou58H3+8YUhDNJWnATiUjCySyaO9/G/qIhdnnkaJbU=

Question 13.16

p75tPrnm79ewn7bTQAQaJhPra9WbXIrtV9gpWwPARpvKg6Erp6P58gOg1ThRxusrQE62YC7HD/ufej5VELSTtTWRdExDNdvxq4yrUPmoUEJ1yfssX6HiHZIhq5z+0AbKnpM/dkunUvvvD0n6ExCxTDBc3xCqDjdmltkdngSCovCcUZJli24qB2MCZwTrvkdrAL8/iERD+o510ML1AK9YxFaPbv1y1km00EPSzYOOqNDleWXEPAMBieO/9XIea79gJfvq4VyLF6FsLc/GkhNGJ+yQg2ni/7Wdn65gr9zdBkuTQTXFzc9f95jGL+dNIP5kuY8fdhDrs3XmI7yMDAw19N/clDcMngbUwQ3jdcinEOJjtobdudbsTkQo5c4YlCsdH9U6cIMkb4aYPdu6E6le8Q==

Question 13.17

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

Question 13.18

6. Critics say that igHlIUiSeAg0rbJ0ntl4jRx+OhLxx8J7 personality theory is very sensitive to an individual's interactions with particular situations, but that it gives too little attention to the person's enduring traits.

525

Question 13.19

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

Question 13.20

yzYFvrMnUUB15PYldAv15rH7ViNp9pAuLVVy4ejr6uORxT+PS3qUC58Lc5Kw+i4MG3r+z1jpVyEPss/grVw+E7RAOKGnaiN/XbER2QhFIBNmFRkbAY42hJxYGmT38frT1R82ulu/WQdvXOZyGUjuVt8/U3to1zdg6FYq946tURH2USdsyeFXlZi/jq4o6Cx9chgpSpR2YiWuLbpRB3zTDr9zpB011HzYkrkmCRKHTUiRGmvLIhORPrFpoQs=
ANSWER: Yes, if that self-love is of the secure type. Secure self-esteem promotes a focus beyond the self and a higher quality of life. Excessive self-love may promote artificially high or defensive self-esteem, which may lead to unhappiness if negative external feedback triggers anger or aggression.

Question 13.21

9. The tendency to overestimate others' attention to and evaluation of our appearance, performance, and blunders is called the CNXcpCEfnSKHhHQ7I638gzHw6HGKUFy2 .

Question 13.22

5XDEICa25QaS887kEWYjlBy4q1WllX6LPsTII77w2AT995lhLM5AMGYTno4o5E1R1nQPwnzsSl+L0QMEK/pN3Af9vgkdOZGrmjlzk6nub3JGSJs5GJRaYHRsX6PN6vcy+FnmFKTOrtD5SlZJsoVsGt2JruoT61EOLrZDlY1hUYdZxI2nMJ57ZcqJU9tXFvnkKplxpZ3eg8eqexhJQzXoRDbGl+SD9dKVA0qBOgU6xpze6iaN6Q1/U/L0TplGkK6pKr/+dMdUABi1zICs4SRcpbnGrVIsnz94dGVY5Sj9sCyscyhQxVX+aFBmNXBGkvLdGUz8DmNzVaxRAOalyivs8Q==

Use image to create your personalized study plan, which will direct you to the resources that will help you most inimage .