Biological Rhythms and Sleep
Like the ocean, life has its rhythmic tides. Over varying time periods, our bodies fluctuate, and with them, our minds. Let’s look more closely at two of those biological rhythms—our 24-hour biological clock and our 90-minute sleep cycle.
Circadian Rhythm
Eric Isselée/Shutterstock
Peter Chadwick/Science Source
3-5 How do our biological rhythms influence our daily functioning?
circadian [ser-KAY-dee-an] rhythm the biological clock; regular bodily rhythms (for example, of temperature and wakefulness) that occur on a 24-hour cycle.
The rhythm of the day parallels the rhythm of life—from our waking at a new day’s birth to our nightly return to what Shakespeare called “death’s counterfeit.” Our bodies roughly synchronize with the 24-hour cycle of day and night thanks to an internal biological clock called the circadian rhythm (from the Latin circa, “about,” and diem, “day”). As morning approaches, body temperature rises; it then peaks during the day, dips for a time in early afternoon (when many in Mediterranean and Central American regions take siestas), and begins to drop again in the evening. Thinking is sharpest and memory most accurate when we are at our daily peak in circadian arousal. Try pulling an all-nighter or working an occasional night shift. You’ll feel groggiest in the middle of the night but may gain new energy when your normal wake-up time arrives.
Some students sleep like the fellow who stayed up all night to see where the Sun went. (Then it dawned on him.)
Age and experience can alter our circadian rhythm. Most 20-year-olds are evening-energized “owls,” with performance improving across the day (May & Hasher, 1998). Most older adults are morning-loving “larks,” with performance declining as the day wears on. By mid-evening, when the night has hardly begun for many young adults, retirement homes are typically quiet. After about age 20 (slightly earlier for women), we begin to shift from being owls to being larks (Roenneberg et al., 2004). Women become more morning oriented as they have children and also as they transition to menopause (Leonhard & Randler, 2009; Randler & Bausback, 2010). Night owls tend to be smart and creative (Giampietro & Cavallera, 2007). Morning types tend to do better in school, arrive at their appointments on time, take more initiative, and to be less vulnerable to depression (Preckel et al., 2013; Randler, 2008, 2009; Werner et al., 2015).
Sleep Stages
3-6 What is the biological rhythm of our sleeping and dreaming stages?
Sooner or later, sleep overtakes us and consciousness fades as different parts of our brain’s cortex stop communicating (Massimini et al., 2005). Yet the sleeping brain remains active and has its own biological rhythm.
REM sleep rapid eye movement sleep; a recurring sleep stage during which vivid dreams commonly occur. Also known as paradoxical sleep, because the muscles are relaxed (except for minor twitches) but other body systems are active.
About every 90 minutes, you cycle through four distinct sleep stages. This fact came to light after 8-year-old Armond Aserinsky went to bed one night in 1952. His father, Eugene, a University of Chicago graduate student, needed to test an electroencephalograph he had repaired that day (Aserinsky, 1988; Seligman & Yellen, 1987). Placing electrodes near Armond’s eyes to record the rolling eye movements then believed to occur during sleep, Aserinsky watched the machine go wild, tracing deep zigzags on the graph paper. Could the machine still be broken? As the night proceeded and the activity recurred, Aserinsky realized that the periods of fast, jerky eye movements were accompanied by energetic brain activity. Awakened during one such episode, Armond reported having a dream. Aserinsky had discovered what we now know as REM sleep (rapid eye movement sleep).
Similar procedures used with thousands of volunteers showed the cycles were a normal part of sleep (Kleitman, 1960). To appreciate these studies, imagine yourself as a participant. As the hour grows late, you feel sleepy and yawn in response to reduced brain metabolism. (Yawning, which is also socially contagious, stretches your neck muscles and increases your heart rate, which increases your alertness [Moorcroft, 2003].) When you are ready for bed, a researcher comes in and tapes electrodes to your scalp (to detect your brain waves), on your chin (to detect muscle tension), and just outside the corners of your eyes (to detect eye movements) (FIGURE 3.8). Other devices will record your heart rate, respiration rate, and genital arousal.
Figure 3.7: FIGURE 3.8 Measuring sleep activity Sleep researchers measure brain-wave activity, eye movements, and muscle tension with electrodes that pick up weak electrical signals from the brain, eyes, and facial muscles. (From Dement, 1978.)
Dolphins, porpoises, and whales sleep with one side of their brain at a time (Miller et al., 2008).
alpha waves the relatively slow brain waves of a relaxed, awake state.
Figure 3.8: FIGURE 3.9 Brain waves and sleep stages The beta waves of an alert, waking state and the regular alpha waves of an awake, relaxed state differ from the slower, larger delta waves of deep NREM-3 sleep. Although the rapid REM sleep waves resemble the near-waking NREM-1 sleep waves, the body is more aroused during REM sleep than during NREM sleep.
When you are in bed with your eyes closed, the researcher in the next room sees on the EEG the relatively slow alpha waves of your awake but relaxed state (FIGURE 3.9). As you adapt to all this equipment, you grow tired and, in an unremembered moment, slip into sleep (FIGURE 3.10). The transition is marked by the slowed breathing and the irregular brain waves of non-REM stage 1 sleep. Using the American Academy of Sleep Medicine classification of sleep stages, this is called NREM-1 sleep (Silber et al., 2008).
Figure 3.9: FIGURE 3.10 The moment of sleep We seem unaware of the moment we fall into sleep, but someone watching our brain waves could tell (Dement, 1999).
In one of his 15,000 research participants, William Dement (1999) observed the moment the brain’s perceptual window to the outside world slammed shut. Dement asked this sleep-deprived young man with eyelids taped open to press a button every time a strobe light flashed in his eyes (about every 6 seconds). After a few minutes the young man missed one. Asked why, he said, “Because there was no flash.” But there was a flash. He missed it because (as his brain activity revealed) he had fallen asleep for 2 seconds, missing not only the flash 6 inches from his nose but also the awareness of the abrupt moment of entry into sleep.
Sidney Harris/Science Cartoons Plus
hallucinations false sensory experiences, such as seeing something in the absence of an external visual stimulus.
During this brief NREM-1 sleep you may experience fantastic images resembling hallucinations—sensory experiences that occur without a sensory stimulus. You may have a sensation of falling (at which moment your body may suddenly jerk) or of floating weightlessly. These hypnagogic sensations may later be incorporated into your memories. People who claim to have been abducted by aliens—often shortly after getting into bed—commonly recall being floated off (or pinned down on) their beds (Clancy, 2005; McNally, 2012).
To catch your own hypnagogic experiences, you might use your alarm’s snooze function.
You then relax more deeply and begin about 20 minutes of NREM-2 sleep, with its periodic sleep spindles—bursts of rapid, rhythmic brain-wave activity. Although you could still be awakened without too much difficulty, you are now clearly asleep.
To better understand EEG readings and their relationship to consciousness, sleep, and dreams, experience the tutorial and simulation at LaunchPad’s PsychSim 6: EEG and Sleep Stages.
delta waves the large, slow brain waves associated with deep sleep.
Then you transition to the deep sleep of NREM-3. During this slow-wave sleep, which lasts for about 30 minutes, your brain emits large, slow delta waves and you are hard to awaken. (It is at the end of the deep, slow-wave NREM-3 sleep that children may wet the bed.)
REM Sleep
About an hour after you first fall asleep, a strange thing happens. Rather than continuing in deep slumber, you ascend from your initial sleep dive. Returning through NREM-2 (where you spend about half your night), you enter the most intriguing sleep phase—REM sleep (FIGURE 3.11). For about 10 minutes, your brain waves become rapid and saw-toothed, more like those of the nearly awake NREM-1 sleep. But unlike NREM-1, during REM sleep your heart rate rises, your breathing becomes rapid and irregular, and every half-minute or so your eyes dart around in momentary bursts of activity behind closed lids. These eye movements announce the beginning of a dream—often emotional, usually story-like, and richly hallucinatory. Because anyone watching a sleeper’s eyes can notice these REM bursts, it is amazing that science was ignorant of REM sleep until 1952.
Figure 3.10: FIGURE 3.11 The stages in a typical night’s sleep People pass through a multistage sleep cycle several times each night, with the periods of deep sleep diminishing and REM sleep periods increasing in duration. As people age, sleep becomes more fragile, with awakenings common among older adults (Kamel & Gammack, 2006; Neubauer, 1999).
People rarely snore during dreams. When REM starts, snoring stops.
Except during very scary dreams, your genitals become aroused during REM sleep. You have an erection or increased vaginal lubrication and clitoral engorgement, regardless of whether the dream’s content is sexual (Karacan et al., 1966). Men’s common “morning erection” stems from the night’s last REM period, often just before waking. In young men, sleep-related erections outlast REM periods, lasting 30 to 45 minutes on average (Karacan et al., 1983; Schiavi & Schreiner-Engel, 1988). A typical 25-year-old man therefore has an erection during nearly half his night’s sleep, a 65-year-old man for one-quarter. Many men troubled by erectile dysfunction (impotence) have sleep-related erections, suggesting the problem is not between their legs.
Your brain’s motor cortex is active during REM sleep, but your brainstem blocks its messages. This leaves your muscles relaxed, so much so that, except for an occasional finger, toe, or facial twitch, you are essentially paralyzed. Moreover, you cannot easily be awakened. REM sleep is thus sometimes called paradoxical sleep: The body is internally aroused, with waking-like brain activity, yet asleep and externally calm.
Horses, which spend 92 percent of each day standing and can sleep standing, must lie down for REM sleep (Morrison, 2003).
RETRIEVE IT
Question
UWsjW/nrSpr4pWbtzATCetB6CQW2JPe7pD7t7YeJ7Sadq7bc6yrQ0JZvWXd84UA6H/UY6yA722YV2g8frTC0c6tSQFA+GDvk0CBvT8xNzzK8PXuiAl7gAA2wH8wOMwE19GEkueR5QyteK7WUtnOMsqgNHSeSVEBrMZwGQEyZrMSWvIWSJKqVpfj5M71i1l1sFHM4rbuod5e3jwYVC9qhLrexZ6RzmaFiHftnPA==
ANSWER: With each soldier cycling through the sleep stages independently, it is very likely that at any given time at least one of them will be awake or easily wakened in the event of a threat.
Uriel Sinai/Getty Images
The sleep cycle repeats itself about every 90 minutes for younger adults (somewhat more frequently for older adults). As the night wears on, deep NREM-3 sleep grows shorter and disappears. The REM and NREM-2 sleep periods get longer (see FIGURE 3.11). By morning, we have spent 20 to 25 percent of an average night’s sleep—some 100 minutes—in REM sleep. Thirty-seven percent of people report rarely or never having dreams “that you can remember the next morning” (Moore, 2004). Yet even they will, more than 80 percent of the time, recall a dream after being awakened during REM sleep. We spend about 600 hours a year experiencing some 1500 dreams, or more than 100,000 dreams over a typical lifetime—dreams swallowed by the night but not acted out, thanks to REM’s protective paralysis.
RETRIEVE IT
Question
LMTewUcdLcq2QRqEaOnTC/1wa6PJUHkKItvNbxgnN+2pHtQCuNgGo/40zhKo5rz++tzG0jt0EKfAYjhM+3X+UmNjg2tSLCE0ycULgGEX1wVPK1md5GrSDIFsJY1yzTl/ibGmzCj+mYoqIqLjJbtsUzPdfAw7pbmbkdkU3FB9cs1PUSi7SVi0XFrA0L0=
ANSWER: REM, NREM-1, NREM-2, NREM-3; normally we move through NREM-1, then NREM-2, then NREM-3, then back up through NREM-2 before we experience REM sleep.
Can you match the cognitive experience with the sleep stage?
Question
8TkRDXQRhovGikbGU/BueDjJ8YAIRh56YqbOUZO7nR8jADO84ZsPAcmkJk6rwS8ght81kp7etvH+WdJVmByt2mC94pHADalV1+Tom8PUsx7WOEYfjJi5r7SWDYkZhDT9eUZOH8/8ywiX3AZK3yhqa0kOGxfsHtXaVK8Rbd10x7n0deA24tbBWK91lCNdRuuj8bK9s0BWZrXh2auS3lmNqb4fbXj9+ThJ5kfKae19zrlfdqr+Q9C3PsWyMqz2T2OjkB6lo+XizmCJ2hSv59LMgVmxpNyywu+YupoAkQepsz0=
What Affects Our Sleep Patterns?
3-7 How do biology and environment interact in our sleep patterns?
The idea that “everyone needs 8 hours of sleep” is untrue. To know how much sleep people need, the first clue is their age. Newborns often sleep two-thirds of their day, most adults no more than one-third (with some thriving on fewer than 6 hours nightly, others racking up 9 or more). But there is more to our sleep differences than age. Some are awake between nightly sleep periods—sometimes called “first sleep” and “second sleep” (Randall, 2012). And some find that a 15-minute midday nap is as effective as another hour of nighttime sleep (Horne, 2011). Sleep patterns are genetically influenced, and researchers are discovering the genes that regulate sleep in humans and animals (Donlea et al., 2009; He et al., 2009; Hor & Tafti, 2009).
Canadian, American, British, German, and Japanese adults average 6½ to 7 hours of sleep on workdays and 7 to 8 hours on other days (National Sleep Foundation, 2013). Thanks to modern lighting, shift work, and social media diversions, many who would have gone to bed at 9:00 P.M. a century ago are now up until 11:00 P.M. or later. With sleep, as with waking behavior, biology and environment interact.
suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) a pair of cell clusters in the hypothalamus that controls circadian rhythm. In response to light, the SCN causes the pineal gland to adjust melatonin production, thus modifying our feelings of sleepiness.
Being bathed in (or deprived of) light disrupts our 24-hour biological clock (Czeisler et al., 1999; Dement, 1999). Bright light affects our sleepiness by activating light-sensitive retinal proteins. This signals the brain’s suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) to decrease production of melatonin, a sleep-inducing hormone (Gandhi et al., 2015; and FIGURE 3.12). Our ancestors’ body clocks were attuned to the rising and setting Sun of the 24-hour day. Many of today’s young adults adopt something closer to a 25-hour day, by staying up too late to get 8 hours of sleep. Most animals, too, when placed under unnatural constant illumination will exceed a 24-hour day.
Figure 3.11: FIGURE 3.12 The biological clock Light striking the retina signals the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) to suppress the pineal gland’s production of the sleep hormone melatonin. At night, the SCN quiets down, allowing the pineal gland to release melatonin into the bloodstream.
A circadian disadvantage: One study of a decade’s 24,121 Major League Baseball games found that teams who had crossed three time zones before playing a multiday series had nearly a 60 percent chance of losing their first game (Winter et al., 2009).
Sleep often eludes those who stay up late and sleep in on weekends, and then go to bed earlier on Sunday to prepare for the week ahead (Oren & Terman, 1998). For North Americans who fly to Europe and need to be up when their circadian rhythm cries “SLEEP,” bright light (spending the next day outdoors) helps reset the biological clock (Czeisler et al., 1986, 1989; Eastman et al., 1995).
RETRIEVE IT
Question
The g/TGG333UQKBiMjKP71TCsCI/m8=
nucleus helps monitor the brain's release of melatonin, which affects our gAS+npUBenJw0IcQUQPrQQ==
rhythm.
Sleep Deprivation and Sleep Disorders
3-9 How does sleep loss affect us, and what are the major sleep disorders?
When our body yearns for sleep but does not get it, we begin to feel terrible. Trying to stay awake, we will eventually lose. In the tiredness battle, sleep always wins.
Effects of Sleep Loss
Today, more than ever, our sleep patterns leave us not only sleepy but drained of energy and feelings of well-being. After a succession of 5-hour nights, we accumulate a sleep debt that need not be entirely repaid but cannot be satisfied by one long sleep. “The brain keeps an accurate count of sleep debt for at least two weeks,” reported sleep researcher William Dement (1999, p. 64).
In 1989, Michael Doucette was named America’s Safest Driving Teen. In 1990, while driving home from college, he fell asleep at the wheel and collided with an oncoming car, killing both himself and the other driver. Michael’s driving instructor later acknowledged never having mentioned sleep deprivation and drowsy driving (Dement, 1999).
Obviously, then, we need sleep. Sleep commands roughly one-third of our lives—some 25 years, on average. Allowed to sleep unhindered, most adults will sleep at least 9 hours a night (Coren, 1996). With that much sleep, we awaken refreshed, sustain better moods, and perform more efficiently and accurately. The U.S. Navy and the National Institutes of Health have demonstrated the benefits of unrestricted sleep in experiments in which volunteers spent 14 hours daily in bed for at least a week. For the first few days, the volunteers averaged 12 hours of sleep or more per day, apparently paying off a sleep debt that averaged 25 to 30 hours. That accomplished, they then settled back to 7.5 to 9 hours nightly and felt energized and happier (Dement, 1999). In one Gallup survey (Mason, 2005), 63 percent of adults who reported getting the sleep they needed also reported being “very satisfied” with their personal life (as did only 36 percent of those needing more sleep).
"Maybe 'Bring Your Pillow To Work Day' wasn't such a good idea."
© 2011 Marty Bucella
College and university students are especially sleep deprived; 69 percent in one national survey reported “feeling tired” or “having little energy” on at least several days during the last two weeks (AP, 2009). This trend toward tiredness has increased in recent years, causing some researchers to label current times as the “Great Sleep Recession” (Keyes et al., 2015). For students, less sleep also predicts more conflicts in friendships and romantic relationships (Gordon & Chen, 2014; Tavernier & Willoughby, 2014). Tired triggers crabbiness. In another survey, 28 percent of high school students acknowledged falling asleep in class at least once a week (National Sleep Foundation, 2006). The going needn’t get boring before students start snoring.
To see whether you are one of the many sleep-deprived students, try LaunchPad’s self-assessment activity—Assess Your Strengths: Sleep Deprivation.
In a 2013 Gallup poll, 40 percent of Americans reported getting 6 hours or less sleep a night (Jones, 2013).
“You wake up in the middle of the night and grab your smartphone to check the time—it’s 3 A.M.—and see an alert. Before you know it, you fall down a rabbit hole of email and Twitter. Sleep? Forget it.”
Nick Bilton, “Disruptions: For a Restful Night, Make Your Smartphone Sleep on the Couch,” 2014
Sleep loss is also a predictor of depression. Researchers who studied 15,500 12- to 18-year-olds found that those who slept 5 or fewer hours a night had a 71 percent higher risk of depression than their peers who slept 8 hours or more (Gangwisch et al., 2010). This link does not appear to reflect an effect of depression on sleep. When children and youth are followed through time, sleep loss predicts depression rather than vice versa (Gregory et al., 2009). Moreover, REM sleep’s processing of emotional experiences helps protect against depression (Walker & van der Helm, 2009). After a good night’s sleep, we often do feel better the next day. And that may help to explain why parentally enforced bedtimes predict less depression, and why pushing back school start time leads to improved adolescent sleep, alertness, and mood (Gregory et al., 2009; Owens et al., 2010; Perkinson-Gloor et al., 2013). Thus, the American Academy of Pediatrics (2014) advocates delaying adolescents’ school start times to “allow students the opportunity to achieve optimal levels of sleep (8 ½–9 ½ hours).”
Sleep-deprived students often function below their peak. And they know it: Four in five teens and three in five 18- to 29-year-olds wish they could get more sleep on weekdays (Mason, 2003, 2005). “Sleep deprivation has consequences—difficulty studying, diminished productivity, tendency to make mistakes, irritability, fatigue,” noted Dement (1999, p. 231). A large sleep debt “makes you stupid.” Yet that teen who staggers glumly out of bed in response to an unwelcome alarm, yawns through morning classes, and feels half-depressed much of the day may be energized at 11:00 p.m. and mindless of the next day’s looming sleepiness (Carskadon, 2002).
“Remember to sleep because you have to sleep to remember.”
James B. Maas and Rebecca S. Robbins, Sleep for Success, 2010
Lack of sleep can also make you gain weight. Sleep deprivation
increases ghrelin, a hunger-arousing hormone, and decreases its hunger-suppressing partner, leptin (Shilsky et al., 2012).
decreases metabolic rate, a gauge of energy use (Buxton et al., 2012).
increases cortisol, a stress hormone that stimulates the body to make fat.
enhances limbic brain responses to the mere sight of food and decreases cortical inhibition (Benedict et al., 2012; Greer et al., 2013; St-Onge et al., 2012).
Thus, children and adults who sleep less are fatter than average, and in recent decades people have been sleeping less and weighing more (Shiromani et al., 2012). Moreover, experimental sleep deprivation increases appetite and eating; our tired brains find fatty foods more enticing (Fang et al., 2015; Nixon et al., 2008; Patel et al., 2006; Spiegel et al., 2004; Van Cauter et al., 2007). So, sleep loss helps explain the weight gain common among sleep-deprived students (Hull et al., 2007).
Sleep also affects our physical health. When infections do set in, we typically sleep more, boosting our immune cells. Sleep deprivation can suppress the immune cells that battle viral infections and cancer (Möller-Levet et al., 2013; Motivala & Irwin, 2007). In one experiment, when researchers exposed volunteers to a cold virus, those who had been averaging less than 7 hours sleep a night were three times more likely to develop the cold than were those sleeping 8 or more hours a night (Cohen et al., 2009). Sleep’s protective effect may help explain why people who sleep 7 to 8 hours a night tend to outlive those who are chronically sleep deprived, and why older adults who have no difficulty falling or staying asleep tend to live longer than their sleep-deprived agemates (Dew et al., 2003; Parthasarathy et al., 2015).
Sleep deprivation slows reactions and increases errors on visual attention tasks similar to those involved in screening airport baggage, performing surgery, and reading X-rays (Caldwell, 2012; Lim & Dinges, 2010). Slow responses can also spell disaster for those operating equipment, piloting, or driving. Driver fatigue has contributed to an estimated 20 percent of American traffic accidents (Brody, 2002) and to some 30 percent of Australian highway deaths (Maas, 1999). One 2-year study examined the driving accidents of more than 20,000 Virginia 16- to 18-year-olds in two major cities. In one city, the high schools started 75 to 80 minutes later than in the other. The late starters had about 25 percent fewer crashes (Vorona et al., 2011). When sleepy frontal lobes confront an unexpected situation, misfortune often results.
Sleep by My Chemical Romance
Stanley Coren capitalized on what is, for many North Americans, a semi-annual sleep-manipulation experiment—the “spring forward” to daylight saving time and “fall backward” to standard time. Searching millions of records, Coren found that in both Canada and the United States, accidents increased immediately after the time change that shortens sleep (FIGURE 3.14 below). Less sleep = more accidents.
Figure 3.12: FIGURE 3.14 Canadian traffic accidents On the Monday after the spring time change, when people lose one hour of sleep, accidents increased, as compared with the Monday before. In the fall, traffic accidents normally increase because of greater snow, ice, and darkness, but they diminished after the time change. (Data from Coren, 1996.)
IMMERSIVE LEARNING Consider how researchers have addressed these issues in LaunchPad's How Would You Know If Sleep Deprivation Affects Academic Performance?
FIGURE 3.15 summarizes the effects of sleep deprivation. But there is good news! Psychologists have discovered a treatment that strengthens memory, increases concentration, boosts mood, moderates hunger, reduces obesity, fortifies the immune system, and lessens the risk of fatal accidents. Even better news: The treatment feels good, it can be self-administered, the supplies are limitless, and it’s free! If you are a typical university-age student, often going to bed near 2:00 A.M. and dragged out of bed 6 hours later by the dreaded alarm, the treatment is simple: Each night, just add 15 minutes to your sleep. Repeat until you feel more like a rested and energized student than a zombie.
Figure 3.13: FIGURE 3.15 How sleep deprivation affects us
Major Sleep Disorders
insomnia recurring problems in falling or staying asleep.
Mark Parisi/offthemark.com
No matter what their normal need for sleep, 1 in 10 adults, and 1 in 4 older adults, complain of insomnia—persistent problems in either falling or staying asleep (Irwin et al., 2006). The result is tiredness and increased risk of depression (Baglioni et al., 2011). All of us, when anxious or excited, may have trouble sleeping. (And smart phones under the pillow and used as alarm clocks increase the likelihood of disrupted sleep.) From middle age on, awakening occasionally during the night becomes the norm, not something to fret over or treat with medication (Vitiello, 2009). Ironically, insomnia is worsened by fretting about it. In laboratory studies, people who think they have insomnia do sleep less than others. But they typically overestimate how long it takes them to fall asleep and underestimate how long they actually have slept (Harvey & Tang, 2012). Even if we have been awake only an hour or two, we may think we have had very little sleep because it’s the waking part we remember.
The most common quick fixes for true insomnia—sleeping pills and alcohol—can aggravate the problem, reducing REM sleep and leaving the person with next-day blahs. Such aids can also lead to tolerance—a state in which increasing doses are needed to produce an effect. An ideal sleep aid would mimic the natural chemicals abundant during sleep, reliably producing sound sleep without side effects. Until scientists can supply this magic pill, sleep experts have offered some tips for getting better quality sleep (TABLE 3.1).
Table 3.1: TABLE 3.1
Some Natural Sleep Aids
Exercise regularly but not in the late evening. (Late afternoon is best.)
Avoid caffeine after early afternoon, and avoid food and drink near bedtime. The exception would be a glass of milk, which provides raw materials for the manufacture of serotonin, a neurotransmitter that facilitates sleep.
Relax before bedtime, using dimmer light.
Sleep on a regular schedule (rise at the same time even after a restless night) and avoid long naps.
Hide the time so you aren’t tempted to check repeatedly.
Reassure yourself that temporary sleep loss causes no great harm.
Focus your mind on nonarousing, engaging thoughts, such as song lyrics, TV programs, or vacation travel (Gellis et al., 2013).
If all else fails, settle for less sleep, either going to bed later or getting up earlier.
|
“The lion and the lamb shall lie down together, but the lamb will not be very sleepy.”
Woody Allen, in the movie Love and Death, 1975
narcolepsy a sleep disorder characterized by uncontrollable sleep attacks. The sufferer may lapse directly into REM sleep, often at inopportune times.
Falling asleep is not the problem for people with narcolepsy (from the Greek narke, “numbness,” and lepsis, “seizure”), who have sudden attacks of overwhelming sleepiness, usually lasting less than 5 minutes. Narcolepsy attacks can occur at the most inopportune times, often triggered by strong emotions—perhaps just after taking a terrific swing at a softball or when laughing loudly, shouting angrily, or having sex (Dement, 1978, 1999). In severe cases, the person collapses directly into a brief period of REM sleep, with loss of muscular tension. People with narcolepsy—1 in 2000 of us, estimated the Stanford University Center for Narcolepsy (2002)—must therefore live with extra caution. As a traffic menace, “snoozing is second only to boozing,” says the American Sleep Disorders Association, and those with narcolepsy are especially at risk (Aldrich, 1989).
“Sleep is like love or happiness. If you pursue it too ardently it will elude you.”
Wilse Webb, Sleep: The Gentle Tyrant, 1992
sleep apnea a sleep disorder characterized by temporary cessations of breathing during sleep and repeated momentary awakenings.
Although 1 in 20 of us have sleep apnea, it was unknown before modern sleep research. Apnea means “with no breath,” and people with this condition intermittently stop breathing during sleep. After an airless minute or so, decreased blood oxygen arouses them enough to snort in air for a few seconds, in a process that repeats hundreds of times each night, depriving them of slow-wave sleep. Apnea sufferers don’t recall these episodes the next day. So, despite feeling fatigued and depressed—and hearing their mate’s complaints about their loud “snoring”—many are unaware of their disorder (Peppard et al., 2006).
Did Brahms need his own lullabies? Cranky, overweight, and nap-prone, classical composer Johannes Brahms exhibited common symptoms of sleep apnea (Margolis, 2000).
Sleep apnea is associated with obesity, and as the number of obese Americans has increased, so has sleep apnea, particularly among overweight men (Keller, 2007). Apnea-related sleep loss also contributes to obesity. In addition to loud snoring, other warning signs are daytime sleepiness, irritability, and (possibly) high blood pressure, which increases the risk of a stroke or heart attack (Dement, 1999). If one doesn’t mind looking a little goofy in the dark, the treatment—a masklike device with an air pump that keeps the sleeper’s airway open (imagine a snorkeler at a slumber party)—can effectively relieve apnea symptoms. By so doing, it can also alleviate the depression symptoms that often accompany sleep apnea (Levine, 2012; Wheaton et al., 2012).
Now I lay me down to sleep For many with sleep apnea, a continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) machine makes for sounder sleeping and better quality of life.
Brian Chase/Shutterstock
night terrors a sleep disorder characterized by high arousal and an appearance of being terrified; unlike nightmares, night terrors occur during NREM-3 sleep, within two or three hours of falling asleep, and are seldom remembered.
Unlike sleep apnea, night terrors target mostly children, who may sit up or walk around, talk incoherently, experience doubled heart and breathing rates, and appear terrified while asleep (Hartmann, 1981). They seldom wake up fully during an episode and recall little or nothing the next morning—at most, a fleeting, frightening image. Unlike nightmares—which, like other dreams, typically occur during early morning REM sleep—night terrors usually occur during the first few hours of NREM-3.
Sleepwalking—another NREM-3 sleep disorder—and sleeptalking are usually childhood disorders and, like narcolepsy, they run in families. (Sleeptalking—usually garbled or nonsensical—can occur during any sleep stage [Mahowald & Ettinger, 1990].) Occasional childhood sleepwalking occurs for about one-third of those with a sleepwalking fraternal twin and half of those with a sleepwalking identical twin. The same is true for sleeptalking (Hublin et al., 1997, 1998). Sleepwalking is usually harmless. After returning to bed on their own or with the help of a family member, few sleepwalkers recall their trip the next morning. About 20 percent of 3- to 12-year-olds have at least one episode of sleepwalking, usually lasting 2 to 10 minutes; some 5 percent have repeated episodes (Giles et al., 1994). Young children, who have the deepest and lengthiest NREM-3 sleep, are the most likely to experience both night terrors and sleepwalking. As we grow older and deep NREM-3 sleep diminishes, so do night terrors and sleepwalking.
RETRIEVE IT
Question
A well-rested person would be more likely to have GjTL9CoIFDg+skEjIOaXEiN0sus7w0Gz
(trouble concentrating/quick reaction times) and a sleep-deprived person would be more likely to sbKPfuRJdq2lkMlnFBHFNQ==
(gain weight/fight off a cold).
Dreams
Now playing at an inner theater near you: the premiere showing of a sleeping person’s vivid dream. This never-before-seen mental movie features captivating characters wrapped in a plot so original and unlikely, yet so intricate and so seemingly real, that the viewer later marvels at its creativity.
Waking from a troubling dream (you were late to something and your legs weren’t working), who among us has not wondered about this weird state of consciousness? How can our brain so creatively, colorfully, and completely construct this alternative world? In the shadowland between our dreaming and waking consciousness, we may even wonder for a moment which is real.
Discovering the link between REM sleep and dreaming ushered in a new era in dream research. Instead of relying on someone’s hazy recall hours or days after having a dream, researchers could catch dreams as they happened. They could awaken people during or within 3 minutes after a REM sleep period and hear a vivid account.
What We Dream
dream a sequence of images, emotions, and thoughts passing through a sleeping person’s mind.
Daydreams tend to involve the familiar detailsof our life—perhaps picturing ourselves explaining to an instructor why a paper will be late, or replaying in our minds personal encounters we relish or regret. REM dreams are vivid, emotional, and often bizarre—so vivid we may confuse them with reality. Awakening from a nightmare, a 4-year-old may be sure there is a bear in the house.
“I do not believe that I am now dreaming, but I cannot prove that I am not.”
Philosopher Bertrand Russell (1872-1970)
We spend 6 years of our life in dreams, many of which are anything but sweet. For both women and men, 8 in 10 dreams are marked by at least one negative event or emotion (Domhoff, 2007). Common themes include repeatedly failing in an attempt to do something; being attacked, pursued, or rejected; or experiencing misfortune (Hall et al., 1982). Dreams with sexual imagery occur less often than you might think. In one study, only 1 in 10 dreams among young men and 1 in 30 among young women had sexual content (Domhoff, 1996).
More commonly, a dream’s story line incorporates traces of previous days’ nonsexual experiences and preoccupations (De Koninck, 2000):
After suffering a trauma, people commonly report nightmares, which help extinguish daytime fears (Levin & Nielsen, 2007, 2009). One sample of Americans recording their dreams during September, 2001 reported an increase in threatening dreams following the 9/11 terrorist attacks (Propper et al., 2007).
“For what one has dwelt on by day, these things are seen in visions of the night.”
Menander of Athens (342–292 B.C.E.), Fragments
Compared with city dwellers, people in hunter-gatherer societies more often dream of animals (Mestel, 1997). Compared with nonmusicians, musicians report twice as many dreams of music (Uga et al., 2006).
Studies in four countries have found blind people mostly dreaming of using their nonvisual senses (Buquet, 1988; Taha, 1972; Vekassy, 1977). But even natively blind people sometimes “see” in their dreams (Bértolo, 2005). Likewise, people born paralyzed below the waist sometimes dream of walking, standing, running, or cycling (Saurat et al., 2011; Voss et al., 2011).
Our two-track mind continues to monitor our environment while we sleep. Sensory stimuli—a particular odor or a phone’s ringing—may be instantly and ingeniously woven into the dream story. In a classic experiment, researchers lightly sprayed cold water on dreamers’ faces (Dement & Wolpert, 1958). Compared with sleepers who did not get the cold-water treatment, these people were more likely to dream about a waterfall, a leaky roof, or even about being sprayed by someone.
A popular sleep myth: If you dream you are falling and hit the ground (or if you dream of dying), you die. Unfortunately, those who could confirm these ideas are not around to do so. Many people, however, have had such dreams and are alive to report them.
So, could we learn a foreign language by hearing it played while we sleep? If only. While sleeping, we can learn to associate a sound with a mild electric shock (and to react to the sound accordingly). We can also learn to associate a particular sound with a pleasant or unpleasant odor (Arzi et al., 2012). But we do not remember recorded information played while we are soundly asleep (Eich, 1990; Wyatt & Bootzin, 1994). In fact, anything that happens during the 5 minutes just before we fall asleep is typically lost from memory (Roth et al., 1988). This explains why sleep apnea patients, who repeatedly awaken with a gasp and then immediately fall back to sleep, do not recall the episodes. Ditto someone who awakens momentarily, sends a text message, and the next day can’t remember doing so. It also explains why dreams that momentarily awaken us are mostly forgotten by morning. To remember a dream, get up and stay awake for a few minutes.
“Follow your dreams, except for that one where you’re naked at work.”
Attributed to comedian Henny Youngman
Why We Dream
3-11 What functions have theorists proposed for dreams?
Dream theorists have proposed several explanations of why we dream, including these five:
manifest content according to Freud, the remembered story line of a dream (as distinct from its latent, or hidden, content).
latent content according to Freud, the underlying meaning of a dream (as distinct from its manifest content).
To satisfy our own wishes. In 1900, in his landmark book The Interpretation of Dreams, Sigmund Freud offered what he thought was “the most valuable of all the discoveries it has been my good fortune to make.” He proposed that dreams provide a psychic safety valve that discharges otherwise unacceptable feelings. He viewed a dream’s manifest content (the apparent and remembered story line) as a censored, symbolic version of its latent content, the unconscious drives and wishes that would be threatening if expressed directly. Although most dreams have no overt sexual imagery, Freud nevertheless believed that most adult dreams could be “traced back by analysis to erotic wishes.” Thus, a gun might be a disguised representation of a penis.
“When people interpret [a dream] as if it were meaningful and then sell those interpretations, it’s quackery.”
Sleep researcher J. Allan Hobson (1995)
Freud considered dreams the key to understanding our inner conflicts. However, his critics say it is time to wake up from Freud’s dream theory, which they regard as a scientific nightmare. Based on the accumulated science, “there is no reason to believe any of Freud’s specific claims about dreams and their purposes,” observed dream researcher William Domhoff (2003). Some contend that even if dreams are symbolic, they could be interpreted any way one wished. Others maintain that dreams hide nothing. A dream about a gun is a dream about a gun. Legend has it that even Freud, who loved to smoke cigars, acknowledged that “sometimes, a cigar is just a cigar.” Freud’s wish-fulfillment theory of dreams has in large part given way to other theories.
To file away memories. The information-processing perspective proposes that dreams may help sift, sort, and fix the day’s experiences in our memory. Some studies support this view. When tested the day after learning a task, those who had been deprived of both slow-wave and REM sleep did not do as well as those who had slept undisturbed (Stickgold, 2012). In other studies, people who heard unusual phrases or learned to find hidden visual images before bedtime remembered less the next morning if they had been awakened every time they began REM sleep than if awakened during other sleep stages (Empson & Clarke, 1970; Karni & Sagi, 1994).
Brain scans confirm the link between REM sleep and memory. The brain regions that buzzed as rats learned to navigate a maze, or as people learned to perform a visual-discrimination task, buzzed again later during REM sleep (Louie & Wilson, 2001; Maquet, 2001). So precise were these activity patterns that scientists could tell where in the maze the rat would be if awake. Some researchers dispute the dreaming-strengthens-memory idea, noting that REM sleep may support memory for reasons unrelated to dreaming and that memory consolidation may also occur during non-REM sleep (Diekelmann & Born, 2010). This much seems true: A night of solid sleep (and dreaming) has an important place in our lives. To sleep, perchance to remember.
This is important news for students, many of whom, observed researcher Robert Stickgold (2000), suffer from a kind of sleep bulimia—binge sleeping on the weekend and “purging” during the week. “If you don’t get good sleep and enough sleep after you learn new stuff, you won’t integrate it effectively into your memories,” he warned. That helps explain why high school students with high grades have averaged 25 minutes more sleep a night than their lower-achieving classmates (Wolfson & Carskadon, 1998; see FIGURE 3.16). Sacrificing sleep time to study actually worsens academic performance, by making it harder the next day to understand class material or do well on a test (Gillen-O’Neel et al., 2012).
Figure 3.14: FIGURE 3.16 A sleeping brain is a working brain
To develop and preserve neural pathways. Perhaps dreams, or the brain activity associated with REM sleep, serve a physiological function, providing the sleeping brain with periodic stimulation. This theory makes developmental sense. Stimulating experiences preserve and expand the brain’s neural pathways. Infants, whose neural networks are fast developing, spend much of their abundant sleep time in REM sleep (FIGURE 3.17).
FIGURE 3.17 Sleep across the life span As we age, our sleep patterns change. During our first few months, we spend progressively less time in REM sleep. During our first 20 years, we spend progressively less time asleep. (Data from Snyder & Scott, 1972.)
swissmacky/Shutterstock
Rapid eye movements also stir the liquid behind the cornea; this delivers fresh oxygen to corneal cells, preventing their suffocation.
To make sense of neural static. Other theories propose that dreams erupt from neural activation spreading upward from the brainstem (Antrobus, 1991; Hobson, 2003, 2004, 2009). According to “activation-synthesis theory,” dreams are the brain’s attempt to synthesize random neural activity. Much as a neurosurgeon can produce hallucinations by stimulating different parts of a patient’s cortex, so can stimulation originating within the brain. These internal stimuli activate brain areas that process visual images, but not the visual cortex area, which receives raw input from the eyes. As Freud might have expected, PET scans of sleeping people also reveal increased activity in the emotion-related limbic system (in the amygdala) during emotional dreams (Schwartz, 2012). In contrast, frontal lobe regions responsible for inhibition and logical thinking seem to idle, which may explain why our dreams are less inhibited than we are when awake (Maquet et al., 1996). Add the limbic system’s emotional tone to the brain’s visual bursts and—Voila!—we dream. Damage either the limbic system or the visual centers active during dreaming, and dreaming itself may be impaired (Domhoff, 2003).
Question: Does eating spicy foods cause us to dream more?
Answer: Any food that causes you to awaken more increases your chance of recalling a dream (Moorcroft, 2003).
To reflect cognitive development. Some dream researchers dispute both the Freudian and neural activation theories, preferring instead to see dreams as part of brain maturation and cognitive development (Domhoff, 2010, 2011; Foulkes, 1999). For example, prior to age 9, children’s dreams seem more like a slide show and less like an active story in which the dreamer is an actor. Dreams overlap with waking cognition and feature coherent speech. They simulate reality by drawing on our concepts and knowledge. They engage brain networks that also are active during daydreaming—and so may be viewed as intensified mind wandering, enhanced by visual imagery (Fox et al., 2013). Unlike the idea that dreams arise from bottom-up brain activation, the cognitive perspective emphasizes our mind’s top-down control of our dream content (Nir & Tononi, 2010).
REM rebound the tendency for REM sleep to increase following REM sleep deprivation.
TABLE 3.2 compares these major dream theories. Although today’s sleep researchers debate dreams’ functions—and some are skeptical that dreams serve any function—there is one thing they agree on: We need REM sleep. Deprived of it by repeatedly being awakened, people return more and more quickly to the REM stage after falling back to sleep. When finally allowed to sleep undisturbed, they literally sleep like babies—with increased REM sleep, a phenomenon called REM rebound. Withdrawing REM-suppressing sleeping medications also increases REM sleep, but with accompanying nightmares. Most other mammals also experience REM rebound, suggesting that the causes and functions of REM sleep are deeply biological. (That REM sleep occurs in mammals—and not in animals such as fish, whose behavior is less influenced by learning—fits the information-processing theory of dreams.)
Table 3.2: TABLE 3.2
Dream Theories
Theory |
Explanation |
Critical Considerations |
Freud’s wish-fulfillment |
Dreams preserve sleep and provide a “psychic safety valve”—expressing otherwise unacceptable feelings; contain manifest (remembered) content and a deeper layer of latent content (a hidden meaning). |
Lacks any scientific support; dreams may be interpreted in many different ways. |
Information-processing |
Dreams help us sort out the day’s events and consolidate our memories. |
But why do we sometimes dream about things we have not experienced and about past events? |
Physiological function |
Regular brain stimulation from REM sleep may help develop and preserve neural pathways. |
This does not explain why we experience meaningful dreams. |
Neural activation |
REM sleep triggers neural activity that evokes random visual memories, which our sleeping brain weaves into stories. |
The individual’s brain is weaving the stories, which still tells us something about the dreamer. |
Cognitive development |
Dream content reflects dreamers’ level of cognitive development—their knowledge and understanding. Dreams simulate our lives, including worst-case scenarios. |
Does not propose an adaptive function of dreams. |
So does this mean that because dreams serve physiological functions and extend normal cognition, they are psychologically meaningless? Not necessarily. Every psychologically meaningful experience involves an active brain. We are once again reminded of a basic principle: Biological and psychological explanations of behavior are partners, not competitors.
Dreams are a fascinating altered state of consciousness. But they are not the only altered state. As we will see next, drugs also alter conscious awareness.
RETRIEVE IT
Question
EVCSpmpMesEA9MEgI/Jb5apRRtM5wZwezWydggT0DY6mEl6Zvf1kLa47Bh3DKxPN3UvFxC7loYXIwHOD7DQewVWHUEsXMNqjWRz/vL1Nq1vnDBSFzel0P3E3FrS6Qe+IzBr1DPxCpqg=
ANSWERS: (1) Freud's wish-fulfillment (dreams as a psychic safety valve), (2) information-processing (dreams sort the day's events and form memories), (3) physiological function (dreams pave neural pathways), (4) neural activation (REM sleep triggers random neural activity that the mind weaves into stories), and (5) cognitive development (dreams reflect the dreamer's developmental stage).
REVIEW Sleep and Dreams
Learning Objectives
Test Yourself by taking a moment to answer each of these Learning Objective Questions (repeated here from within the chapter). Research suggests that trying to answer these questions on your own will improve your long-term memory of the concepts (McDaniel et al., 2009).
Question
CN6cFt1+1Qg5HoQch2fvVugCswalnkJVtMGKFNZWmt+bYSsRJ4c5bMLfHZjsyYkX2/EjVBXY0uI/MGls6O7jx7OWFIPNaxjze9RxuH9hs+tCZaEAdbqpdVH3WBAOejEiQ0HGRxDnX3TVVjyM0ueITh05/RcYgib50sS9iv19tFw=
ANSWER: Sleep is the periodic, natural loss of consciousness—as distinct from unconsciousness resulting from a coma, general anesthesia, or hibernation. (Adapted from Dement, 1999.)
Question
iYxdmOQK2xi2sQR8CjzkvgRjh3YHmZu/SYqA9vK5SC0GBOv1X80Z8CA/pvypAaVeEh8KCQR1lxxCOSsUt0NsAVFsHeEZFnJGah2RHUQreSTXals4YgXoy3/sEXyDYYR15/qx1/Xt/7fAS2t8TZ92kI3HEaGsTugH+vWqC2BtpGP34oiqnEBQF+/4f3l3X52lB/M5Uv/MSuKtBTU6N9baMR14jxsZTXBognw39nj1gDo=
ANSWER: Our bodies have an internal biological clock, roughly synchronized with the 24-hour cycle of night and day. This circadian rhythm appears in our daily patterns of body temperature, arousal, sleeping, and waking. Age and experiences can alter these patterns, resetting our biological clock.
Question
9rwAQzXcXcJXjY+oOxClp57xum+Ll6iSIRXL0gXaeTbDK9tsTqKQ7gTgxCz/fiPTtXVBOMmtiHMjrylb7jzJVA9voXnaVuBz35l2C1CSNkmvD8Aaw1K7Az18MAN8HTDViuEfQcR7ftR8Z+GLWEdHguwmct0S2iNOO0cNo85QQH2TWuizFr1WOxOaPWByIEytDojnRbvGZAauKjZrUfNrVhZILUV37Pm9FX8hpFV894bMS1kT
ANSWER: Younger adults cycle through four distinct sleep stages about every 90 minutes. (The sleep cycle repeats more frequently for older adults.) Leaving the alpha waves of the awake, relaxed stage, we descend into the irregular brain waves of non-REM stage 1 (NREM-1) sleep, often with hallucinations, such as the sensation of falling or floating. NREM-2 sleep (in which we spend the most time) follows, lasting about 20 minutes, with its characteristic sleep spindles. We then enter NREM-3 sleep, lasting about 30 minutes, with large, slow delta waves. About an hour after falling asleep, we begin periods of REM (rapid eye movement) sleep. Most dreaming occurs in this stage (also known as paradoxical sleep) of internal arousal but outward paralysis. During a normal night's sleep, NREM-3 sleep shortens and REM and NREM-2 sleep lengthens.
Question
HO7G3FSj6yLTmSBKZfU8Y3az678xh3bkoLRXHNfC5V0LcfjaGv7LZMRNSfVcYyATByrrGVbjgPv6MLuDjvRKl4Ltwu5hQegxJEXvvjs0Aq+IZGRkKy21nzH1R9CTVBrqnYAwYN8MfzA0lOfoJKA5kNOcps5y8gh2IMdY3vNamy8Ej5eYp/jJ+KyslVGQqP+JHGbnFKNyiN48hrHE72HJWqJr1WTTRfiMh545qAU4iyw=
ANSWER: Our biology—our circadian rhythm as well as our age and our body's production of melatonin (influenced by the brain's suprachiasmatic nucleus)—interacts with cultural expectations and individual behaviors to determine our sleeping and waking patterns.
Question
tnW+TIRo32x2DsaNSWyezBsJZ2kQcaVn3K0DBS85NYaZQWc8kio+xu93T/uLkYLJXOoNgBVCDZIW8mj4xauBT/oppwhfWt/SFj20sC1YBOKmCDuYFn7HIcsy2NZ2/k8rLOK8CNr5MnBGYvdRxDXwEGeOfV3fmReyFI9OBJB6zrtpzJ1M6bycHWGrud2mh5n6
ANSWER: Sleep may have played a protective role in human evolution by keeping people safe during potentially dangerous periods. Sleep also helps restore and repair damaged neurons. REM and NREM-2 sleep help strengthen neural connections that build enduring memories. Sleep promotes creative problem solving the next day. Finally, during deep sleep, the pituitary gland secretes a growth hormone necessary for muscle development.
Question
YdQw3tCinfRk3T9bP1UdBZMP7DGvuXN6+tW3IdwIEWbJNwqYNeXPvZ8JCTR4K/v0gAVZyr7hI06+KRj/4LHlfkG/1RYlCtdU2Xo99P4i2aeRtYnCTogvv9kHsxaoFWHgCJpSNjQ2FAfiEtKCFTbQx+E1rebTvIKjZvR2ISWuskF/fjsAmNGNOEizR2+SZHBoeDVSINdQzmycMnZj4dtC4CPMX8YDK8Jv6nBO/3cvm7cnKjZ4SHSp2w==
ANSWER: Sleep deprivation causes fatigue and irritability, and it impairs concentration, productivity, and memory consolidation. It can also lead to depression, obesity, joint pain, a suppressed immune system, and slowed performance (with greater vulnerability to accidents). Sleep disorders include insomnia (recurring wakefulness); narcolepsy (sudden uncontrollable sleepiness or lapsing into REM sleep); sleep apnea (the stopping of breathing while asleep; associated with obesity, especially in men); night terrors (high arousal and the appearance of being terrified; NREM-3 disorder found mainly in children); sleepwalking (NREM-3 disorder also found mainly in children); and sleeptalking.
Question
K8YeB1BangLhqrg+hcqage66d2Qwvlz4KCnST4sfHfSPAMcTCeZSa8z24GC5Aipe7xQM6wTeCuDV7bwrrrWm83Q+itmPMOZfmD+Q0qAIizpvUjBYsIdbpeLli9ctesguxahVD+1TGmUS7nWTkL3UHm8Scavdm34qQbrzuJcNZUTe+3Ya
ANSWER: We usually dream of ordinary events and everyday experiences, most involving some anxiety or misfortune. Fewer than 10 percent of dreams among men (and fewer still among women) have any sexual content. Most dreams occur during REM sleep.
Question
GIfnVrGI5yTBbi1vJF6NmRpeLqlj7QrXdBIblAeooxSQPCU8A1eOcRAHqwgJHYw6jxM1lYDzLCazLOGOoTR75xmuAvmEWrurmLbVObNAoOrYk0jzkleyGZJfWWvFB/PyiirF2qlRgVpR5kze8Jp/zGKqoDYIXKP5evPD8suAJTSFJ8ZVxzXT272egcPyhNBp2HBY2Mv973tIGhR2BOLkroAS+Ffix9RK
ANSWER: There are five major views of the function of dreams. (1) Freud's wish-fulfillment: Dreams provide a psychic "safety valve," with manifest content (story line) acting as a censored version of latent content (underlying meaning that gratifies our unconscious wishes). (2) Information-processing: Dreams help us sort out the day's events and consolidate them in memory. (3) Physiological function: Regular brain stimulation may help develop and preserve neural pathways in the brain. (4) Neural activation: The brain attempts to make sense of neural static by weaving it into a story line. (5) Cognitive development: Dreams reflect the dreamer's level of development. Most sleep theorists agree that REM sleep and its associated dreams serve an important function, as shown by the REM rebound that occurs following REM deprivation in humans and other species.
Terms and Concepts to Remember
Test yourself on these terms.
Question
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
Experience the Testing Effect
Test yourself repeatedly throughout your studies. This will not only help you figure out what you know and don’t know; the testing itself will help you learn and remember the information more effectively thanks to the testing effect.
Question
3.4
1. Our body temperature tends to rise and fall in sync with a biological clock, which is referred to as Jdb86YPx4p/Xv+rlgelSfi2eqmY=
.
Question
3.5
YxCBzq0wLIP2QUF55WVcDoUEtjIsredvyKMLqQ9xUtON154zdFS3+16JT9ABJwpnIIUW3NVo2IQxGTi4PFsSl0VgIOzw0iSaXCGCM5G/EWpZWfld8OKSvcgeAQzLk1Y57cXHPicrqmCej3eKhBK9Fupy0y0uC7c47m8J7OFPu8tnx75Y4LRAT4i07/1hRufOPIidPE9c5dZR9HCoudUZIxz2pVtT9lmxbG3/DylNejbDb67fq1/DeU5Up1mqz+JUWWjP2K9pQOXZj8TxgxjG5+vT/ZBWmonPwLTbktAfoZ/Ap9UQ0LCDTMDjwkGtFFjb58NsRqr6DvlCh+fdmPDpbwFCny8=
Question
3.6
3. The brain emits large, slow delta waves during Iz4rMpkv/wziBZLD
sleep.
Question
3.7
VIJsPdsdmvF5a5ZYaB7h3kzfIgJZsr/JDLcE6OoErOCNzgv+F1ji2jYKvwRIyq9g/0JfI21A6SphxG+eLlICf0t1OVy9G+hdV1/B0UBe9tI7AKv4+zXgR3sFpQZuRA5JorxL8Dnn6Mefh+PUZJIuNxFTgnz3bUqd6nDerBGIUfxc/C7Zg3hSILSPeJfLadXFr4FgcSsmieh0ZnVA
ANSWER: It increases in duration.
Question
3.8
Dp+uTNgYNAwNboz3Fq4YoCMCaFF8zQZqLexidqz3pC7DmzQv8Ucc59zdp2pbkm4MCTN0Z8nTNT8Cz3tgdmOhMoewo5Umt4rIq2psO6Mw/8lIRGlgHn4MwE7aFQn6PbDy7fX69EWYal5hywaH+jJnVtL4xQjewpC0zbD+b6wI0pNvkRLx7mc6qOzptX5Zrd7AxDCgja3JDXc5qn8cLa+gj7Pk8jfFypGQkJgc/LN0CMhEnGCbplXooDK5+iRMHBVAEIPLVqrG43w51WtS2p8M6MXs4Ducrfe/+76E2Qv360bQUBZG+9GRm6ReNfOyDLS8LL9UOAzKK4yheeZfSKEUkWTeS+aC32catAP2lf8ucdSDOGyYC6uPO1Mxwww=
Question
3.9
8H2BPk/nNj9KN5vwg8WVfsxJd0hco0ASrYLBrQ0X/f9S1mtYtw0xTrftpgoRgWCK03RVGZKDkCwnhLdm+QzTQAGqZ2VfuGW6w1ZqSN11c09uqln4rpioc10n/sAucYycHmhGUTi0QuoIX2JLsDH8KhcGUnGI3/QVMYE5/oDpKpeGKeKzEMT0CkckOEkOEark2aSTq8c0WEE=
ANSWER: With narcolepsy, the person periodically falls directly into REM sleep, with no warning; with sleep apnea, the person repeatedly awakens during the night.
Question
3.10
A24IB7wGId4st6JCi0aQpWik0WgcKhflRPO7tjy2QmEpNzbvokMWjFG0EYGrfR0je5y53QJ5ABTVqWwnp1d1kOjYbaXpiK2x7iocw8+0POV81hyrHEWQHYZdjlMIxyS2llJwrM2eJOfrAYgkL8rQbBBQ6GdFCGVYMCVES+xD5jiji7kmgoeo5N/0Yqv+d6ahxXiZjWywzX3qqJWT8ED743hDv8Auyzuo+8rYq0yBG0PWldUzTmXw1HQib3GTqoUN6LK+qmRnOzBtBBLiuWuUVwDxUfRHsC5PrX17RZVsVj2kzBc62ZQGja17qwLOB8g5vtsuo7BuWnGuhVTm3/OG32Q3OSIrMhVL23zowN2nm7n20K7bdPYUZzU4zhCs8igao3Pk1m/f6t4=
Question
3.11
CHFN8vGvqPCdAdqLuBjsTzh6BZDoqyG0DIyZxeUzmuhikRcLBnvnBk4HEVMqkUaRXziCyarjl7KdtjAM7UIO2H9F3jTepQiDMHfR795SLp5vICt05cyJywLqDjxodvVWVUVLkNK2tuYRMjkwpv4wSNpeDg0FK5Y9jV2bbuMubHzkQWRu8ZYXthmJIhRFCh0FEZeigv4G7NkyjUrKQ4kDNxR8hOs=
ANSWER: The activation-synthesis theory suggests that dreams are the brain's attempt to synthesize random neural activity.
Question
3.12
oiHR7TH+Y/ioVN74W82onoCsrwtTokGx+l3ffJhTFjuxlEeqr4bh4+RvVWe027qvVYHsWkqVGw9/lsMF2fudW3/fvxdsmD/DbukLil1FV/PBfuvpjdtQbYHJ6yQAayq2loBQrYogXXmVn9kGXIXapPEOWUYHUGggakoFSOb46h8uZriuTMXED1DxDNp3Oo7VsfoncgD8gCofpIsbBUIFf2mQ78rM5Np+Vg48dVvk+u+zSbQxtqqS0evfZ4zcFRAPK4S9AX60VJ8vxFVQNF8rfl48mB9fpGMLjwtIZJMmof9mPr9Dv3gRNmGm1TcE0rohzhy6e82vuZVdHb25+dVuxc+HAxGnN6wgiiSaF6KdOZPrRChSCfh69VqVh84s0RS0pQk85A2SgTAf598OeEGj0fYgdhCl+JCMyp06OjjLi6BPb+4QKepldjbfN6nE+jpJ2vnrY42TwR/XJg32P4YSRb4PpZhrXvbh8gCjPg==
ANSWER: The information-processing explanation of dreaming proposes that brain activity during REM sleep enables us to sift through what one has dwelt on by day.
Question
3.13
10. The tendency for REM sleep to increase following REM sleep deprivation is referred to as e609dh46clvrx4A0YzywGQ==
.
Use
to create your personalized study plan, which will direct you to the resources that will help you most in
.