4.1 Developmental Issues, Prenatal Development, and the Newborn

120

Developmental Psychology’s Major Issues

4-1 What three issues have engaged developmental psychologists?

developmental psychology a branch of psychology that studies physical, cognitive, and social change throughout the life span.

Researchers find human development interesting for the same reasons most of the rest of us do—they are eager to understand more about how we’ve become our current selves, and how we may change in the years ahead. Developmental psychology examines our physical, cognitive, and social development across the life span, with a focus on three major issues:

  1. Nature and nurture: How does our genetic inheritance (our nature) interact with our experiences (our nurture) to influence our development? How have your nature and your nurture influenced your life story?

  2. Continuity and stages: What parts of development are gradual and continuous, like riding an escalator? What parts change abruptly in separate stages, like climbing rungs on a ladder?

  3. Stability and change: Which of our traits persist through life? How do we change as we age?

“Nature is all that a man brings with him into the world; nurture is every influence that affects him after his birth.”

Francis Galton,
English Men of Science, 1874

Nature and Nurture

The unique gene combination created when our mother’s egg engulfed our father’s sperm helped form us, as individuals. Genes predispose both our shared humanity and our individual differences.

But our experiences also shape us. Our families and peer relationships teach us how to think and act. Even differences initiated by our nature may be amplified by our nurture. We are not formed by either nature or nurture, but by the interaction between them. Biological, psychological, and social-cultural forces interact.

Mindful of how others differ from us, however, we often fail to notice the similarities stemming from our shared biology. Regardless of our culture, we humans share the same life cycle. We speak to our infants in similar ways and respond similarly to their coos and cries (Bornstein et al., 1992a,b). All over the world, the children of warm and supportive parents feel better about themselves and are less hostile than are the children of punishing and rejecting parents (Rohner, 1986; Scott et al., 1991). Although ethnic groups have differed in some ways, including average school achievement, the differences are “no more than skin deep.” To the extent that family structure, peer influences, and parental education predict behavior in one of these ethnic groups, they do so for the others as well. Compared with the person-to-person differences within groups, between-group differences are small.

Continuity and Stages

Do adults differ from infants as a giant redwood differs from its seedling—a difference created by gradual, cumulative growth? Or do they differ as a butterfly differs from a caterpillar—a difference of distinct stages?

Researchers who emphasize experience and learning typically see development as a slow, continuous shaping process. Those who emphasize biological maturation tend to see development as a sequence of genetically predisposed stages or steps: Although progress through the various stages may be quick or slow, everyone passes through the stages in the same order.

121

Are there clear-cut stages of psychological development, as there are physical stages such as walking before running? The stage theories we will consider—of Jean Piaget on cognitive development, Lawrence Kohlberg on moral development, and Erik Erikson on psychosocial development—propose developmental stages (summarized in FIGURE 4.1). But as we will also see, some research casts doubt on the idea that life proceeds through neatly defined age-linked stages. Young children have some abilities Piaget attributed to later stages. Kohlberg’s work reflected an individualist worldview and emphasized thinking over acting. And adult life does not progress through a fixed, predictable series of steps. Chance events can influence us in ways we would never have predicted.

image
Figure 4.1: FIGURE 4.1 Comparing the stage theories (With thanks to Dr. Sandra Gibbs, Muskegon Community College, for inspiring this illustration.)

Although many modern developmental psychologists do not identify as stage theorists, the stage concept remains useful. The human brain does experience growth spurts during childhood and puberty that correspond roughly to Piaget’s stages (Thatcher et al., 1987). And stage theories contribute a developmental perspective on the whole life span, by suggesting how people of one age think and act differently when they arrive at a later age.

image
Stages of the life cycle
Shannon Wheeler

Stability and Change

As we follow lives through time, do we find more evidence for stability or change? If reunited with a long-lost grade-school friend, do we instantly realize that “it’s the same old Andy”? Or do people we befriend during one period of life seem like strangers at a later period? (At least one acquaintance of mine [DM’s] would choose the second option. He failed to recognize a former classmate at his 40-year college reunion. The aghast classmate was his long-ago first wife.)

Research reveals that we experience both stability and change. Some of our characteristics, such as temperament, are very stable:

“At 70, I would say the advantage is that you take life more calmly. You know that ‘this, too, shall pass’!”

Eleanor Roosevelt, 1954

image
Smiles predict marital stability In one study of 306 college alums, 1 in 4 with yearbook expressions like the one on the left later divorced, as did only 1 in 20 with smiles like the one on the right (Hertenstein et al., 2009).
Tom Prokop/Shutterstock
Photodisc/Getty Images

“As at 7, so at 70,” says a Jewish proverb. People predict that they will not change much in the future (Quoidbach et al., 2013). In some ways they are correct. The widest smilers in childhood and college photos are, years later, the ones most likely to enjoy enduring marriages (Hertenstein et al., 2009).

We cannot, however, predict all aspects of our future selves based on our early life. Our social attitudes, for example, are much less stable than our temperament (Moss & Susman, 1980). Older children and adolescents learn new ways of coping. Although delinquent children have elevated rates of later problems, many confused and troubled children blossom into mature, successful adults (Moffitt et al., 2002; Roberts et al., 2013; Thomas & Chess, 1986). The struggles of the present may be laying a foundation for a happier tomorrow. Life is a process of becoming.

In some ways, we all change with age. Most shy, fearful toddlers begin opening up by age 4, and most people become more conscientious, stable, agreeable, and self-confident in the years after adolescence (Lucas & Donnellan, 2009; Roberts & Mroczek, 2008; Shaw et al., 2010). Many irresponsible 18-year-olds have matured into 40-year-old business or cultural leaders. (If you are the former, you aren’t done yet.) Openness, self-esteem, and agreeableness often peak in midlife (Lucas & Donnellan, 2011; Orth et al., 2012, 2015; Specht et al., 2011). Such changes can occur without changing a person’s position relative to others of the same age. The hard-driving young adult may mellow by later life, yet still be a relatively driven senior citizen.

image
As adults grow older, there is continuity of self.
The New Yorker Collection, 1998, Peter Mueller from cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved.

Life requires both stability and change. Stability provides our identity. It enables us to depend on others and be concerned about children’s healthy development. Our potential for change gives us our hope for a brighter future. It motivates our concerns about present influences and lets us adapt and grow with experience.

RETRIEVE IT

Question

Developmental researchers who consider how biological, psychological, and social-cultural forces interact are focusing on V24Gd/RCpqToyZod and Ovs7voDlfrlPz+PY .

Question

Developmental researchers who emphasize learning and experience are supporting EQrXbmp2BRTVDKJmSh8NKA== ; those who emphasize biological maturation are supporting hUoJ86+tCQe+gYKj .

Question

jpSDtuqx8gR+uCMHyGBbS2CvitpbNgd+q/TEFZiuaGjFbbeOVLTtqR7gLPtp0UIryg6YbP3KO27s+s67OgqQme9A7jEBb6VqRoGMpNEKyrAx1WI5DdxBinLAbE9cnvPIHsDjNtMwuFX/jZvJRDfkyCUbSli9LoWphFY7d9ykCzK/qO1nK3FMgPPQqgP15Cey/Mzpm0xi/SnO5kZk7PDsjUUD6gMk2Ehpmwz1aO5I4KIKMbvXXWiBnzRkXFa5n9Ekf5cSis2cP27RKcsOm8d7g5Sd1rnAJ7WmG+hYaFn6UVA=
ANSWER: (1) Stage theory is supported by the work of Piaget (cognitive development), Kohlberg (moral development), and Erikson (psychosocial development), but it is challenged by findings that change is more gradual and less culturally universal than these theorists supposed. (2) Some traits, such as temperament, do exhibit remarkable stability across many years. But we do change in other ways, such as in our social attitudes.

Prenatal Development and the Newborn

4-2 What is the course of prenatal development, and how do teratogens affect that development?

Conception

Nothing is more natural than a species reproducing itself. And nothing is more wondrous. For you, the process started inside your grandmother—as an egg formed inside a developing female inside of her. (Your mother was born with all the immature eggs she would ever have.) Your father, in contrast, began producing sperm cells nonstop at puberty—in the beginning at a rate of more than 1000 sperm during the second it takes to read this phrase.

123

Some time after puberty, your mother’s ovary released a mature egg—a cell roughly the size of the period at the end of this sentence. Like space voyagers approaching a huge planet, some 250 million deposited sperm began their race upstream, approaching a cell 85,000 times their own size. Those reaching the egg released digestive enzymes that ate away its protective coating (FIGURE 4.2a). As soon as one sperm penetrated the coating and was welcomed in (FIGURE 4.2b), the egg’s surface blocked out the others. Before half a day elapsed, the egg nucleus and the sperm nucleus fused: The two became one.

image
Figure 4.2: FIGURE 4.2 Life is sexually transmitted (a) Sperm cells surround an egg. (b) As one sperm penetrates the egg’s jellylike outer coating, a series of chemical events begins that will cause sperm and egg to fuse into a single cell. If all goes well, that cell will subdivide again and again to emerge 9 months later as a 100-trillion-cell human being.
Meckes/Ottawa/Eye of Science/Science Source
David M. Phillips/Science Source

Consider it your most fortunate of moments. Among 250 million sperm, the one needed to make you, in combination with that one particular egg, won the race. And so it was for innumerable generations before us. If any one of our ancestors had been conceived with a different sperm or egg, or died before conceiving, or not chanced to meet their partner or. . . . The mind boggles at the improbable, unbroken chain of events that produced us.

Prenatal Development

zygote the fertilized egg; it enters a 2-week period of rapid cell division and develops into an embryo.

How many fertilized eggs, called zygotes, survive beyond the first 2 weeks? Fewer than half (Grobstein, 1979; Hall, 2004). But for us, good fortune prevailed. One cell became 2, then 4—each just like the first—until this cell division had produced some 100 identical cells within the first week. Then the cells began to differentiate—to specialize in structure and function. (“I’ll become a brain, you become intestines!”)

Question

CJRiQciLDMz+DOA4qa/F9KnqevHYIsG9RpERwiXjaJvoEhonMNPr1vCubAO0CahekMM2bTQ1LOFXc4DI40mv+wIx5UHc4D1CXJPiSPt1Xpo9ax/HJRqiz+08bVnVInw5
ANSWER: By one careful estimate, the average human has 37.2 trillion cells (Bianconi et al., 2013).

embryo the developing human organism from about 2 weeks after fertilization through the second month.

About 10 days after conception, the zygote attaches to the mother’s uterine wall, beginning approximately 37 weeks of the closest human relationship. The zygote’s inner cells become the embryo (FIGURE 4.3a below). Many of its outer cells become the placenta, the life-link that transfers nutrients and oxygen from mother to embryo. Over the next 6 weeks, the embryo’s organs begin to form and function. The heart begins to beat.

image
Figure 4.3: FIGURE 4.3 Prenatal development (a) The embryo grows and develops rapidly. At 40 days, the spine is visible and the arms and legs are beginning to grow. (b) By the end of the second month, when the fetal period begins, facial features, hands, and feet have formed. (c) As the fetus enters the fourth month, its 3 ounces could fit in the palm of your hand.
Lennart Nilsson/Albert Bonniers Publishing Company
Lennart Nilsson/Albert Bonniers Publishing Company
Lennart Nilsson/Albert Bonniers Publishing Company

fetus the developing human organism from 9 weeks after conception to birth.

By 9 weeks after conception, an embryo looks unmistakably human (FIGURE 4.3b). It is now a fetus (Latin for “offspring” or “young one”). During the sixth month, organs such as the stomach have developed enough to give the fetus a good chance of survival if born prematurely.

At each prenatal stage, genetic and environmental factors affect our development. By the sixth month, microphone readings taken inside the uterus reveal that the fetus is responsive to sound and is exposed to the sound of its mother’s muffled voice (Ecklund-Flores, 1992; Hepper, 2005). Immediately after emerging from their underwater world, newborns prefer their mother’s voice to another woman’s, or to their father’s (Busnel et al., 1992; DeCasper et al., 1984, 1986, 1994).

124

They also prefer hearing their mother’s language. At about 30 hours old, American and Swedish newborns pause more in their pacifier sucking when listening to familiar vowels from their mother’s language (Moon et al., 2013). After repeatedly hearing a fake word (tatata) in the womb, Finnish newborns’ brain waves display recognition when hearing the word after birth (Partanen et al., 2014). If their mother spoke two languages during pregnancy, they display interest in both (Byers-Heinlein et al., 2010). And just after birth, babies born to French-speaking mothers tend to cry with the rising intonation of French; babies born to German-speaking mothers cry with the falling tones of German (Mampe et al., 2009). Would you have guessed? The learning of language begins in the womb.

Prenatal development
Zygote: Conception to 2 weeks
Embryo: 2 to 9 weeks
Fetus: 9 weeks to birth

In the two months before birth, fetuses demonstrate learning in other ways, as when they adapt to a vibrating, honking device placed on their mother’s abdomen (Dirix et al., 2009). Like people who adapt to the sound of trains in their neighborhood, fetuses get used to the honking. Moreover, four weeks later, they recall the sound (as evidenced by their blasé response, compared with the reactions of those not previously exposed).

“You shall conceive and bear a son. So then drink no wine or strong drink.”

Judges 13:7

teratogens (literally, “monster maker”) agents, such as chemicals and viruses, that can reach the embryo or fetus during prenatal development and cause harm.

Sounds are not the only stimuli fetuses are exposed to in the womb. In addition to transferring nutrients and oxygen from mother to fetus, the placenta screens out many harmful substances. But some slip by. Teratogens, agents such as viruses and drugs, can damage an embryo or fetus. This is one reason pregnant women are advised not to drink alcoholic beverages or smoke cigarettes. A pregnant woman never drinks or smokes alone. When alcohol enters her bloodstream and that of her fetus, it reduces activity in both their central nervous systems. Alcohol use during pregnancy may prime the woman’s offspring to like alcohol and may put them at risk for heavy drinking and alcohol use disorder during their teen years. In experiments, when pregnant rats drank alcohol, their young offspring later displayed a liking for alcohol’s taste and odor (Youngentob et al., 2007, 2009).

fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) physical and cognitive abnormalities in children caused by a pregnant woman’s heavy drinking. In severe cases, signs include a small, out-of-proportion head and abnormal facial features.

image For an interactive review of prenatal development, see LaunchPad’s PsychSim 6: Conception to Birth. See also LaunchPad's 8-minute Video: Prenatal Development, below.

Even light drinking or occasional binge drinking can affect the fetal brain (Braun, 1996; Ikonomidou et al., 2000; Marjonen et al., 2015; Sayal et al., 2009). Persistent heavy drinking puts the fetus at risk for a dangerously low birth weight, birth defects, and for future behavior problems, hyperactivity, and lower intelligence. For 1 in about 700 children, the effects are visible as fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), marked by lifelong physical and mental abnormalities (May et al., 2014). The fetal damage may occur because alcohol has an epigenetic effect: It leaves chemical marks on DNA that switch genes abnormally on or off (Liu et al., 2009). Smoking during pregnancy also leaves epigenetic scars that weaken infants’ ability to handle stress (Stroud et al., 2014).

125

If a pregnant woman experiences extreme stress, the stress hormones flooding her body may indicate a survival threat to the fetus and produce an earlier delivery (Glynn & Sandman, 2011). Some stress in early life prepares us to cope with later adversity in life. But substantial prenatal stress exposure puts a child at increased risk for health problems such as hypertension, heart disease, obesity, and psychiatric disorders.

RETRIEVE IT

Question

The first two weeks of prenatal development is the period of the Jl4PgKyNytwtLj7E . The period of the KoAqPsjQENb1dgrm lasts from 9 weeks after conception until birth. The time between those two prenatal periods is considered the period of the u0tU4HWD9/pIaWcc .

The Competent Newborn

4-3 What are some newborn abilities, and how do researchers explore infants’ mental abilities?

“I felt like a man trapped in a woman’s body. Then I was born.”

Comedian Chris Bliss

Babies come with software preloaded on their neural hard drives. Having survived prenatal hazards, we as newborns came equipped with automatic reflex responses ideally suited for our survival. We withdrew our limbs to escape pain. If a cloth over our face interfered with our breathing, we turned our head from side to side and swiped at it.

New parents are often in awe of the coordinated sequence of reflexes by which their baby gets food. When something touches their cheek, babies turn toward that touch, open their mouth, and vigorously root for a nipple. Finding one, they automatically close on it and begin sucking—which itself requires a coordinated sequence of reflexive tonguing, swallowing, and breathing. Failing to find satisfaction, the hungry baby may cry—a behavior parents find highly unpleasant and very rewarding to relieve.

The pioneering American psychologist William James presumed that newborns experience a “blooming, buzzing confusion,” an assumption few people challenged until the 1960s. Then scientists discovered that babies can tell you a lot—if you know how to ask. To ask, you must capitalize on what babies can do—gaze, suck, turn their heads. So, equipped with eye-tracking machines and pacifiers wired to electronic gear, researchers set out to answer parents’ age-old questions: What can my baby see, hear, smell, and think?

habituation decreasing responsiveness with repeated stimulation. As infants gain familiarity with repeated exposure to a stimulus, their interest wanes and they look away sooner.

Consider how researchers exploit habituation—decreased responding with repeated stimulation. We saw this earlier when fetuses adapted to a vibrating, honking device placed on their mother’s abdomen. The novel stimulus gets attention when first presented. With repetition, the response weakens. This seeming boredom with familiar stimuli gives us a way to ask infants what they see and remember.

Even as newborns, we prefer sights and sounds that facilitate social responsiveness. We turn our heads in the direction of human voices. We gaze longer at a drawing of a face-like image (FIGURE 4.4). We prefer to look at objects 8 to 12 inches away, which—wonder of wonders—just happens to be the approximate distance between a nursing infant’s eyes and its mother’s (Maurer & Maurer, 1988). Our brain’s default settings help us connect socially.

image
Figure 4.4: FIGURE 4.4 Newborns’ preference for faces When shown these two stimuli with the same elements, Italian newborns spent nearly twice as many seconds looking at the face-like image (Johnson & Morton, 1991). Canadian newborns—average age 53 minutes in one study—displayed the same apparently inborn preference to look toward faces (Mondloch et al., 1999).

Within days after birth, our brain’s neural networks were stamped with the smell of our mother’s body. Week-old nursing babies, placed between a gauze pad from their mother’s bra and one from another nursing mother, have usually turned toward the smell of their own mother’s pad (MacFarlane, 1978). What’s more, that smell preference lasts. One experiment capitalized on the fact that some nursing mothers in a French maternity ward used a chamomile-scented balm to prevent nipple soreness (Delaunay-El Allam, 2010). Twenty-one months later, their toddlers preferred playing with chamomile-scented toys! Their peers who had not sniffed the scent while breast feeding showed no such preference. (Hmm. Will adults, who as babies associated chamomile scent with their mother’s breast, become devoted chamomile tea drinkers?) Such studies reveal the remarkable abilities with which we enter our world.

126

image
Prepared to feed and eat Animals are predisposed to respond to their offsprings’ cries for nourishment.
Lightscapes Photography, Inc./Corbis
Asia Images/Getty Images

RETRIEVE IT

Question

Developmental psychologists use repeated stimulation to test an infant's jvlni5BJrYxlfqAWTy9MAw== to a stimulus.

REVIEW Developmental Issues, Prenatal Development, and the Newborn

Learning Objectives

Test Yourself by taking a moment to answer each of these Learning Objective Questions (repeated here from within the chapter). Research suggests that trying to answer these questions on your own will improve your long-term memory of the concepts (McDaniel et al., 2009).

Question

5jWF2HoM+SSlmTkImfhro0gJgdk3+Sq1RL35rhpWl0JwTmr+zRSvUcAa4pduQWl8ZYoyQYh0LKPBCbcy3/minAkO3dWWpuc1u/e9ytXKk6VXquJZqaHkP/HGaHfs5hR2OB/iJ8U8kZNPk8ZDnLDx55AtSmnEap8QkoPGGlQrCgYGUrCiiY68TPS3XaAJhgT7DvnmYrWTd9GGmf2UxePu3y6kmlybK6b6SQAR0wxw74k=
ANSWER: Developmental psychologists study physical, mental, and social changes throughout the life span. They focus on three issues: nature and nurture (the interaction between our genetic inheritance and our experiences); continuity and stages (whether development is gradual and continuous or a series of relatively abrupt changes); and stability and change (whether our traits endure or change as we age).

Question

yurmGyKvZbqjuGtILNP9A7GTFZSU3Anm3XJU0hmHWqDyKFLGNEmLHbbzzbZ0vEqiAWQ65Ta4iYH9rk7+B/ppUuosHGlBo8R55tcrhX0+Hz9/D0VWV/12rsYrK4pxF4ix2Uqj92nNJ61wwjez4CrORdvsABGsmINfx1HfjFP8iuniKGx3xeTbY6qqgu2w5StEWkaAF9wiLAZC5jo8GOQucxblhA/V0zxYJoPSZzOA4Igj1HzIxrfRZUV4RGp8fQmOZTY+RNYfz91ZOTYh
ANSWER: The life cycle begins at conception, when one sperm cell unites with an egg to form a zygote. The zygote's inner cells become the embryo, and the outer cells become the placenta. In the next 6 weeks, body organs begin to form and function, and by 9 weeks, the fetus is recognizably human. Teratogens are potentially harmful agents that can pass through the placental screen and harm the developing embryo or fetus, as happens with fetal alcohol syndrome.

Question

yXSTbIEHwsWKpLp7V7ZMAbFCtdn6KmuEhlUK64NBjI1VJc22YxR77w63ZiZzFrdqm0ktiDm53kvgmY2zznkB2IaCWDZPCAXQOo+Ly+7hTd8ER7QQA4poL6Sc7O8zHOIwPwbL+4PXeMU86w+oxoyBmknAp+hXF1JJBe8uHEbPELMq6WQeTcD9zlpDznyaP3tkW3uuLN6PsNYSIEHSx933zHGEoa4Mwu5UrLT+c2xQ9ljoj+hCpIHuWJxlga0en7BGuoLDmTCOMYFDem+j
ANSWER: Babies are born with sensory equipment and reflexes that facilitate their survival and their social interactions with adults. For example, they quickly learn to discriminate their mother's smell and sound. Researchers use techniques that test habituation, such as the novelty-preference procedure, to explore infants' abilities.

Terms and Concepts to Remember

Test yourself on these terms.

Question

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

Experience the Testing Effect

Test yourself repeatedly throughout your studies. This will not only help you figure out what you know and don’t know; the testing itself will help you learn and remember the information more effectively thanks to the testing effect.

Question 4.1

1. The three major issues that interest developmental psychologists are nature/nurture, stability/change, and EQrXbmp2BRTVDKJmSh8NKA== /hUoJ86+tCQe+gYKj .

Question 4.2

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

Question 4.3

3. From the very first weeks of life, infants differ in their characteristic emotional reactions, with some infants being intense and anxious, while others are easygoing and relaxed. These differences are usually explained as differences in yPkUqFg1aGdvOW44AX0Q3A== .

Question 4.4

UHvYvVE43WrJSW0aROe4Hf2PKInCKYH2MSTo/ITkZBeHw4zwBaA9RA/DnxLO66GV9I00ZBlRwn2xxKj70ppQFKF+G0tEJ8LRXFTRWJnOw+qM5Kwe78g8hdTOhBf4YugVPuB2mN0GX6cd5VPsHTxfJFVocrD+d7T2N7c0+uPLk0U2CdUPPFBWRBGZzljkxWt8BqL7xKo1+/u7QPpOZ0WCtqfhFatVveGuWggdXRTJn7VXbO8KfKIEh6z85nW49FoPHBcszqRsdcIzUC6YUliK6ZTR9xFJOQ45Zc1qcMu76H/3TwAGwHUSRqEDwKybcGytqU8/2+u/95Dpl68vR1hWKEw3Pme8R/egNadfaTF3GJE9lBB+BMa25Y1LxkyKbFVk7oZwU64GHHNdhqCz4Gb9nJBYdVspXZUnLlX6eZkDexK+7ghSuJ6/5g==

Question 4.5

5. Chemicals that pass through the placenta's screen and may harm an embryo or fetus are called HFAQ+T+6zn+M299chytQuw== .

Use image to create your personalized study plan, which will direct you to the resources that will help you most in image .