8.2 Storing and Retrieving Memories

292

Memory Storage

8-9 What is the capacity of long-term memory? Are our long-term memories processed and stored in specific locations?

In Arthur Conan Doyle’s A Study in Scarlet, Sherlock Holmes offers a popular theory of memory capacity:

I consider that a man’s brain originally is like a little empty attic, and you have to stock it with such furniture as you choose. . . . It is a mistake to think that that little room has elastic walls and can distend to any extent. Depend upon it, there comes a time when for every addition of knowledge you forget something that you knew before.

Contrary to Holmes’ “memory model,” our brains are not like attics, which once filled can store more items only if we discard old ones. Our capacity for storing long-term memories is essentially limitless.

Retaining Information in the Brain

I [DM] marveled at my aging mother-in-law, a retired pianist and organist. At age 88, her blind eyes could no longer read music. But let her sit at a keyboard and she would flawlessly play any of hundreds of hymns, including ones she had not thought of for 20 years. Where did her brain store those thousands of sequenced notes?

For a time, some surgeons and memory researchers marveled at patients’ apparently vivid memories triggered by brain stimulation during surgery. Did this prove that our whole past, not just well-practiced music, is “in there,” in complete detail, just waiting to be relived? On closer analysis, the seeming flashbacks appeared to have been invented, not a vivid reliving of long-forgotten experiences (Loftus & Loftus, 1980). In a further demonstration that memories do not reside in single, specific spots, psychologist Karl Lashley (1950) trained rats to find their way out of a maze, then surgically removed pieces of their brain’s cortex and retested their memory. No matter which small brain section he removed, the rats retained at least a partial memory of how to navigate the maze. Memories are brain-based, but the brain distributes the components of a memory across a network of locations. These specific locations include some of the circuitry involved in the original experience: Some brain cells that fire when we experience something fire again when we recall it (G. Miller, 2012b; J. F. Miller et al., 2013).

The point to remember: Despite the brain’s vast storage capacity, we do not store information as libraries store their books, in single, precise locations. Instead, brain networks encode, store, and retrieve the information that forms our complex memories.

“Our memories are flexible and superimposable, a panoramic blackboard with an endless supply of chalk and erasers.”

Elizabeth Loftus and Katherine Ketcham, The Myth of Repressed Memory, 1994

EXPLICIT MEMORY SYSTEM: THE FRONTAL LOBES AND HIPPOCAMPUS

8-10 What are the roles of the frontal lobes and hippocampus in memory processing?

semantic memory explicit memory of facts and general knowledge; one of our two conscious memory systems (the other is episodic memory).

episodic memory explicit memory of personally experienced events; one of our two conscious memory systems (the other is semantic memory).

image
Figure 8.9: FIGURE 8.9  The hippocampus Explicit memories for facts and episodes are processed in the hippocampus (orange structures) and fed to other brain regions for storage.
Roger Harris/Science Source
image
Hippocampus hero Among animals, one contender for champion memorist would be a mere birdbrain—the Clark’s Nutcracker—which during winter and spring can locate up to 6000 caches of pine seed it had previously buried (Shettleworth, 1993).
Tim Zurowski/All Canada Photos/Corbis

Explicit, conscious memories are either semantic (facts and general knowledge) or episodic (experienced events). The network that processes and stores your explicit memories for these facts and episodes includes your frontal lobes and hippocampus. When you summon up a mental encore of a past experience, many brain regions send input to your frontal lobes for working memory processing (Fink et al., 1996; Gabrieli et al., 1996; Markowitsch, 1995). The left and right frontal lobes process different types of memories. Recalling a password and holding it in working memory, for example, would activate the left frontal lobe. Calling up a visual party scene would more likely activate the right frontal lobe.

hippocampus a neural center located in the limbic system; helps process explicit memories for storage.

293

Cognitive neuroscientists have found that the hippocampus, a temporal-lobe neural center located in the limbic system, can be likened to a “save” button for explicit memories (FIGURE 8.9). The hippocampus and nearby brain networks are active as people form explicit memories of names, images, and events (Squire & Wixted, 2011).

Damage to this structure therefore disrupts recall of explicit memories. Chickadees and other birds can store food in hundreds of places and return to these unmarked caches months later—but not if their hippocampus has been removed (Kamil & Cheng, 2001; Sherry & Vaccarino, 1989). With left-hippocampus damage, people have trouble remembering verbal information, but they have no trouble recalling visual designs and locations. With right-hippocampus damage, the problem is reversed (Schacter, 1996).

Subregions of the hippocampus also serve different functions. One part is active as people learn to associate names with faces (Zeineh et al., 2003). Another part is active as memory champions engage in spatial mnemonics (Maguire et al., 2003b). The rear area, which processes spatial memory, grows bigger as London cabbies learn to navigate the city’s complicated maze of streets (Woolett & Maguire, 2011).

memory consolidation the neural storage of a long-term memory.

Memories are not permanently stored in the hippocampus. Instead, this structure seems to act as a loading dock where the brain registers and temporarily holds the elements of a remembered or retrieved episode—its smell, feel, sound, and location. Then, like older files shifted to a basement storeroom, memories migrate for storage elsewhere. This storage process is called memory consolidation.

Sleep supports memory consolidation. During deep sleep, the hippocampus processes memories for later retrieval. After a training experience, the greater the hippocampus activity during sleep, the better the next day’s memory will be (Peigneux et al., 2004). Researchers have watched the hippocampus and brain cortex displaying simultaneous activity rhythms during sleep, as if they were having a dialogue (Euston et al., 2007; Mehta, 2007). They suspect that the brain is replaying the day’s experiences as it transfers them to the cortex for long-term storage. Cortex areas surrounding the hippocampus support the processing and storing of explicit memories (Squire & Zola-Morgan, 1991).

IMPLICIT MEMORY SYSTEM: THE CEREBELLUM AND BASAL GANGLIA

8-11 What are the roles of the cerebellum and basal ganglia in memory processing?

Your hippocampus and frontal lobes are processing sites for your explicit memories. But you could lose those areas and still, thanks to automatic processing, lay down implicit memories for skills and newly conditioned associations. Joseph LeDoux (1996) recounted the story of a brain-damaged patient whose amnesia left her unable to recognize her physician as, each day, he shook her hand and introduced himself. One day, she yanked her hand back, for the physician had pricked her with a tack in his palm. The next time he returned to introduce himself she refused to shake his hand but couldn’t explain why. Having been classically conditioned, she just wouldn’t do it. Intuitively (implicitly) she felt what she could not explain.

The cerebellum plays a key role in forming and storing the implicit memories created by classical conditioning. With a damaged cerebellum, people cannot develop certain conditioned reflexes, such as associating a tone with an impending puff of air—and thus do not blink in anticipation of the puff (Daum & Schugens, 1996; Green & Woodruff-Pak, 2000). Implicit memory formation needs the cerebellum.

image
Mark Parisi/offthemark.com

294

The basal ganglia, deep brain structures involved in motor movement, facilitate formation of our procedural memories for skills (Mishkin, 1982; Mishkin et al., 1997). The basal ganglia receive input from the cortex but do not return the favor of sending information back to the cortex for conscious awareness of procedural learning. If you have learned how to ride a bike, thank your basal ganglia.

Our implicit memory system, enabled partly by these more ancient brain areas, helps explain why the reactions and skills we learned during infancy reach far into our future. Yet as adults, our conscious memory of our first three years is blank, an experience called infantile amnesia. In one study, events children experienced and discussed with their mothers at age 3 were 60 percent remembered at age 7 but only 34 percent remembered at age 9 (Bauer et al., 2007). Two influences contribute to infantile amnesia: First, we index much of our explicit memory using words that nonspeaking children have not learned. Second, the hippocampus is one of the last brain structures to mature, and as it does, more gets retained (Akers et al., 2014).

RETRIEVE IT

Question

6QL8aARBoy2tvnr0kPE3yCPgZBux2exCvMsDX41KtTb2mheVO+YqLLpYnb6vVmOYK8UWXr7cjPJqBZqdta0xUgYd6nbnM5vUtS4bYup0/Yz+zJZZbULNxt0K5IdPU11Hnwe7Plm/Hp47L2GBNPxkpkzGSH+11w0bXFMPQgNLHILfCBC/JHjF/O4lG7CT2s+bGWZT5uTD7kedGAFwgP6x3GSRoat7U32+QUiezNjfgAZtd0OyCnaZLE3N7vGiAZ4j+/AA2HCJWXs=
ANSWER: The cerebellum and basal ganglia are important for implicit memory processing, and the frontal lobes and hippocampus are key to explicit memory formation.

Question

i4zbVUy9HbJ661GJe58zX7fDm4aHngQCM/5R2sWDdbK8jz1qD5InW1/Ra9hWrDucus4i7FFv+Rh3obUAfV7h8CBJV/5NKGL4NwYd8rZny8L2G+l5635mmi3LIQt+3A37QK61wlcX3uiK2SmUrC12KsVywjOsQNd2mz4ouVpzUUxI9YAkdGEi2RofRZwCvKvVfMOspItGMYu6w6vLd8W46FX4HTEGJ8xo1dKLRrj71A33474LuWduUK0WTpVc0p2OgmRZ5JQ14dPE7Qyhcs7Zhxgt8eaOQpKSnvombtvGJVRIEaac34ohDc5utGCBlMsR
ANSWER: Our explicit conscious memories of facts and episodes differ from our implicit memories of skills (such as shoe tying) and classically conditioned responses. The parts of the brain involved in explicit memory processing (the frontal lobes and hippocampus) may have sustained damage in the accident, while the parts involved in implicit memory processing (the cerebellum and basal ganglia) appear to have escaped unharmed.

THE AMYGDALA, EMOTIONS, AND MEMORY

8-12 How do emotions affect our memory processing?

Our emotions trigger stress hormones that influence memory formation. When we are excited or stressed, these hormones make more glucose energy available to fuel brain activity, signaling the brain that something important has happened. Moreover, stress hormones focus memory. Stress provokes the amygdala (two limbic system, emotion-processing clusters) to initiate a memory trace that boosts activity in the brain’s memory-forming areas (Buchanan, 2007; Kensinger, 2007) (FIGURE 8.10). It’s as if the amygdala says, “Brain, encode this moment for future reference!” The result? Emotional arousal can sear certain events into the brain, while disrupting memory for irrelevant events that occur around the same time (Birnbaum et al., 2004; Brewin et al., 2007).

image
Figure 8.10: FIGURE 8.10 Review key memory structures in the brain
Figure 8.10: Frontal lobes and hippocampus: explicit memory formation
Figure 8.10: Cerebellum and basal ganglia: implicit memory formation
Figure 8.10: Amygdala: emotion-related memory formation

Significantly stressful events can form almost indelible memories. After traumatic experiences—a school shooting, a house fire, a rape—vivid recollections of the horrific event may intrude again and again. It is as if they were burned in: “Stronger emotional experiences make for stronger, more reliable memories,” noted James McGaugh (1994, 2003). Such experiences even strengthen recall for relevant, immediately preceding events (Dunsmoor et al., 2015). This makes adaptive sense: If we can remember what happened right before, we may recognize potential dangers. Emotional events produce tunnel vision memory. They focus our attention and recall on high-priority information, and reduce our recall of irrelevant details (Mather & Sutherland, 2012). Whatever rivets our attention gets well recalled, at the expense of the surrounding context.

flashbulb memory a clear memory of an emotionally significant moment or event.

Emotion-triggered hormonal changes help explain why we long remember exciting or shocking events, such as our first kiss or our whereabouts when learning of a loved one’s death. In a 2006 Pew survey, 95 percent of American adults said they could recall exactly where they were or what they were doing when they first heard the news of the 9/11 attacks. This perceived clarity of memories of surprising, significant events leads some psychologists to call them flashbulb memories. It’s as if the brain commands, “Capture this!”

295

The people who experienced a 1989 San Francisco earthquake did just that. A year and a half later, they had perfect recall of where they had been and what they were doing (verified by their recorded thoughts within a day or two of the quake). Others’ memories for the circumstances under which they merely heard about the quake were more prone to errors (Neisser et al., 1991; Palmer et al., 1991).

Which is more important—your experiences or your memories of them?

Our flashbulb memories are noteworthy for their vividness and our confidence in them. But as we relive, rehearse, and discuss them, these memories may come to err. With time, some errors crept into people’s 9/11 recollections (compared with their earlier reports taken right after 9/11). Mostly, however, people’s memories of 9/11 remained consistent over the next two to three years (Conway et al., 2009; Hirst et al., 2009; Kvavilashvili et al., 2009).

Dramatic experiences remain bright and clear in our memory in part because we rehearse them. We think about them and describe them to others. Memories of our best experiences, which we enjoy recalling and recounting, also endure (Storm & Jobe, 2012; Talarico & Moore, 2012). One study invited 1563 Boston Red Sox and New York Yankees fans to recall the baseball championship games between their two teams in 2003 (Yankees won) and 2004 (Red Sox won). Fans recalled much better the game their team won (Breslin & Safer, 2011).

Synaptic Changes

8-13 How do changes at the synapse level affect our memory processing?

As you read this chapter and think and learn about memory, your brain is changing. Given increased activity in particular pathways, neural interconnections are forming and strengthening.

The quest to understand the physical basis of memory—how information becomes embedded in brain matter—has sparked study of the synaptic meeting places where neurons communicate with one another via their neurotransmitter messengers. Eric Kandel and James Schwartz (1982) observed synaptic changes during learning in the neurons of the California sea slug, Aplysia, a simple animal with a mere 20,000 or so unusually large and accessible nerve cells. A sea slug can be classically conditioned (with electric shock) to reflexively withdraw its gills when squirted with water, much as a soldier traumatized by combat jumps at the sound of fireworks. When learning occurs, Kandel and Schwartz discovered, the slug releases more of the neurotransmitter serotonin into certain neurons. These synapses then become more efficient at transmitting signals. Experience and learning can increase—even double—the number of synapses, even in slugs (Kandel, 2012).

image
Aplysia The California sea slug, which neuroscientist Eric Kandel studied for 45 years, has increased our understanding of the neural basis of learning and memory.
© Donna Ikenberry/Art Directors & TRIP Alamy

long-term potentiation (LTP) an increase in a cell’s firing potential after brief, rapid stimulation. Believed to be a neural basis for learning and memory.

In experiments with people, rapidly stimulating certain memory-circuit connections has increased their sensitivity for hours or even weeks to come. The sending neuron now needs less prompting to release its neurotransmitter, and more connections exist between neurons. This increased efficiency of potential neural firing, called long-term potentiation (LTP), provides a neural basis for learning and remembering associations (Lynch, 2002; Whitlock et al., 2006) (FIGURE 8.11 below). Several lines of evidence confirm that LTP is a physical basis for memory:

image
FIGURE 8.11  Doubled receptor sites An electron microscope image (a) shows just one receptor site (gray) reaching toward a sending neuron before long-term potentiation. Image (b) shows that, after LTP, the receptor sites have doubled. This means the receiving neuron has increased sensitivity for detecting the presence of the neurotransmitter molecules that may be released by the sending neuron. (From Toni et al., 1999.)
From N. Toni et al., Nature, 402, Nov. 25, 1999. Dominique Muller

296

After long-term potentiation has occurred, passing an electric current through the brain won’t disrupt old memories. But the current will wipe out very recent memories. Such is the experience both of laboratory animals and of severely depressed people given electroconvulsive therapy. A blow to the head can do the same. Football players and boxers momentarily knocked unconscious typically have no memory of events just before the knockout (Yarnell & Lynch, 1970). Their working memory had no time to consolidate the information into long-term memory before the lights went out.

Recently, I [DM] did a little test of memory consolidation. While on an operating table for a basketball-related tendon repair, I was given a face mask and soon could smell the anesthesia gas. “So how much longer will I be with you?” I asked the anesthesiologist. My last moment of memory was her answer: “About 10 seconds.” My brain spent that 10 seconds consolidating a memory for her words, but could not tuck any further memory away before I was out cold.

Some memory-biology explorers have helped found companies that are competing to develop memory-altering drugs. The target market for memory-boosting drugs includes millions of people with memory-destroying Alzheimer’s disease, millions more with mild cognitive impairment that often becomes Alzheimer’s, and countless millions who would love to turn back the clock on age-related memory decline. Meanwhile, one safe and free memory enhancer is already available on your college campus: effective study techniques followed by adequate sleep!

Some of us may wish instead for memory-blocking drugs that, when taken after a traumatic experience, might blunt intrusive memories (Adler, 2012; Kearns et al., 2012). In one experiment, victims of car accidents, rapes, and other traumas received, for 10 days following their horrific event, either one such drug, propranolol, or a placebo. When tested three months later, half the placebo group but none of the drug-treated group showed signs of stress disorder (Pitman et al., 2002, 2005).

FIGURE 8.12 summarizes the brain’s two-track memory processing and storage system for implicit (automatic) and explicit (effortful) memories. The bottom line: Learn something and you change your brain a little.

image
Figure 8.11: FIGURE 8.12  Our two memory systems

297

RETRIEVE IT

Question

N1r2vN4040ZeVnIYElVPtKsyi9LKNJXQF79Pw/1zgxTrReeK/nUiuiJGaPSpaT8Q/jD2F2+zmV5Eo1WmB+yD9xNuG91l0tC+/f2xPS/44R/XgLTB+kNhmSoFTABmQ1Ki5yPaBvBAQ6QtFfjET0zbu3+ztBSVhr0o2Q87NHbti/Ai7nf9
ANSWER: the amygdala

Question

The neural basis for learning and memory, found at the synapses in the brain's memory-circuit connections, results from brief, rapid stimulation. It is called LuUWjbuSrIKumE6U5/wShS8CPqxC5VIdKfo28A== .

Memory Retrieval

After the magic of brain encoding and storage, we still have the daunting task of retrieving the information. What triggers retrieval?

Retrieval Cues

8-14 How do external cues, internal emotions, and order of appearance influence memory retrieval?

Imagine a spider suspended in the middle of her web, held up by the many strands extending outward from her in all directions to different points. If you were to trace a pathway to the spider, you would first need to create a path from one of these anchor points and then follow the strand down into the web.

“Memory is not like a container that gradually fills up; it is more like a tree growing hooks onto which memories are hung.”

Peter Russell, The Brain Book, 1979

The process of retrieving a memory follows a similar principle, because memories are held in storage by a web of associations, each piece of information interconnected with others. When you encode into memory a target piece of information, such as the name of the person sitting next to you in class, you associate with it other bits of information about your surroundings, mood, seating position, and so on. These bits can serve as retrieval cues that you can later use to access the information. The more retrieval cues you have, the better your chances of finding a route to the suspended memory.

image For an 8-minute synopsis of how we access what’s stored in our brain, see LaunchPad’s Video: Memory Retrieval, below.

The best retrieval cues come from associations we form at the time we encode a memory—smells, tastes, and sights that can evoke our memory of the associated person or event. To call up visual cues when trying to recall something, we may mentally place ourselves in the original context. After losing his sight, British scholar John Hull (1990, p. 174) described his difficulty recalling such details:

I knew I had been somewhere, and had done particular things with certain people, but where? I could not put the conversations . . . into a context. There was no background, no features against which to identify the place. Normally, the memories of people you have spoken to during the day are stored in frames which include the background.

priming the activation, often unconsciously, of particular associations in memory.

PRIMING Often our associations are activated without our awareness. Philosopher-psychologist William James referred to this process, which we call priming, as the “wakening of associations.” After seeing or hearing rabbit, we are later more likely to spell the spoken word “hair/hare” as h-a-r-e, even if we don’t recall seeing or hearing rabbit (FIGURE 8.13).

image
Figure 8.12: FIGURE 8.13  Priming—awakening associations After seeing or hearing rabbit, we are later more likely to spell the spoken word “hair/hare” as h-a-r-e (Bower, 1986). Associations unconsciously activate related associations. This process is called priming.

298

Priming is often “memoryless memory”—invisible memory, without your conscious awareness. If, walking down a hallway, you see a poster of a missing child, you may then unconsciously be primed to interpret an ambiguous adult-child interaction as a possible kidnapping (James, 1986). Although you no longer have the poster in mind, it predisposes your interpretation.

Priming can influence behaviors as well (Herring et al., 2013). In one study, participants primed with money-related words were less likely to help another person when asked (Vohs et al., 2006). In another, people primed with money words or images expressed more support for free-market capitalism and social inequality (Caruso et al., 2013). In such cases, money may prime our materialism and self-interest rather than the social norms that encourage us to help (Ariely, 2009).

Ask a friend two rapid-fire questions: (a) How do you pronounce the word spelled by the letters s-h-o-p? (b) What do you do when you come to a green light? If your friend answers “stop” to the second question, you have demonstrated priming.

CONTEXT-DEPENDENT MEMORY Have you noticed? Putting yourself back in the context where you experienced something, such as in a childhood home or neighborhood, can prime your memory retrieval. When scuba divers listened to a word list in two different settings (either 10 feet underwater or sitting on the beach), they recalled more words if tested in the same place (Godden & Baddeley, 1975).

By contrast, experiencing something outside the usual setting can be confusing. Have you ever run into your doctor in an unusual place, such as at the store or park? You knew the person but struggled to figure out who it was and how you were acquainted? Our memories depend on context, and on the cues we have associated with that context.

In several experiments, Carolyn Rovee-Collier (1993) found that a familiar context could activate memories even in 3-month-olds. After infants learned that kicking would make a crib mobile move (via a connecting ribbon from the ankle), the infants kicked more when tested again in the same crib than when in a different context.

STATE-DEPENDENT MEMORY Closely related to context-dependent memory is state-dependent memory. What we learn in one state—be it drunk or sober—may be more easily recalled when we are again in that state. What people learn when drunk they don’t recall well in any state (alcohol disrupts storage). But they recall it slightly better when again drunk. Someone who hides money when drunk may forget the location until drunk again.

mood-congruent memory the tendency to recall experiences that are consistent with one’s current good or bad mood.

Our mood states provide an example of memory’s state dependence. Emotions that accompany good or bad events become retrieval cues (Gaddy & Ingram, 2014). Thus, our memories are somewhat mood congruent. If you’ve had a bad evening—your date never showed, your Toledo Mud Hens hat disappeared, your TV went out 10 minutes before the end of a show—your gloomy mood may facilitate recalling other bad times. Being depressed sours memories by priming negative associations, which we then use to explain our current mood. In many experiments, people put in a buoyant mood—whether under hypnosis or just by the day’s events (a World Cup soccer victory for German participants in one study)—have recalled the world through rose-colored glasses (DeSteno et al., 2000; Forgas et al., 1984; Schwarz et al., 1987). They recall their behaviors as competent and effective, other people benevolent, happy events more frequent.

image
The New Yorker Collection, 2005 David Sipress from cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved.

Knowing this mood-memory connection, we should not be surprised that in some studies, currently depressed people have recalled their parents as rejecting, punitive, and guilt promoting, whereas formerly depressed people’s recollections more closely resembled the more positive descriptions given by those who never suffered depression (Lewinsohn & Rosenbaum, 1987; Lewis, 1992). Similarly, adolescents’ ratings of parental warmth in one week gave little clue to how they would rate their parents six weeks later (Bornstein et al., 1991). When teens were down, their parents seemed inhuman; as their mood brightened, their parents morphed from devils into angels. We may nod our heads knowingly. Yet, in a good or bad mood, we persist in attributing to reality our own changing judgments, memories, and interpretations. In a bad mood, we may read someone’s look as a glare and feel even worse. In a good mood, we may encode the same look as interest and feel even better. Passions exaggerate.

299

“When a feeling was there, they felt as if it would never go; when it was gone, they felt as if it had never been; when it returned, they felt as if it had never gone.”

George MacDonald, What’s Mine’s Mine, 1886

Mood effects on retrieval help explain why our moods persist. When happy, we recall happy events and therefore see the world as a happy place, which helps prolong our good mood. When depressed, we recall sad events, which darkens our interpretations of current events. For those of us with a predisposition to depression, this process can help maintain a vicious, dark cycle. Moods magnify.

serial position effect our tendency to recall best the last (recency effect) and first (primacy effect) items in a list.

SERIAL POSITION EFFECT Another memory-retrieval quirk, the serial position effect, explains why we may have large holes in our memory of a list of recent events. Imagine it’s your first day in a new job, and your manager is introducing co-workers. As you meet each person, you silently repeat everyone’s name, starting from the beginning. As the last person smiles and turns away, you feel confident you’ll be able to greet your new co-workers by name the next day.

Don’t count on it. Because you have spent more time rehearsing the earlier names than the later ones, those are the names you’ll probably recall more easily the next day. In experiments, when people viewed a list of items (words, names, dates, even odors) and immediately tried to recall them in any order, they fell prey to the serial position effect (Reed, 2000). They briefly recalled the last items especially quickly and well (a recency effect), perhaps because those last items were still in working memory. But after a delay, when their attention was elsewhere, their recall was best for the first items (a primacy effect; see FIGURE 8.14).

image
FIGURE 8.14  The serial position effect Immediately after Pope Francis made his way through this receiving line of special guests, he would probably have recalled the names of the last few people best (recency effect). But later he may have been able to recall the first few people best (primacy effect).
Vincenzo Pinto/AFP/Getty Images

RETRIEVE IT

Question

9d1au1mDv8kP3tyaNARQIkh0nVXUmjRgb6S1gRgeWqnAB2qmpcDZro6UpsZTZBGtuoLX3PXh7SfnL2YCLV404A==
ANSWER: Priming is the activation (often without our awareness) of associations. Seeing a gun, for example, might temporarily predispose someone to interpret an ambiguous face as threatening or to recall a boss as nasty.

Question

When we are tested immediately after viewing a list of words, we tend to recall the first and last items best, which is known as the kb6fnVD684d8CGC/moKHx8gJdws= effect.

REVIEW Storing and Retrieving Memories

300

Learning Objectives

Test Yourself by taking a moment to answer each of these Learning Objective Questions (repeated here from within the chapter). Research suggests that trying to answer these questions on your own will improve your long-term memory of the concepts (McDaniel et al., 2009).

Question

h4L+7q4x+bFpCmABNFrcLCNFpCb0lkoA1zqe9KFebSUOl+FrbnH6ounobg4YjsCUZqXn/Zhx4BmHemHL7bQOxv0xAgozFtACrjkIda6X3Ds/T6Kyr3OCCYN4dR1pAM5IC9MS6pDM3tUftXyzI+GqXwO0QodeGtNSFxaMcMqtu19LAJGz1bhqmIG+EiDW5Nf3KhhW25VgSOMSBsl+OqIb/sYdVk/Oj0W+ML/mkrhHI+2rra/0ylg1B30TDvpnmrEcScEICp5E53TIsVb3Bl/U8qJRKhe9pdYZ276qXvztGIxLdDhfa9mN1nUSdbbv8VOl2gI1SZSi/2TXEaQ1BH4zyIl9Lsu190KwXuV0asOgfdrhV10xnk0lAcKhcaD+A78LWaRzIEbMiWy04KsIPXayBB0lUvO+QxM+ENXUd0p8qmk=
ANSWER: Our long-term memory capacity is essentially unlimited. Memories are not stored intact in the brain in single spots. Many parts of the brain interact as we encode, store, and retrieve memories.

Question

74NWwVjBivcj/IPZgwuJvifpjUOOJG8SYMnRyDyQlYpjhgxfKkQnoeHKDpjO7h9Mp3FTK3irgVrgZcyIazfd6pOqpNKzyg0R/z2L8zPGGkCYj/6Fx52C606hI0Ga800Hwo7kwDucuWnGF28NL00cJ9XxHDHeKt2BSaWw14NTp7sr//FY0YbFlRiDUU9E7AqT0/hKwaobrZEAoaZ/j1AJ6jI09fJk7kqH7rfUVkNr+OSvfi+p9/Om/CRYEoanwD/R
ANSWER: The frontal lobes and hippocampus are parts of the brain network dedicated to explicit memory formation. Many brain regions send information to the frontal lobes for processing. The hippocampus, with the help of surrounding areas of cortex, registers and temporarily holds elements of explicit memories (which are either semantic or episodic) before moving them to other brain regions for long-term storage. The neural storage of long-term memories is called memory consolidation.

Question

+cQz2777YwN91+EbZy8lJY1ggV1jAAbdiYb68vqnjavLPmviq8WKSvjWDKnLTFSs9EOBt+ngCtGFw88LTxZfBMytRWUsC777Xa6JUQ+A6nf1ldHXavvIX5URGKMt5iPQ3irf1owvojZYyFYNmhMSkab3dfqIN8OAWt96VipBlEHHRcOFLMOUEPhQdcZqMfGDEG2mh1qjj2KFyoFtOkSNBCstwsgVRvWqyuEuqVzIIMkm6VHrn4XUtbtG5vWqdDrQ
ANSWER: The cerebellum and basal ganglia are parts of the brain network dedicated to implicit memory formation. The cerebellum is important for storing classically conditioned memories. The basal ganglia are involved in motor movement and help form procedural memories for skills. Many reactions and skills learned during our first three years continue into our adult lives, but we cannot consciously remember learning these associations and skills, a phenomenon psychologists call infantile amnesia.

Question

X+SPDdj1YuRm2DbUQ5oUqRs7GSF86RwW2gGmxdMV6MuDCJ0ulN5XzCsqeJoLV5srWaClDkWRlpc6nAiXAguB8Kjq+bJUxzwG3Oic6KbylCel/tM4m1qjqhL1/3WtYg09pWEbwJXALuyRAra+BphKMAxUZUL2vxXV8rKIc7wSMusvN/QPIqqOC2DFDQqd5Tw10ZQSKEMO8hgiRwV68RZKEw==
ANSWER: Emotional arousal causes an outpouring of stress hormones, which lead to activity in the brain's memory-forming areas. Significantly stressful events can trigger very clear flashbulb memories.

Question

5dX+Q+9Fu0BJg4PUNxodqAU+pXKPjTyvQm76mBjJJnSQJDzBhFfdoQSO/Ev4/qXXoI2uRPbopD4GwvvzSKmXVQmI74bunDBPo6Qt2EMsMkoRX3MMeqx/XG7Tkijkls7Dh6Ug1l9rCuJ+kS/4yqXrfYvmSdqjDxnoy3DBibUExU01naPweK9e67QatdJKqNEIrPjpZUUCy0Lol+Q7k4VAHVZ7i5+0UDkXcjamXa+ddQ++GZQG
ANSWER: Long-term potentiation (LTP) appears to be the neural basis of learning. In LTP, neurons become more efficient at releasing and sensing the presence of neurotransmitters, and more connections develop between neurons.

Question

3GorBgzZrxmpmty9GkfjBxNNZgKVFtdAs5k0m1uZkXCgvl0NEaVnGcfcoAbOH6cUUWH5uqh+oY0bb2PoU9BAm0jyVu0L2blF0oXV6V1mCxt2adfKi/T1UpJZkR6yLRC/sxeCcs+amKSS6IzIXx6qn6CTmhhBwGdQ0Uqk/x49PB3OJdJf/J+Tcd7Diwr9UYsSJ/AvVpAJYPjS8OjqrDJy8dHQ6Xin3cjqVH4lTjVym36ZkT+a9/dA1zlsN8Zuq+F2VbPXpWxoRAe2CJJiX4Difw==
ANSWER: External cues activate associations that help us retrieve memories; this process may occur without our awareness, as it does in priming. Returning to the same physical context or emotional state (mood congruency) in which we formed a memory can help us retrieve it. The serial position effect accounts for our tendency to recall best the last items (which may still be in working memory) and the first items (which we've spent more time rehearsing) in a list.

Terms and Concepts to Remember

Test yourself on these terms.

Question

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

Experience the Testing Effect

Test yourself repeatedly throughout your studies. This will not only help you figure out what you know and don’t know; the testing itself will help you learn and remember the information more effectively thanks to the testing effect.

Question 8.7

BEMs2jwlf5PS9hqc9bgmf60iFlEj807aJvMMgaEPJ9o5GqnT+TAdQDwiZw0Ysx5PsW6UU8e4zN30NPRBb3i4XAazFGO4++LTCEw/mC8z6dcrMnAwuQFwg5vRCNJL1QLe6+dR+nekVKpIhq/3TdinLCcZ9HjIa68FXqsoNAdjLemD9MJyVbeI8fnZ4+hfCZxMqXZ33UF2kvyUuZC877jXgD+XKGSuLKBanCjxnSi94JsrGYx5vJFtwOWmqonrFDY7aHGbcH9jXwVkNrQZhiqE0eCbN40GETROWzztIj3n1LxkikQhXsNE0mwYHxcGj/ROzHDm/h6tFY7h3VulFsCrEUnxI5+c6XmjS9g+MnPM27UUpOESOpl/ubGEaRVchxws66nAF6X1VO8YRPxhrvd0mAuzyTFvRaZEbyqdJELKbI+ajQvS1luZ1N4CX6KSNrPdstJ1Q9/qC8K6TDs00rugMg==

Question 8.8

2. Hippocampus damage typically leaves people unable to learn new facts or recall recent events. However, they may be able to learn new skills, such as riding a bicycle, which is an dU7oeVLigOsooOD/BszTWw== (explicit/implicit) memory.

Question 8.9

1bEKF0Ct4/1Wx6sVcZGL1uL1JciOf+f8LnfV1buG+aK9S/JFwygG96kZgAwaJC5ci7cNmREUmKHsTOd5Br4N2hK+OSClv5OdhlZ3lkOhzoyZUezPzekcNMwhbIghvAuL9jJLHGv9fukflYG4yOkP1xuEgWhMlchCRw+qZtWCfpxSIiXCW8IwfN28sQhpg/Qs24tA9YMNPVeB14ztr5BGXH5OJ2ooSGZUmDsugDsUh8EcUfo1zPuTemg+SS4aXUN3x4zbmJp//jxaszUXEyBBqyATNe1++9CxNMLAuttleLvUf/YLtUucZPDRopqFsprbrhq8b0YcXu/2fQPLrigM8c2e1sCl6xsK1hMlBoLLNtNiEiMTDxJPgFOOT6frwvSBlaC39DCcf0acTNT32Xk0x+xfNQPIIH7j

Question 8.10

4. Specific odors, visual images, emotions, or other associations that help us access a memory are examples of xtjiZTSXwMfr6CzDYGTi4/qHtg0= .

Question 8.11

BieR/isp8F2Pt2xZpAvGNlrhTwSvrPcYS/iFu2FSgaWyhuHHKfjCxSR1vO+MFyZHArXVHIiAlh3dFwgBGjE54jlVMRKHDvR2Od025S9zSYhTkMynEhsndynlx8CPzstdUrlpnWaxWlvyhYcSOEwIB9XdiCrWamP5jSLIYsjaGoRZ2JGzl1PVNaNVFDws3I1thWIpEmtwfysB+VwapezMYTy/AA5TOdli6vvrKtRXiM2XkpCcQWfajvFfAP8uLNYn
ANSWER: Memories are stored within a web of many associations, one of which is mood. When you recall happy moments from your past, you deliberately activate these positive links. You may then experience mood-congruent memory and recall other happy moments, which could improve your mood and brighten your interpretation of current events.

Question 8.12

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

Use image to create your personalized study plan, which will direct you to the resources that will help you most in image .