Psychologists arm their scientific attitude with the scientific method—
2-
theory an explanation using an integrated set of principles that organizes observations and predicts behaviors or events.
In everyday conversation, we often use theory to mean “mere hunch.” Someone might, for example, discount evolution as “only a theory”—as if it were mere speculation. In science, a theory explains behaviors or events by offering ideas that organize what we have observed. By organizing isolated facts, a theory simplifies. By linking facts with deeper principles, a theory offers a useful summary. As we connect the observed dots, a coherent picture emerges.
A theory about sleep effects on memory, for example, helps us organize countless sleep-
hypothesis a testable prediction, often implied by a theory.
Yet no matter how reasonable a theory may sound—
Our theories can bias our observations. Having theorized that better memory springs from more sleep, we may see what we expect: We may perceive sleepy people’s comments as less insightful. The urge to see what we expect is ever-
operational definition a carefully worded statement of the exact procedures (operations) used in a research study. For example, human intelligence may be operationally defined as what an intelligence test measures.
replication repeating the essence of a research study, usually with different participants in different situations, to see whether the basic finding can be reproduced.
As a check on their biases, psychologists report their research with precise operational definitions of procedures and concepts. Sleep deprived, for example, might be defined as “X hours less” than one’s natural sleep. Using these carefully worded statements, others can replicate (repeat) the original observations with different participants, materials, and circumstances. If they get similar results, confidence in the finding’s reliability grows. The first study of hindsight bias aroused psychologists’ curiosity. Now, after many successful replications with different people and questions, we feel sure of the phenomenon’s power. Although “mere replications” of others’ research are unglamorous—
18
In the end, our theory will be useful if it (1) organizes observations and (2) implies predictions that anyone can use to check the theory or to derive practical applications. (Does people’s sleep predict their retention?) Eventually, our research may (3) stimulate further research that leads to a revised theory that better organizes and predicts.
For more information about statistical methods that psychological scientists use in their work, see Appendix A, Statistical Reasoning in Everyday Life.
As we will see next, we can test our hypotheses and refine our theories using descriptive methods (which describe behaviors, often through case studies, naturalistic observations, or surveys), correlational methods (which associate different factors), and experimental methods (which manipulate factors to discover their effects). To think critically about popular psychology claims, we need to understand these methods and know what conclusions they allow.
2-
The starting point of any science is description. In everyday life, we all observe and describe people, often drawing conclusions about why they act as they do. Professional psychologists do much the same, though more objectively and systematically, through
case studies (in-
naturalistic observations (recording individuals’ behavior in their natural setting).
surveys and interviews (self-
“‘Well my dear,’ said Miss Marple, ‘human nature is very much the same everywhere, and of course, one has opportunities of observing it at closer quarters in a village’.”
Agatha Christie, The Tuesday Club Murders, 1933
case study a descriptive technique in which one individual or group is studied in depth in the hope of revealing universal principles.
19
THE CASE STUDY Among the oldest research methods, the case study examines one individual or group in depth in the hope of revealing things true of us all. Some examples: Much of our early knowledge about the brain came from case studies of individuals who suffered a particular impairment after damage to a certain brain region. Jean Piaget taught us about children’s thinking after carefully observing and questioning only a few children. Studies of only a few chimpanzees revealed their capacity for understanding and language. Intensive case studies are sometimes very revealing, and they often suggest directions for further study.
See LaunchPad's Video: Case Studies for a helpful tutorial animation.
But atypical individual cases may mislead us. Both in our everyday lives and in science, unrepresentative information can lead to mistaken conclusions. Indeed, anytime a researcher mentions a finding (Smokers die younger: 95 percent of men over 85 are nonsmokers) someone is sure to offer a contradictory anecdote (Well, I have an uncle who smoked two packs a day and lived to be 89). Dramatic stories and personal experiences (even psychological case examples) command our attention and are easily remembered. Journalists understand that, and often begin their articles with personal stories. Stories move us. But stories can mislead. Which of the following do you find more memorable? (1) “In one study of 1300 dream reports concerning a kidnapped child, only 5 percent correctly envisioned the child as dead” (Murray & Wheeler, 1937). (2) “I know a man who dreamed his sister was in a car accident, and two days later she died in a head-
The point to remember: Individual cases can suggest fruitful ideas. What’s true of all of us can be glimpsed in any one of us. But to discern the general truths that cover individual cases, we must employ other research methods.
naturalistic observation a descriptive technique of observing and recording behavior in naturally occurring situations without trying to manipulate and control the situation.
NATURALISTIC OBSERVATION A second descriptive method records behavior in natural environments. These naturalistic observations range from watching chimpanzee societies in the jungle, to videotaping and analyzing parent-
Naturalistic observation has mostly been “small science”—science that can be done with pen and paper rather than fancy equipment and a big budget (Provine, 2012). But new technologies, such as smart-
20
See LaunchPad's Video: Naturalistic Observation for a helpful tutorial animation.
Like the case study, naturalistic observation does not explain behavior. It describes it. Nevertheless, descriptions can be revealing. We once thought, for example, that only humans use tools. Then naturalistic observation revealed that chimpanzees sometimes insert a stick in a termite mound and withdraw it, eating the stick’s load of termites. Such unobtrusive naturalistic observations paved the way for later studies of animal thinking, language, and emotion, which further expanded our understanding of our fellow animals. Thanks to researchers’ observations, we know that chimpanzees and baboons use deception: Psychologists repeatedly saw one young baboon pretending to have been attacked by another as a tactic to get its mother to drive the other baboon away from its food. “Observations, made in the natural habitat, helped to show that the societies and behavior of animals are far more complex than previously supposed,” chimpanzee observer Jane Goodall noted (1998).
Naturalistic observations also illuminate human behavior. Here are three findings you might enjoy:
A funny finding. We humans laugh 30 times more often in social situations than in solitary situations (Provine, 2001). (Have you noticed how seldom you laugh when alone?)
Sounding out students. What, really, are introductory psychology students saying and doing during their everyday lives? To find out, Matthias Mehl and James Pennebaker (2003) equipped 52 such students from the University of Texas with electronic recorders, which enabled the researchers to eavesdrop on more than 10,000 half-
Culture, climate, and the pace of life. Naturalistic observation also enabled Robert Levine and Ara Norenzayan (1999) to compare the pace of life—
Naturalistic observation offers interesting snapshots of everyday life, but it does so without controlling for all the factors that may influence behavior. It’s one thing to observe the pace of life in various places, but another to understand what makes some people walk faster than others. Even so, the observation of natural everyday behavior is an important part of psychological science.
survey a descriptive technique for obtaining the self-
21
THE SURVEY A survey looks at many cases in less depth by asking people to report their behavior or opinions. Questions about everything from sexual practices to political opinions are put to the public. In recent surveys:
Saturdays and Sundays have been the week’s happiest days (confirming what the Twitter researchers found) (Stone et al., 2012).
1 in 5 people across 22 countries report believing that alien beings have come to Earth and now walk among us disguised as humans (Ipsos, 2010b).
68 percent of all humans—
But asking questions is tricky, and the answers often depend on question wording and respondent selection.
WORDING EFFECTS Even subtle changes in the order or wording of questions can have major effects. People are much more approving of “aid to the needy” than of “welfare,” of “affirmative action” than of “preferential treatment,” of “not allowing” televised cigarette ads and pornography than of “censoring” them, and of “revenue enhancers” than of “taxes.” Because wording is such a delicate matter, critical thinkers will reflect on how the phrasing of a question might affect people’s expressed opinions.
RANDOM SAMPLING In everyday thinking, we tend to generalize from cases we observe, especially vivid cases. Given (a) a statistical summary of a professor’s student evaluations and (b) the vivid comments of a biased sample (two irate students), an administrator’s impression of the professor may be influenced as much by the two unhappy students as by the many favorable evaluations in the statistical summary. The temptation to ignore the sampling bias and to generalize from a few vivid but unrepresentative cases is nearly irresistible.
population all those in a group being studied, from which samples may be drawn. (Note: Except for national studies, this does not refer to a country’s whole population.)
random sample a sample that fairly represents a population because each member has an equal chance of inclusion.
With very large samples, estimates become quite reliable. E is estimated to represent 12.7 percent of the letters in written English. E, in fact, is 12.3 percent of the 925,141 letters in Melville’s Moby-
So how do you obtain a representative sample of, say, the students at your college or university? It’s not always possible to survey the whole group you want to study and describe. How could you choose a group that would represent the total student population? Typically, you would seek a random sample, in which every person in the total group has an equal chance of being included in the sample group. You might number the names in the general student listing and then use a random number generator to pick your survey participants. (Sending each student a questionnaire wouldn’t work because the conscientious people who returned it would not be a random sample.) Large representative samples are better than small ones, but a small representative sample of 100 is better than an unrepresentative sample of 500.
Political pollsters sample voters in national election surveys just this way. Using some 1500 randomly sampled people, drawn from all areas of a country, they can provide a remarkably accurate snapshot of the nation’s opinions. Without random sampling (also called random selection), large samples—
The point to remember: Before accepting survey findings, think critically. Consider the sample. The best basis for generalizing is from a representative sample. You cannot compensate for an unrepresentative sample by simply adding more people.
22
2-
correlation a measure of the extent to which two factors vary together, and thus of how well either factor predicts the other.
correlation coefficient a statistical index of the relationship between two things (from –1.00 to +1.00).
See LaunchPad's Video: Correlational Studies for a helpful tutorial animation.
Describing behavior is a first step toward predicting it. Naturalistic observations and surveys often show us that one trait or behavior relates to another. In such cases, we say the two correlate. A statistical measure (the correlation coefficient) indicates how closely two things vary together, and thus how well either one predicts the other. Knowing how much aptitude test scores correlate with school success tells us how well the scores predict school success.
A positive correlation (above 0 to +1.00) indicates a direct relationship, meaning that two things increase together or decrease together. For example, height and weight are positively correlated.
A negative correlation (below 0 to −1.00) indicates an inverse relationship: As one thing increases, the other decreases. The weekly number of hours spent in TV watching and video gaming correlates negatively with grades. Negative correlations could go as low as −1.00, which means that, like people on opposite ends of a teeter-
Though informative, psychology’s correlations usually explain only part of the variation among individuals. As we will see, there is a positive correlation between parents’ abusiveness and their children’s later abusiveness when they become parents. But this does not mean that most abused children become abusive. The correlation simply indicates a statistical relationship: Most abused children do not grow into abusers, but nonabused children are even less likely to become abusive. Correlations point us toward predictions, but usually imperfect ones.
The point to remember: A correlation coefficient helps us see the world more clearly by revealing the extent to which two things relate.
Indicate whether each association is a positive correlation or a negative correlation.
For an animated tutorial on correlations, visit LaunchPad’s Concept Practice: Positive and Negative Correlations.
CORRELATION AND CAUSATION Consider some recent newsworthy correlations:
“Study finds that increased parental support for college results in lower grades” (Jaschik, 2013).
“People with mental illness more likely to be smokers, study finds” (Belluck, 2013).
“Teens who play mature-
What shall we make of these correlations? Do they indicate that students would achieve more if their parents supported them less? That stopping smoking would improve mental health? That abstaining from video games would make reckless teen drivers more responsible?
No, because such correlations do not come with built-
23
This point is so important—
The point to remember (turn the volume up here): Correlation does not prove causation. Correlation indicates the possibility of a cause-
Correlation need not mean causation.
2-
Recall that in a well-
Happy are they, remarked the Roman poet Virgil, “who have been able to perceive the causes of things.” How might psychologists perceive causes in correlational studies, such as the correlation between breast feeding and intelligence? Is breast really best?
Intelligence scores of children who were breast-
experiment a research method in which an investigator manipulates one or more factors (independent variables) to observe the effect on some behavior or mental process (the dependent variable). By random assignment of participants, the experimenter aims to control other relevant factors.
experimental group in an experiment, the group exposed to the treatment, that is, to one version of the independent variable.
control group in an experiment, the group not exposed to the treatment; contrasts with the experimental group and serves as a comparison for evaluating the effect of the treatment.
random assignment assigning participants to experimental and control groups by chance, thus minimizing preexisting differences between the -different groups.
What do such findings mean? Do the nutrients of mother’s milk, as some researchers believe, contribute to brain development? Or do smarter mothers have smarter children? (Breast-
24
To experiment with breast feeding, one research team randomly assigned some 17,000 Belarus newborns and their mothers either to a control group given normal pediatric care, or to an experimental group that promoted breast feeding, thus increasing expectant mothers’ breast-
With parental permission, one British research team directly experimented with breast milk. They randomly assigned 424 hospitalized premature infants either to formula feedings or to breast-
See LaunchPad's Video: Random Assignment for a helpful tutorial animation.
No single experiment is conclusive, of course. But randomly assigning participants to one feeding group or the other effectively eliminated all factors except nutrition. This supported the conclusion that for developing intelligence, breast is indeed best. If test performance changes when we vary infant nutrition, then we infer that nutrition matters.
The point to remember: Unlike correlational studies, which uncover naturally occurring relationships, an experiment manipulates a factor to determine its effect.
Consider, then, how we might assess therapeutic interventions. Our tendency to seek new remedies when we are ill or emotionally down can produce misleading testimonies. If three days into a cold we start taking vitamin C tablets and find our cold symptoms lessening, we may credit the pills rather than the cold naturally subsiding. In the 1700s, bloodletting seemed effective. People sometimes improved after the treatment; when they didn’t, the practitioner inferred the disease was too advanced to be reversed. So, whether or not a remedy is truly effective, enthusiastic users will probably endorse it. To determine its effect, we must control for other factors.
double-blind procedure an experimental procedure in which both the research participants and the research staff are ignorant (blind) about whether the research participants have received the treatment or a placebo. Commonly used in drug-
And that is precisely how investigators evaluate new drug treatments and new methods of psychological therapy. They randomly assign participants either to the group receiving a treatment (such as a medication), or to a group receiving a pseudotreatment—
placebo [pluh-
In double-
25
INDEPENDENT AND DEPENDENT VARIABLES Here is an even more potent example: The drug Viagra was approved for use after 21 clinical trials. One trial was an experiment in which researchers randomly assigned 329 men with erectile disorder to either an experimental group (Viagra takers) or a control group (placebo takers given an identical-
independent variable in an experiment, the factor that is manipulated; the variable whose effect is being studied.
confounding variable a factor other than the factor being studied that might produce an effect.
This simple experiment manipulated just one factor: the drug dosage (none versus peak dose). We call this experimental factor the independent variable because we can vary it independently of other factors, such as the men’s age, weight, and personality. Other factors which could influence a study’s results are called confounding variables. Random assignment controls for possible confounding variables.
dependent variable in an experiment, the outcome that is measured; the variable that may change when the independent variable is manipulated.
See two tutorial animations below: LaunchPad's Experiments and Confounding Variables.
Experiments examine the effect of one or more independent variables on some measurable behavior, called the dependent variable because it can vary depending on what takes place during the experiment. Both variables are given precise operational definitions, which specify the procedures that manipulate the independent variable (in this study, the exact drug dosage and timing) or measure the dependent variable (the questions that assessed the men’s responses). These definitions answer the “What do you mean?” question with a level of precision that enables others to replicate the study. (See FIGURE 2.4 for the British breast milk experiment’s design.)
Let’s pause to check your understanding using a simple psychology experiment: To test the effect of perceived ethnicity on the availability of rental housing, Adrian Carpusor and William Loges (2006) sent identically worded e-
“[We must guard] against not just racial slurs, but … against the subtle impulse to call Johnny back for a job interview, but not Jamal.”
Barack Obama, Eulogy for Clementa Pinckney, June 26, 2015
Experiments can also help us evaluate social programs. Do early childhood education programs boost impoverished children’s chances for success? What are the effects of different antismoking campaigns? Do school sex-
Let’s recap. A variable is anything that can vary (infant nutrition, intelligence, TV exposure—
TABLE 2.1 compares the features of psychology’s main research methods. You will read later about other research designs, including cross-
26
Research Method | Basic Purpose | How Conducted | What Is Manipulated | Weaknesses |
---|---|---|---|---|
Descriptive | To observe and record behavior | Do case studies, naturalistic observations, or surveys | Nothing | No control of variables; single cases may be misleading |
Correlational | To detect naturally occurring relationships; to assess how well one variable predicts another | Collect data on two or more variables; no manipulation | Nothing | Cannot specify cause and effect |
Experimental | To explore cause and effect | Manipulate one or more factors; use random assignment | The independent variable(s) | Sometimes not feasible; results may not generalize to other contexts; not ethical to manipulate certain variables |
Research Design: How Would You Know?
Throughout this book, you will read about amazing psychological science discoveries. But how do we know fact from fiction? How do psychological scientists choose research methods and design their studies in ways that provide meaningful results? Understanding how research is done—
In psychological research, no questions are off limits, except untestable ones. Does free will exist? Are people born evil? Is there an afterlife? Psychologists can’t test those questions, but they can test whether free will beliefs, aggressive personalities, and a belief in life after death influence how people think, feel, and act (Dechesne et al., 2003; Shariff et al., 2014; Webster et al., 2014).
To help you build your understanding, and your scientific literacy skills, we created IMMERSIVE LEARNING research activities in LaunchPad. In these How Would You Know activities, you get to play the role of the researcher, making choices about the best ways to test interesting questions, such as How Would You Know If Having Children Relates to Being Happier?, How Would You Know If a Cup of Coffee Can Warm Up Relationships?, and How Would You Know If People Can Learn to Reduce Anxiety?
Having chosen their question, psychologists then select the most appropriate research design—
Next, psychological scientists decide how to measure the behavior or mental process being studied. For example, consider the researchers mentioned earlier in this box, who tested whether aggressive personalities affect how people act. They measured aggression by determining participants’ willingness to blast a stranger with intense noise.
Researchers want to have confidence in their findings, so they carefully consider confounding variables—
Psychological research is a fun and creative adventure. The new Immersive Learning: How Would You Know? activities invite you to join the scientific journey to uncover new knowledge. We will both [DM and ND] encourage you via videos as you DESIGN each of your studies, MEASURE target behaviors, INTERPRET your results, and learn more about the fascinating process of scientific discovery along the way!
27
To review and test your understanding of experimental methods and concepts, visit LaunchPad’s Concept Practice: The Language of Experiments, and the interactive PsychSim 6: Understanding Psychological Research. For a 9.5-
By using random assignment, researchers are able to control for 0ZRuWpHgrpTl7/lAu3tnJuJ8b+HP15BCP1sxqg== , which are other factors besides the independent variable(s) that may influence research results.
Match the term on the left with the description on the right.
2-
When you see or hear about psychological research, do you ever wonder whether people’s behavior in the lab will predict their behavior in real life? Does detecting the blink of a faint red light in a dark room say anything useful about flying a plane at night? After viewing a violent, sexually explicit film, does an aroused man’s increased willingness to push buttons that he thinks will electrically shock a woman really say anything about whether violent pornography makes a man more likely to abuse a woman?
Before you answer, consider: The experimenter intends the laboratory environment to be a simplified reality—
An experiment’s purpose is not to re-
When psychologists apply laboratory research on aggression to actual violence, they are applying theoretical principles of aggressive behavior, principles refined through many experiments. Similarly, it is the principles of the visual system, developed from experiments in artificial settings (such as looking at red lights in the dark), that researchers apply to more complex behaviors such as night flying. And many investigations have demonstrated that principles derived in the laboratory do typically generalize to the everyday world (Anderson et al., 1999).
The point to remember: Psychological science focuses less on particular behaviors than on seeking general principles that help explain many behaviors.