16.7 Motivation and Emotion

Perceptions are also influenced, top-down, by our motivation and emotions.

Hearing sad rather than happy music can predispose people to perceive a sad meaning in spoken homophonic words—mourning rather than morning, die rather than dye, pain rather than pane (Halberstadt et al., 1996). After listening to irritating (and anger-cuing) music, people perceive a harmful action such as robbery as more serious (Seidel & Prinz, 2013). Dennis Proffitt (2006a, b; Schnall et al., 2008) and others have demonstrated the power of emotions with other clever experiments showing that

Motives also matter. Desired objects, such as a water bottle when thirsty, seem closer (Balcetis & Dunning, 2010). This perceptual bias energizes our going for it. Our motives also direct our perception of ambiguous stimuli.

“When you’re hitting the ball, it comes at you looking like a grapefruit. When you’re not, it looks like a black-eyed pea.”

Former major league baseball player George Scott

Emotions and motives color our social perceptions, too. People more often perceive solitary confinement, sleep deprivation, and cold temperatures as “torture” when experiencing a small dose of such themselves (Nordgren et al., 2011). Spouses who feel loved and appreciated perceive less threat in stressful marital events—“He’s just having a bad day” (Murray et al., 2003). The moral of these stories: To believe is, indeed, to see.