Perceptions are also influenced, top-
Hearing sad rather than happy music can predispose people to perceive a sad meaning in spoken homophonic words—
walking destinations look farther away to those fatigued by prior exercise.
a hill looks steeper to those who are wearing a heavy backpack or have just been exposed to sad, heavy classical music rather than light, bouncy music. As with so many of life’s challenges, a hill also seems less steep to those who feel others understand them (Oishi et al., 2013).
a target seems farther away to those throwing a heavy rather than a light object at it.
a softball appears bigger when you are hitting well. Jessica Witt and Proffitt (2005) observed this after asking players to choose a circle the size of the ball they had just hit well or poorly. (There’s also a reciprocal phenomenon: Seeing a target as bigger—
Motives also matter. Desired objects, such as a water bottle when thirsty, seem closer (Balcetis & Dunning, 2010). This perceptual bias energizes our going for it. Our motives also direct our perception of ambiguous stimuli.
“When you’re hitting the ball, it comes at you looking like a grapefruit. When you’re not, it looks like a black-
Former major league baseball player George Scott
Emotions and motives color our social perceptions, too. People more often perceive solitary confinement, sleep deprivation, and cold temperatures as “torture” when experiencing a small dose of such themselves (Nordgren et al., 2011). Spouses who feel loved and appreciated perceive less threat in stressful marital events—