5.8 CHAPTER REVIEW

Learning

KEY POINTS

Introduction: What Is Learning?

Classical Conditioning: Associating Stimuli

Contemporary Views of Classical Conditioning

Operant Conditioning: Associating Behaviors and Consequences

Contemporary Views of Operant Conditioning

Observational Learning: Imitating the Actions of Others

KEY TERMS

Match each of the terms on the left with its definition on the right. Click on the term first and then click on the matching definition. As you match them correctly they will move to the bottom of the activity.

Question

behavior modification
behaviorism
biological preparedness
classical conditioning
cognitive map
conditioned reinforcer
conditioned response (CR)
conditioned stimulus (CS)
conditioning
continuous reinforcement
discriminative stimulus
extinction (in classical conditioning)
extinction (in operant conditioning)
fixed-interval (FI) schedule
fixed-ratio (FR) schedule
higher order conditioning
instinctive drift
latent learning
law of effect
learned helplessness
learning
mirror neurons
negative punishment
negative reinforcement
observational learning
operant
operant chamber or Skinner box
operant conditioning
partial reinforcement
partial reinforcement effect
placebo response
positive punishment
positive reinforcement
primary reinforcer
punishment
reinforcement
schedule of reinforcement
shaping
spontaneous recovery
stimulus discrimination
stimulus generalization
taste aversion
unconditioned response (UCR)
unconditioned stimulus (UCS)
variable-interval (VI) schedule
variable-ratio (VR) schedule
The gradual weakening and disappearance of conditioned behavior. In operant conditioning, extinction occurs when an emitted behavior is no longer followed by a reinforcer.
The application of learning principles to help people develop more effective or adaptive behaviors.
The gradual weakening and apparent disappearance of conditioned behavior. In classical conditioning, extinction occurs when the conditioned stimulus is repeatedly presented without the unconditioned stimulus.
The presentation of a stimulus or event following a behavior that acts to decrease the likelihood of the behavior being repeated.
Tolman's term for the mental representation of the layout of a familiar environment.
The basic learning process that involves changing the probability that a response will be repeated by manipulating the consequences of that response.
A specific stimulus in the presence of which a particular response is more likely to be reinforced, and in the absence of which a particular response is not likely to be reinforced.
The occurrence of a learned response to a specific stimulus but not to other, similar stimuli.
A reinforcement schedule in which a reinforcer is delivered for the first response that occurs after a preset time interval has elapsed.
A classically conditioned dislike for and avoidance of a particular food that develops when an organism becomes ill after eating the food.
Skinner's term for an actively emitted (or voluntary) behavior that operates on the environment to produce consequences.
The tendency of an animal to revert to instinctive behaviors that can interfere with the performance of an operantly conditioned response.
Learning principle, proposed by Thorndike, in which responses followed by a satisfying effect become strengthened and are more likely to recur in a particular situation, while responses followed by a dissatisfying effect are weakened and less likely to recur in a particular situation.
The natural stimulus that reflexively elicits a response without the need for prior learning.
A reinforcement schedule in which a reinforcer is delivered for the first response that occurs after an average time interval, which varies unpredictably from trial to trial.
A procedure in which a conditioned stimulus from one learning trial functions as the unconditioned stimulus in a new conditioning trial; the second conditioned stimulus comes to elicit the conditioned response, even though it has never been directly paired with the unconditioned stimulus.
In learning theory, the idea that an organism is innately predisposed to form associations between certain stimuli and responses.
The unlearned, reflexive response that is elicited by an unconditioned stimulus.
A stimulus or event that is naturally or inherently reinforcing for a given species, such as food, water, or other biological necessities.
The occurrence of a learned response not only to the original stimulus but to other, similar stimuli as well.
The experimental apparatus invented by B. F. Skinner to study the relationship between environmental events and active behaviors.
A situation in which a response is followed by the addition of a reinforcing stimulus, increasing the likelihood that the response will be repeated in similar situations.
The basic learning process that involves repeatedly pairing a neutral stimulus with a response-producing stimulus until the neutral stimulus elicits the same response.
A situation in which an operant is followed by the presentation or addition of an aversive stimulus; also called punishment by application.
The learned, reflexive response to a conditioned stimulus.
The process of learning associations between environmental events and behavioral responses.
The reappearance of a previously extinguished conditioned response after a period of time without exposure to the conditioned stimulus.
A situation in which a response results in the removal of, avoidance of, or escape from a punishing stimulus, increasing the likelihood that the response will be repeated in similar situations.
An individual's psychological and physiological response to what is actually a fake treatment or drug; also called placebo effect.
Learning that occurs through observing the actions of others.
Neurons that activate both when an action is performed and when the same action is perceived.
A reinforcement schedule in which a reinforcer is delivered after a fixed number of responses has occurred.
A situation in which the occurrence of a particular response is only sometimes followed by a reinforcer.
School of psychology and theoretical viewpoint that emphasizes the study of observable behaviors, especially as they pertain to the process of learning.
The delivery of a reinforcer according to a preset pattern based on the number of responses or the time interval between responses.
Tolman's term for learning that occurs in the absence of reinforcement but is not behaviorally demonstrated until a reinforcer becomes available.
A phenomenon in which exposure to inescapable and uncontrollable aversive events produces passive behavior.
The occurrence of a stimulus or event following a response that increases the likelihood of that response being repeated.
The operant conditioning procedure of selectively reinforcing successively closer approximations of a goal behavior until the goal behavior is displayed.
A formerly neutral stimulus that acquires the capacity to elicit a reflexive response.
A schedule of reinforcement in which every occurrence of a particular response is followed by a reinforcer.
A reinforcement schedule in which a reinforcer is delivered after an average number of responses, which varies unpredictably from trial to trial.
A process that produces a relatively enduring change in behavior or knowledge as a result of past experience.
A situation in which an operant is followed by the removal or subtraction of a reinforcing stimulus; also called punishment by removal.
A stimulus or event that has acquired reinforcing value by being associated with a primary reinforcer; also called a secondary reinforcer.
The phenomenon in which behaviors that are conditioned using partial reinforcement are more resistant to extinction than behaviors that are conditioned using continuous reinforcement.

KEY PEOPLE

Albert Bandura (b. 1925) American psychologist who experimentally investigated observational learning, emphasizing the role of cognitive factors. (p. 215)

John Garcia (b. 1917) American psychologist who experimentally demonstrated the learning of taste aversions in animals, a finding that challenged several basic assumptions of classical conditioning. (p. 194)

Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936) Russian physiologist who first described the basic learning process of associating stimuli that is now called classical conditioning. (p. 183)

Robert A. Rescorla (b. 1940) American psychologist who experimentally demonstrated the involvement of cognitive processes in classical conditioning. (p. 192)

Martin Seligman (b. 1942) American psychologist who is best known for his theory of learned helplessness and for founding the modern positive psychology movement (see Chapter 1). (pp. 195, 212)

B. F. Skinner (1904–1990) American psychologist who developed the operant conditioning model of learning; emphasized studying the relationship between environmental factors and observable actions, not mental processes, in trying to achieve a scientific explanation of behavior. (p. 197)

Edward L. Thorndike (1874–1949) American psychologist who was the first to experimentally study animal behavior and document how active behaviors are influenced by their consequences; postulated the law of effect. (p. 196)

Edward C. Tolman (1898–1956) American psychologist who used the terms cognitive map and latent learning to describe experimental findings that strongly suggested that cognitive factors play a role in animal learning. (p. 210)

John B. Watson (1878–1958) American psychologist who, in the early 1900s, founded behaviorism, an approach that emphasizes the scientific study of outwardly observable behavior rather than subjective mental states. (p. 188)

[Leave] [Close]