Chapter 9. Learning Environments

9.1 Welcome

Think Like a Scientist
true
dependent variable
The factor that is observed and measured for change in an experiment, thought to be influenced by the independent variable.
negative correlation
A finding that two factors vary systematically in opposite directions, one increasing as the other decreases.
independent variable
The purposely manipulated factor thought to produce change in an experiment.

Think Like a Scientist

Learning Environments

By:

Susan A. Nolan, Seton Hall University

Sandra E. Hockenbury

REFERENCES

Fisher, Anna V.; Godwin, Karrie E.; & Seltman, Howard. (2014). Visual environment, attention allocation, and learning in young children: When too much of a good thing may be bad. Psychological Science, 25, 1362–1370 doi:0956797614533801.

Mashburn, Andrew. J.; Pianta, Robert C.; Hamre, Bridget K.; Downer, Jason. T.; Barbarin, Oscar A.; Bryant, Donna; . . . & Howes, Carollee. (2008). Measures of classroom quality in prekindergarten and children’s development of academic, language, and social skills. Child Development, 79, 732–749.

FAQ

What is Think Like a Scientist?
Think Like a Scientist is a digital activity designed to help you develop your scientific thinking skills. Each activity places you in a different, real-world scenario, asking you to think critically about a specific claim.

Can instructors track your progress in Think Like a Scientist?
Scores from the five-question assessments at the end of each activity can be reported to your instructor. To ensure your privacy while participating in non-assessment features, which can include pseudoscientific quizzes or games, no other student response is saved or reported.

How is Think Like a Scientist aligned with the APA Guidelines 2.0?
The American Psychological Association’s “Guidelines for the Undergraduate Psychology Major” provides a set of learning goals for students. Think Like a Scientist addresses several of these goals, although it is specifically designed to develop skills from APA Goal 2: Scientific Inquiry and Critical Thinking.
“Learning Environments” covers many outcomes, including:

  • Use scientific reasoning to interpret psychological phenomena: Use psychology concepts to explain personal experiences and recognize the potential for flaws in behavioral explanations based on simplistic, personal theories [consider “common sense” as source]
  • Demonstrate psychology information literacy: Interpret simple graphs and statistical findings [examine research findings in a graph]

9.2 Introduction

This activity invites you to test the claim that highly decorated classrooms are the best learning environments for kindergarten students. First, you will describe the ideal kindergarten classroom. Then you will explore evidence from an experiment comparing the effects of different environments on learning. You will also examine alternative explanations for the study’s findings, questioning whether there may be situations in which the findings may not apply. Finally, you will explore the role of scientific research in examining conclusions based on “common sense.”

9.3 Identify the Claim

1

Identify the Claim

9.3.1 The Ideal Classroom Decor

This is a kindergarten classroom designed as the ideal classroom for children to learn.
edeCandoniPhoto/Shutterstock

Do you remember your kindergarten classroom? Teachers spend countless hours transforming empty classrooms into engaging environments for their students. Pretend you’re a new kindergarten teacher. The first day of school is approaching and you have to decorate your empty classroom. Imagine that you have been assigned the room shown here.

Question 9.1

Ff1AOsA2ZcZ+JXgpjdzknXb/jWxWDqcupEyP0vNALNFPL1TAb0azhWpzCHUxuDAXgVa0ZsuFyRL7xMrToYG3UmB0C0cd6biknj2+/mHhU7hDBYPUtlBZYZOY76VnHb7tIaW3m5kBYV8ghFrKK24OxwPehLLhE+yHZePxwQah6S0NbsRE1PZPlRUWLVJP4GUdHCtrFP1FPJZzfOvIers72k/YgjrMGMOrL2enLNQOnG3JgXyYP7Q+LfJoS3VeJLmwWPlTC0UD9RTIfXKu1ck8X8agPI9S0PRWtjpRB6E310KjiqY467xQnUCityg/vkxpqUv6aq6Qxws=
You’ll soon get a chance to make some decisions based on your ideas about the best learning environment.

9.3.2 Messages to Teachers

School administrators, teachers, and parents see lots of information describing what a good classroom will look like. Educational supply stores offer vast arrays of classroom-decorating materials, and there are many teacher-created Pinterest boards and blogs dedicated to classroom decoration. Look at this mock Web site, developed from actual Web sites on classroom design. (You can click the image to enlarge.) Below the site, we’ll ask you to identify the claim this site is making.

A mock website that claims a decorated classroom helps students learn under optimal conditions.
Myrleen Pearson/Alamy, i studio / Alamy, Maria Dryfhout / Shutterstock, Diego Cervo / age fotostock, Nikolay Solidcreature / Shutterstock, Jamie Pham Photography / Alamy

Question

ClrQdu46CGCggBDjWe2VYlgZgTzz0V1m2Nl3Jra8Kvj4AXrnb1gPvualmGwhez9026sRfLCwDLu2Ryt690jiZbT5pmXDisVNkUelsyZQKiziaJ/58OeWuXR2+/8=
The Web site claims that a decorated classroom will help students learn more than if the classroom were not decorated. This claim seems like “common sense,” and the site does not provide any research to justify it. For most of us, it just seems that a decorated classroom would help students learn best. Sometimes the claims that researchers test come from common sense or from observations about the world around them.

9.3.3 Choose Your Ideal Classroom

chapter_9_choose_classroom

Look at these photos showing different decorating schemes for a kindergarten classroom. Based on your ideas about the ideal classroom environment, click the circle next to the classroom you think offers the best learning environment for your students.

FedeCandoniPhoto/Shutterstock
Spaces Images / Alamy
Aping Vision / STS/Getty Images
krsmanovic/Shutterstock
edeCandoniPhoto/Shutterstock
Peet Simard/Corbis

Question

q3iE+B0yUKAytK210agTWX7eowOsX/pk30AbAlOATK4Vmxy53DDG6ICR69fSGzbX5UAAb1Z5RJD7y7ldA9loJPHUM4u9903j39NSn3ODoXuETOrcGZEQJggJjMA9e17yfw6oGwGRIFJ700HxTV8kdsp0pAm2qhLFFMPB6ZHGZEDGe34EJdMXudsKiRc1dRKdeDmmfJkjkghhv/APCPGbwZPz2GVkGI5CNf+nYiADRofYybSkHYcGGOuU/Nbm8rxprKdvNJB3Ouydz0W5CODoVpYHH7s=
Like most people, you clicked one of the decorated classrooms, and not one of the undecorated classrooms. Most people think that a highly decorated classroom is ideal for kindergarten students. This matches the claim that we identified previously.

9.4 Evaluate the Evidence

2

Evaluate the Evidence

9.4.1 Studying the Impact of Classrooms on Learning

“Common sense” might feel accurate, but it can and should be scientifically tested. Like most of us, psychologist Anna Fisher and her colleagues noted that the typical kindergarten classroom is highly decorated (2014). In this way, kindergarten classrooms are different from classrooms for older students, like the one shown here. The researchers described a negative correlation between student age and classroom décor: the younger the student, the more decorations. Fisher decided to explore the effect of that typical classroom environment on student learning.

A photo shows an undecorated kindergarten classroom used for the study to understand the learning abilities of the children in the class.
Peter Noyce PLB / Alamy

9.4.2 Test the Claim

Does a decorated classroom help or harm the learning ability of kindergarten students? To find out, Dr. Fisher and her colleagues had to see whether the same students learned the same material differently in different environments. In their experiment, students were taught science in a classroom that was either highly decorated or sparsely decorated, like those shown here. (A) shows the “decorated-classroom condition,” and (B) shows the bare “sparse-classroom condition.” Students were taught the same lesson in both types of classroom. Type of classroom, then, was the independent variable.

A photo shows a highly decorated kindergarten classroom used for the study to understand the learning abilities of children in the class.
(A) Jamie Pham Photography / Alamy
A photo shows an undecorated kindergarten classroom used for the study to understand the learning abilities of the children in the class.
(B) Jamie Pham Photography / Alamy

9.4.3 Learning Science in Two Different Environments

All students in the experiment experienced three science lessons in each classroom. In one lesson, the instructor read aloud from a book with information about insects, pausing to show students the illustrations. The page shown here is similar to what the students would have seen.

Three illustrations of insects has text that reads “Insects have different ways of communicating with each other. Honeybees do a dance. Fireflies flash light. Grasshoppers rub their legs against their wings to make a sound. But all insects have three body parts and six legs.”

9.4.4 Assessing Students’ Learning in Two Different Environments

After each lesson, students were tested on their learning. For example, they were asked to circle the picture of an insect on a sheet like the one shown here. Test scores in each classroom condition was the dependent variable. The researchers wanted to know whether students’ test scores depended on whether they experienced the lesson in the decorated classroom or the sparse classroom.

An illustration entitled “which picture shows an insect” has four different insects with different numbers of body parts and legs. This illustration tests the child’s learning ability in both decorated and undecorated classrooms.

Question

S/f1o6cvNKuZ2O4sEFTmPSADVyFk3rdkdl0IfdigqXG9/FRiuRNGoUonleuVaKdj9myws69kOK26z707PyOPF8ORtXoA/XbGGbtMdRlftpLz38dv90Wa7PqWs/GVObGSP6dgxTBuGXHyNWZd0qe/BsoTpZGKRN0ktZzqeIz5qlyMN88cRI5TTJvJER5JutawTASWe5yWpuJVYrubIjjoGdDmzy6yIBpAUxLQiKTJWbWgVAFMAPRgWmrj3iTOBS8EUmrVVR3lDgiCGPvguuKlTQ==
You guessed XX %. Your guess was (lower than, very close to, higher than) the actual percentage of 55%.

9.4.5 Explaining the Results

Did you guess that kindergarteners actually learn information better in sparse classrooms than in decorated classrooms? If so, you were right! As you can see in the graph here, when students learned in the sparse classroom, they answered an average of 55% of the questions correctly. When they learned in the decorated classroom, they answered fewer questions correctly, averaging only 42%. So, now we know that students tend to learn information better in the sparse classroom than in the decorated classroom. But we don’t yet know why. Thinking like a scientist involves asking follow-up questions to understand exactly what is going on.

So, let’s ask: Why did students learn less in the decorated classroom? Because distraction is a common problem for young children, the researchers also measured the time students spent “off task”—that is, on activities other than learning. They found that the kindergarteners were more likely to be distracted in the decorated classrooms, especially by things in the classroom that were unrelated to the current lesson. So, the researchers concluded that highly decorated classrooms decrease student learning by distracting them from the task—in this case, learning information about science.

A bar chart with vertical axis labelled as percent correct is marked from 0 to 100 percent with marked intervals of 20 percent. The blue colored bar from the horizontal axis labelled as sparse classroom raises up to 55 percent and the green colored bar labelled as decorated classroom raises up to 42 percent.

9.5 Consider Alternative Explanations

3

Consider Alternative Explanations

9.5.1 Are Sparse Classrooms <em>Always</em> Better?

This study was an experiment, so we can draw a causal conclusion. We have evidence that kindergarteners tend to perform better in sparse classrooms than in decorated classrooms. So why is the claim that decorated classrooms are best so pervasive? Let’s consider the type of learning. Students in this experiment had a science lesson in which they listened to an instructor read aloud from an informational book and then answered questions about what they learned. They had to remember facts and apply this information to answer questions. But does all science learning look like this?

A teacher reads a book and teaches science to kindergarten students in a decorated classroom.
Ariel Skelley/Corbis

Or does some science learning look like this?

A kindergarten student waters plants as part of science project in an undecorated classroom.
Randy Faris/age fotostock

9.5.2 Are Decorated Classrooms <em>Ever</em> Better?

hockenbury_tls_ch9_submit_list

Some types of learning might not benefit from a sparse classroom environment. In a typical kindergarten classroom, you might see children singing and playing musical instruments, manipulating toys, or engaging in a game involving physical activity. What general skills are being taught in these activities? For example, in the science lesson described above, the general skill is remembering new information. Look at these photos of kindergarteners. In the box alongside each photo, write one skill that is being learned.

(You must write a response for each photo before advancing to the next screen.)

A group of kindergarten students play musical instruments, which helps in developing their interaction with each other.
hl-studios/Getty Images

Question 9.2

snByA+gbH414Bn9ZPCHnGz5j+bJaRyfVU0UDU1U0BNJsReHmhwRRqokvbDf6tLrxGm9FkMaPPcTU2gfIR5eBEw==
Two kindergarten children play in a classroom that enhances their creative skills.
Ariel Skelley /age fotostock

Question 9.3

snByA+gbH414Bn9ZPCHnGz5j+bJaRyfVU0UDU1U0BNJsReHmhwRRqokvbDf6tLrxGm9FkMaPPcTU2gfIR5eBEw==
Kindergarten children play in a classroom that improves their motor skills.
Olesya Feketa/Shutterstock

Question 9.4

snByA+gbH414Bn9ZPCHnGz5j+bJaRyfVU0UDU1U0BNJsReHmhwRRqokvbDf6tLrxGm9FkMaPPcTU2gfIR5eBEw==
Among many other things, we expect kindergarten students to learn to be creative, interact appropriately with other children, and develop fine and gross motor skills.

From the experiment run by Dr. Fisher and her colleagues, we can’t know whether the types of learning shown in these photos might also be affected by classroom type. We also can’t know whether this finding would be true for older students. It remains for future experiments to expand on this work. As critical consumers of information, we should be cautious about applying the results of one study to different situations. On the other hand, we should not discount this research. It is an important and interesting finding that, at least in some situations, kindergarten students get distracted and then don’t learn as well in the typical, highly decorated classroom.

9.6 Consider the Source of the Research or Claim

4

Consider the Source of the Research or Claim

9.6.1 Common Sense Versus a Scientific Finding

Earlier, you described the ideal classroom. You wrote:

New Paragraph

Did you describe a highly decorated classroom? Most people do. Remember the Web site from earlier in this activity? It claimed: “An exciting classroom encourages learning. Who likes to study or attend class in a room without color or pictures to make it interesting?” The source of this claim is our “common sense.” It just seems strange to have a sparse kindergarten classroom. It seems as if a decorated classroom would be more welcoming and more fun, and therefore better for learning. But common sense is not always supported by research. That’s why scientific thinking is so important.

At least for learning facts and answering questions about those facts, research shows that the sparse classroom is better for kindergarteners than the decorated classroom. Sometimes scientific findings reveal that “common sense” ideas aren’t true.

Colorful table and chairs
AlexKrul/Shutterstock

9.6.2 Common Sense and a Scientific Finding

Were you surprised by the results of the experiment because they conflicted with common sense? Or did the findings make perfect sense to you once you knew more about young children’s tendency to be distracted? Unexpected findings sometimes seem perfectly obvious after we know the outcome of a study. But as the saying goes, hindsight is 20/20. In hindsight, we are looking at the question about learning environments in a new context—distraction. Before we know the outcome, however, our “common sense” guess may be completely wrong, even for those of us who spend our lives doing this kind of research.

A child looks away from the task he is working on. His looking away indicates that young children distract easily from tasks.
Masterfile Royalty-Free

9.7 Assessment

Assessment

9.7.1 Assessment

Question

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
The correct answer is D because common sense is not always backed by research, as was true with the hunch that decorated classrooms provide the best learning environment for kindergarteners.

Question

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
The correct answer is B because the decorations were the only difference between the classrooms, and the researchers found that students were more distracted in the decorated classrooms than in the sparse classrooms. So, teacher behavior and the science lessons would not have led to different learning outcomes.

Question

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
The correct answer is B. Our common sense is often wrong. In this case, the research on class size is mixed, with much of it suggesting that class size does not affect learning (see Mashburn & others, 2008).

Question

ssTCqoR2BQGDz8Xf6zussF2pF1rL+C6I0akUemZX2O+g3zgnNpTCOIObPswEQjku1Vao5bqVvFzwJJ6sR8t1Ug4tU3xGEeDI7l9vbQYuvdzyzNxcyn5sLFcnscFZf2FI7/w3ACuFFeAWiq2ldiUadpxDbDUcvShTVKSJ8AGTOQaRdyS3A0XEe0GlyLmT/geZWsAQfviVBkf61T+nUpCJ1Zo+Us8Ao5avA3QpemJRXy/qSm00ozTI90ZrBGMSHMRbm3nJItpE2iJ0+cQ0dienCIBNfXVcR1CGU8xfC1s7QXMA0Npgga6P9s09ghl7O7N8jaHfrq4rTiksLE+knyCSwkzSXvA1zjavGmTZOtk8H79ldBqypWYPy6kBf9s9YaotYnsRNFaZWkaPYwV/pkcW5cBkF0FUoBOavyrnc/o4vP9zUx/lcrS9TzE/Lh8r/X6EbPtbGkJg7TMo4ixS8HxkCaHrgOsRnJ4+9D5yog==
The correct answer is A because it is the only source that we know is providing information that is based on scientific research.

Question

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
The correct answer is C because we know it is a negative correlation, so it must be less than 0. We also know that all correlation coefficients fall between -1 and 1, so it cannot be less than -1.