Please continue to the next section.
audition the sense or act of hearing.
Like our other senses, our audition, or hearing, helps us adapt and survive. For those of us who communicate invisibly—
Most of us, however, can hear a wide range of sounds, and the ones we hear best are those in the range of the human voice. With normal hearing, we are remarkably sensitive to faint sounds, such as a child’s whimper. (If our ears were much more sensitive, we would hear a constant hiss from the movement of air molecules.) Our distant ancestors’ survival depended on this keen hearing when hunting or being hunted.
We are also remarkably attuned to sound variations. Among thousands of possible human voices, we easily recognize a friend on the phone, from the moment she says “Hi.” Moreover, hearing is fast. “It might take you a full second to notice something out of the corner of your eye, turn your head toward it, recognize it, and respond to it,” notes auditory neuroscientist Seth Horowitz (2012). “The same reaction to a new or sudden sound happens at least 10 times as fast.” A fraction of a second after such events stimulate the ear’s receptors, millions of neurons have simultaneously coordinated in extracting the essential features, comparing them with past experience, and identifying the stimulus (Freeman, 1991). For hearing as for our other senses, we wonder: How do we do it?
The Stimulus Input: Sound Waves
6-
Draw a bow across a violin, and you will unleash the energy of sound waves. Jostling molecules of air, each bumping into the next, create waves of compressed and expanded air, like the ripples on a pond circling out from a tossed stone. As we swim in our ocean of moving air molecules, our ears detect these brief air pressure changes.
Like light waves, sound waves vary in shape (FIGURE 6.36). The amplitude of sound waves determines their loudness. Their length, or frequency, determines the pitch we experience. Long waves have low frequency—
frequency the number of complete wavelengths that pass a point in a given time (for example, per second).
pitch a tone’s experienced highness or lowness; depends on frequency.
257
We measure sounds in decibels, with zero decibels representing the absolute threshold for hearing. Every 10 decibels correspond to a tenfold increase in sound intensity. Thus, normal conversation (60 decibels) is 10,000 times more intense than a 20-
The Ear
6-
The intricate process that transforms vibrating air into nerve impulses, which our brain decodes as sounds, begins when sound waves enter the outer ear. An intricate mechanical chain reaction begins as the visible outer ear channels the waves through the auditory canal to the eardrum, a tight membrane, causing it to vibrate (FIGURE 6.37 below). In the middle ear, a piston made of three tiny bones (the hammer, anvil, and stirrup) picks up the vibrations and transmits them to the cochlea, a snail-
middle ear the chamber between the eardrum and cochlea containing three tiny bones (hammer, anvil, and stirrup) that concentrate the vibrations of the eardrum on the cochlea’s oval window.
cochlea [KOHK-
inner ear the innermost part of the ear, containing the cochlea, semicircular canals, and vestibular sacs.
sensorineural hearing loss the most common form of hearing loss, also called nerve deafness; caused by damage to the cochlea’s receptor cells or to the auditory nerves.
Perhaps the most intriguing part of the hearing process is the hair cells—
conduction hearing loss less common form of hearing loss caused by damage to the mechanical system that conducts sound waves to the cochlea.
Across the world, 360 million people are challenged by hearing loss (WHO, 2012). Damage to the cochlea’s hair cell receptors or their associated nerves can cause sensorineural hearing loss (or nerve deafness). Occasionally, disease damages these receptors, but more often the culprits are biological changes linked with heredity, aging, and prolonged exposure to ear-
The cochlea’s hair cells have been likened to carpet fibers. Walk around on them and they will spring back with a quick vacuuming. But leave a heavy piece of furniture on them for a long time and they may never rebound. As a general rule, if we cannot talk over a noise, it is potentially harmful, especially if prolonged and repeated (Roesser, 1998). Such experiences are common when sound exceeds 100 decibels, as happens in venues from frenzied sports arenas to personal music systems playing near maximum volume (FIGURE 6.38 below). Ringing in the ears after exposure to loud sounds indicates that we have been bad to our unhappy hair cells. One study of teen rock concert attendees found that after three hours of sound averaging 99 decibels, 54 percent reported not hearing as well, and 1 in 4 had ringing in their ears. As pain alerts us to possible bodily harm, ringing of the ears alerts us to possible hearing damage. It is hearing’s equivalent of bleeding.
258
cochlear implant a device for converting sounds into electrical signals and stimulating the auditory nerve through electrodes threaded into the cochlea.
259
The rate of teen hearing loss, now 1 in 5, has risen by a third since the early 1990s (Shargorodsky et al., 2010). Teen boys more than teen girls or adults blast themselves with loud volumes for long periods (Zogby, 2006). Males’ greater noise exposure may help explain why men’s hearing tends to be less acute than women’s. But male or female, those who spend many hours in a loud nightclub, behind a power mower, or above a jackhammer should wear earplugs. “Condoms or, safer yet, abstinence,” say sex educators. “Earplugs or walk away,” say hearing educators.
For now, the only way to restore hearing for people with nerve deafness is a sort of bionic ear—
The outer ear collects sound waves, which are translated into mechanical waves by the middle ear and turned into fluid waves in the inner ear. The auditory nerve then translates the energy into electrical waves and sends them to the brain, which perceives and interprets the sound.
loudness
lower; lower
Perceiving Loudness, Pitch, and Location
6-
Experiments are also under way to restore vision—
Responding to Loud and Soft Sounds How do we detect loudness? If you guessed that it’s related to the intensity of a hair cell’s response, you’d be wrong. Rather, a soft, pure tone activates only the few hair cells attuned to its frequency. Given louder sounds, neighboring hair cells also respond. Thus, your brain interprets loudness from the number of activated hair cells.
If a hair cell loses sensitivity to soft sounds, it may still respond to loud sounds. This helps explain another surprise: Really loud sounds may seem loud to people with or without normal hearing. As a person with hearing loss, I [DM] used to wonder what really loud music must sound like to people with normal hearing. Now I realize it sounds much the same; where we differ is in our perception of soft sounds. This is why we hard-
Hearing Different Pitches How do we know whether a sound is the high-
For an interactive review of how we perceive sound, visit LaunchPad’s PsychSim 6: The Auditory System. For an animated explanation, visit LaunchPad’s Concept Practice: The Auditory Pathway.
place theory in hearing, the theory that links the pitch we hear with the place where the cochlea’s membrane is stimulated.
frequency theory in hearing, the theory that the rate of nerve impulses traveling up the auditory nerve matches the frequency of a tone, thus enabling us to sense its pitch. (Also called temporal theory.)
260
place theory; frequency theory
Locating Sounds Why don’t we have one big ear—
Because sound travels 750 miles per hour and human ears are but 6 inches apart, the intensity difference and the time lag are extremely small. A just noticeable difference in the direction of two sound sources corresponds to a time difference of just 0.000027 second! Lucky for us, our supersensitive auditory system can detect such minute differences (Brown & Deffenbacher, 1979; Middlebrooks & Green, 1991).
Our brain gives seeing and hearing priority in the allocation of cortical tissue. But extraordinary happenings also occur within our other senses. Sharks and dogs rely on their outstanding sense of smell, aided by large brain areas devoted to this system. Without our senses of touch, taste, smell, and body position and movement, we humans would be seriously handicapped, and our capacities for enjoying the world would be greatly diminished.
261
Touch
6-
Touch is vital. Right from the start, touch aids our development. Infant rats deprived of their mother’s grooming produce less growth hormone and have a lower metabolic rate—
Humorist Dave Barry was perhaps right to jest that your skin “keeps people from seeing the inside of your body, which is repulsive, and it prevents your organs from falling onto the ground.” But skin does much more. Touching various spots on the skin with a soft hair, a warm or cool wire, and the point of a pin reveals that some spots are especially sensitive to pressure, others to warmth, others to cold, still others to pain. Our “sense of touch” is actually a mix of these four basic and distinct skin senses, and our other skin sensations are variations of pressure, warmth, cold, and pain: Some examples:
Touch sensations involve more than tactile stimulation, however. A self-
Pain
6-
Be thankful for occasional pain. Pain is your body’s way of telling you something has gone wrong. By drawing your attention to a burn, a break, or a sprain, pain orders you to change your behavior—
More numerous are those who live with chronic pain, which is rather like an alarm that won’t shut off. The suffering of such people, and of those with persistent or recurring backaches, arthritis, headaches, and cancer-
Understanding Pain Our pain experiences vary widely. Women are more sensitive to pain than men are (their senses of hearing and smell also tend to be more sensitive) (Ruau et al., 2011; Wickelgren, 2009). Our individual pain sensitivity varies, too, depending on genes, physiology, experience, attention, and surrounding culture (Gatchel et al., 2007; Reimann et al., 2010). Thus, our experience of pain reflects both bottom-
nociceptors sensory receptors that enable the perception of pain in response to potentially harmful stimuli.
262
BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES There is no one type of stimulus that triggers pain (as light triggers vision). Instead, there are different nociceptors—sensory receptors in our skin, muscles, and organs that detect hurtful temperatures, pressure, or chemicals (FIGURE 6.41).
gate-
Although no theory of pain explains all available findings, psychologist Ronald Melzack and biologist Patrick Wall’s (1965, 1983; Melzack & Katz, 2013) classic gate-
But pain is not merely a physical phenomenon of injured nerves sending impulses to a definable brain area—
The brain can also create pain, as it does in people’s experiences of phantom limb sensations, after a limb has been amputated. Their brain may misinterpret the spontaneous central nervous system (CNS) activity that occurs in the absence of normal sensory input. As the dreamer may see with eyes closed, so 7 in 10 such people may feel pain or movement in nonexistent limbs (Melzack, 1992, 2005). (Some may also try to step off a bed onto a phantom limb or to lift a cup with a phantom hand.) Even those born without a limb sometimes perceive sensations from the absent arm or leg. The brain, Melzack (1998) has surmised, comes prepared to anticipate “that it will be getting information from a body that has limbs.”
Phantoms may haunt other senses too, as the brain, responding to the absence of sensory signals, amplifies irrelevant neural activity. People with hearing loss often experience the sound of silence: tinnitus, the phantom sound of ringing in the ears. Those who lose vision to glaucoma, cataracts, diabetes, or macular degeneration may experience phantom sights—
263
PSYCHOLOGICAL INFLUENCES One powerful influence on our perception of pain is the attention we focus on it. Athletes, focused on winning, may play through the pain. Halfway through his lap of the 2012 Olympics 1600 meter relay, Manteo Mitchell broke one of his leg bones—
We also seem to edit our memories of pain, which often differ from the pain we actually experienced. In experiments, and after medical procedures, people overlook a pain’s duration. Their memory snapshots instead record two factors: their pain’s peak moment (which can lead them to recall variable pain, with peaks, as worse [Stone et al., 2005]), and how much pain they felt at the end. In one experiment, researchers asked people to immerse one hand in painfully cold water for 60 seconds, and then the other hand in the same painfully cold water for 60 seconds followed by a slightly less painful 30 seconds more (Kahneman et al., 1993). Which experience would you expect to recall as most painful?
Curiously, when asked which trial they would prefer to repeat, most preferred the 90-
The end of an experience can color our memory of pleasures, too. In one simple experiment, some people, on receiving a fifth and last piece of chocolate, were told it was their “next” one. Others, told it was their “last” piece, liked it better and also rated the whole experiment as being more enjoyable (O’Brien & Ellsworth, 2012). Endings matter.
SOCIAL-
Thus, our perception of pain is a biopsychosocial phenomenon (Hadjistavropoulos et al., 2011). Viewing pain from many perspectives can help us better understand how to cope with it and treat it (FIGURE 6.42).
Controlling Pain If pain is where body meets mind—
That explains some striking influences on pain. When we are distracted from pain (a psychological influence) and soothed by the release of our naturally painkilling endorphins (a biological influence), our experience of pain diminishes. Sports injuries may go unnoticed until the after-
264
PLACEBOS Even an inert placebo can help, by dampening the central nervous system’s attention and responses to painful experiences—
Another experiment pitted two placebos—
“When belly with bad pains doth swell, It matters naught what else goes well.”
Sadi, The Gulistan, 1258
“Pain is increased by attending to it.”
Charles Darwin, Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals, 1872
DISTRACTION Distracting people with pleasant images (“Think of a warm, comfortable environment”) or drawing their attention away from the painful stimulation (“Count backward by 3’s”) is an effective way to activate pain-
265
Hypnosis and Pain Relief
hypnosis a social interaction in which one person (the hypnotist) suggests to another (the subject) that certain perceptions, feelings, thoughts, or behaviors will spontaneously occur.
Imagine you are about to be hypnotized. The hypnotist invites you to sit back, fix your gaze on a spot high on the wall, and relax. In a quiet, low voice the hypnotist suggests, “Your eyes are growing tired…. Your eyelids are becoming heavy…now heavier and heavier…. They are beginning to close…. You are becoming more deeply relaxed…. Your breathing is now deep and regular…. Your muscles are becoming more and more relaxed. Your whole body is beginning to feel like lead.”
After a few minutes of this hypnotic induction, you may experience hypnosis. Hypnotists have no magical mind-
Can hypnosis relieve pain? Yes. When unhypnotized people put their arms in an ice bath, they felt intense pain within 25 seconds (Elkins et al., 2012; Jensen, 2008). When hypnotized people did the same after being given suggestions to feel no pain, they indeed reported feeling little pain. As some dentists know, light hypnosis can reduce fear, thus reducing hypersensitivity to pain.
Hypnosis inhibits pain-
dissociation a split in consciousness, which allows some thoughts and behaviors to occur simultaneously with others.
Psychologists have proposed two explanations for how hypnosis works. One theory proposes that hypnosis is a form of normal social influence (Lynn et al., 1990; Spanos & Coe, 1992). In this view, hypnosis is a by-
posthypnotic suggestion a suggestion, made during a hypnosis session, to be carried out after the subject is no longer hypnotized; used by some clinicians to help control undesired symptoms and behaviors.
Another theory views hypnosis as a special dual-
Another form of dual processing—
c.
266
Taste
6-
Like touch, our sense of taste involves several basic sensations. Taste’s sensations were once thought to be sweet, sour, salty, and bitter, with all others stemming from mixtures of these four (McBurney & Gent, 1979). Then, as investigators searched for specialized nerve fibers for the four taste sensations, they encountered a receptor for what we now know is a fifth—
Tastes exist for more than our pleasure (see TABLE 6.2). Pleasureful tastes attracted our ancestors to energy-
Taste is a chemical sense. Inside each little bump on the top and sides of your tongue are 200 or more taste buds, each containing a pore that catches food chemicals. In each taste bud pore, 50 to 100 taste receptor cells project antenna-
Taste receptors reproduce themselves every week or two, so if you burn your tongue with hot food it hardly matters. However, as you grow older, the number of taste buds decreases, as does taste sensitivity (Cowart, 1981). (No wonder adults enjoy strong-
“Life is not measured by the number of breaths we take, but by the moments that take our breath away.”
Author unknown
Essential as taste buds are, there’s more to taste than meets the tongue. Expectations can influence taste. When told a sausage roll was “vegetarian,” people in one experiment found it decidedly inferior to its identical partner labeled “meat” (Allen et al., 2008). In another experiment, being told that a wine cost $90 rather than its real $10 price made it taste better and triggered more activity in a brain area that responds to pleasant experiences (Plassmann et al., 2008).
Impress your friends with your new word for the day: People unable to see are said to experience blindness. People unable to hear experience deafness. People unable to smell experience anosmia. The 1 in 7500 people born with anosmia not only have trouble cooking and eating, but also are somewhat more prone to depression, accidents, and relationship insecurity (Croy et al., 2012, 2013).
Smell
Life begins with an inhale and ends with an exhale. Between birth and death, you will daily inhale and exhale nearly 20,000 breaths of life-
Like taste, smell is a chemical sense. We smell something when molecules of a substance carried in the air reach a tiny cluster of 20 million receptor cells at the top of each nasal cavity (FIGURE 6.44). These olfactory receptor cells, waving like sea anemones on a reef, respond selectively—
For an animated explanation of how we smell, visit LaunchPad’s Concept Practice: Sense of Smell.
267
Even nursing infants and their mothers have a literal chemistry to their relationship. They quickly learn to recognize each other’s scents (McCarthy, 1986). Aided by smell, a mother fur seal returning to a beach crowded with pups will find her own. Our human sense of smell is less acute than our senses of seeing and hearing. Looking out across a garden, we see its forms and colors in exquisite detail and hear a variety of birds singing, yet we smell little of it without sticking our nose into the blossoms.
Odor molecules come in many shapes and sizes—
Gender and age influence our ability to identify scents. Women and young adults have the best sense of smell (Wickelgren, 2009; Wysocki & Gilbert, 1989). Physical condition also matters. Smokers and people with Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, or alcohol use disorder typically have a diminished sense of smell (Doty, 2001). For all of us, however, the sense of smell tends to peak in early adulthood and gradually declines thereafter (FIGURE 6.45 below).
Despite our skill at discriminating scents, we aren’t very good at describing them. Try it: Which is easier, describing the sound of coffee brewing, or the aroma of coffee? For most people, it’s the sound. Compared with how we experience and remember sights and sounds, smells are primitive and harder to describe and recall (Richardson & Zucco, 1989; Zucco, 2003).
“There could be a stack of truck tires burning in the living room, and I wouldn’t necessarily smell it. Whereas my wife can detect a lone spoiled grape two houses away.”
Dave Barry, 2005
As any dog or cat with a good nose could tell us, we each have our own identifiable chemical signature. (One noteworthy exception: A dog will follow the tracks of one identical twin as though they had been made by the other [Thomas, 1974].) Animals that have many times more olfactory receptors than we do also use their sense of smell to communicate and to navigate. Long before a shark can see its prey, or a moth its mate, olfactory cues direct their way, as they also do for migrating salmon returning to their home stream. After being exposed in a hatchery to one of two odorant chemicals, salmon have, when returning two years later, sought whichever stream near their release site was spiked with the familiar smell (Barinaga, 1999).
268
For humans, too, the attractiveness of smells depends on learned associations (Herz, 2001). As babies nurse, their preference for the smell of their mother’s breast builds. So, too, with other associations. As good experiences are linked with a particular scent, people come to like that scent. This helps explain why people in the United States tend to like the smell of wintergreen (which they associate with candy and gum) more than do those in Great Britain (where it often is associated with medicine). In another example of odors evoking unpleasant emotions, researchers frustrated Brown University students with a rigged computer game in a scented room (Herz et al., 2004). Later, if exposed to the same odor while working on a verbal task, the students’ frustration was rekindled and they gave up sooner than others exposed to a different odor or no odor.
Though it’s difficult to recall odors by name, we may recognize long-
Our brain’s circuitry helps explain an odor’s power to evoke feelings and memories (FIGURE 6.46). A hotline runs between the brain area receiving information from the nose and the brain’s ancient limbic centers associated with memory and emotion. Thus, when put in a foul-
Smell is indeed primitive. Eons before the elaborate analytical areas of our cerebral cortex had fully evolved, our mammalian ancestors sniffed for food—
269
We have four basic touch senses and five basic taste sensations. But we have no basic smell receptors. Instead, different combinations of odor receptors send messages to the brain, enabling us to recognize some 10,000 different smells.
Body Position and Movement
kinesthesia [kin-
6-
vestibular sense the sense of body movement and position, including the sense of balance.
Important sensors in your joints, tendons, and muscles enable your kinesthesia—your sense of the position and movement of your body parts. By closing your eyes or plugging your ears you can momentarily imagine being without sight or sound. But what would it be like to live without touch or kinesthesia—
A companion vestibular sense monitors your head’s (and thus your body’s) position and movement. The biological gyroscopes for this sense of equilibrium are two structures in your inner ear. The first, your semicircular canals, look like a three-
If you twirl around and then come to an abrupt halt, neither the fluid in your semicircular canals nor your kinesthetic receptors will immediately return to their neutral state. The dizzy aftereffect fools your brain with the sensation that you’re still spinning. This illustrates a principle that underlies perceptual illusions: Mechanisms that normally give us an accurate experience of the world can, under special conditions, fool us. Understanding how we get fooled provides clues to how our perceptual system works.
Kinesthetic receptors are located in our joints, tendons, and muscles. Vestibular sense receptors are located in our inner ear.
6-
Our senses—
sensory interaction the principle that one sense may influence another, as when the smell of food influences its taste.
270
Thus, to savor a taste, we normally breathe the aroma through our nose—
Vision and hearing may similarly interact. A weak flicker of light that we have trouble perceiving becomes more visible when accompanied by a short burst of sound (Kayser, 2007). And a sound may be easier to hear with a visual cue. If I [DM], as a person with hearing loss, watch a video with simultaneous captioning, I have no trouble hearing the words I am seeing. I may therefore think I don’t need the captioning, but if I then turn off the captioning, I suddenly realize I do need it. The eyes guide the ears (FIGURE 6.47).
But what do you suppose happens if the eyes and the ears disagree? What if we see a speaker saying one syllable while we hear another? Surprise: We may perceive a third syllable that blends both inputs. Seeing the mouth movements for ga while hearing ba we may perceive da. This phenomenon is known as the McGurk effect, after its discoverers, Scottish psychologist Harry McGurk and his assistant John MacDonald (1976). For all of us, lip reading is part of hearing.
Touch also interacts with our other senses. In detecting events, the brain can combine simultaneous touch and visual signals, thanks to neurons projecting from the somatosensory cortex back to the visual cortex (Macaluso et al., 2000). Touch even interacts with hearing. One experiment blew a puff of air (such as our mouths produce when saying pa and ta) on the neck or hands as people heard either these sounds or the more airless sounds ba or da. The result? People more often misheard ba or da as pa or ta when played with the faint puff (Gick & Derrick, 2009). Thanks to sensory interaction, they heard with their skin.
Our brain even blends our tactile and social judgments, as demonstrated in these playful experiments:
Are you wondering how researchers test these kinds of questions? Try LaunchPad’s How Would You Know If a Cup of Coffee Can Warm Up Relationships?
embodied cognition in psychological science, the influence of bodily sensations, gestures, and other states on cognitive preferences and judgments.
271
These examples of embodied cognition illustrate how brain circuits processing bodily sensations connect with brain circuits responsible for cognition. We think from within a body.
So, the senses interact: As we attempt to decipher our world, our brain blends inputs from multiple channels. For many people, an odor, perhaps of mint or chocolate, can evoke a sensation of taste (Stevenson & Tomiczek, 2007). But in a few select individuals, the senses become joined in a phenomenon called synesthesia, where one sort of sensation (such as hearing sound) involuntarily produces another (such as seeing color). Early in life, “exuberant neural connectivity” produces some arbitrary associations among the senses, which later are normally—
***
For a summary of our sensory systems, see TABLE 6.3. The river of perception is fed by sensation, cognition, and emotion. And that is why we need biological, psychological, and social-
If perception is the product of these three sources, what can we say about extrasensory perception, which claims that perception can occur apart from sensory input? For more on that question, see Thinking Critically About: ESP—
***
To feel awe, mystery, and a deep reverence for life, we need look no further than our own perceptual system and its capacity for organizing formless nerve impulses into colorful sights, vivid sounds, and evocative smells. As Shakespeare’s Hamlet recognized, “There are more things in Heaven and Earth, Horatio, than are dreamt of in your philosophy.” Within our ordinary sensory and perceptual experiences lies much that is truly extraordinary—
272
ESP—
extrasensory perception (ESP) the controversial claim that perception can occur apart from sensory input; includes telepathy, clairvoyance, and precognition.
6-
Without sensory input, are we capable of extrasensory perception (ESP)? Are there indeed people—
The most testable and, for this discussion, most relevant ESP claims are
Closely linked is psychokinesis, or “mind over matter,” such as levitating a table or influencing the roll of a die. (The claim is illustrated by the wry request, “Will all those who believe in psychokinesis please raise my hand?”)
parapsychology the study of paranormal phenomena, including ESP and psychokinesis.
If ESP is real, we would need to overturn the scientific understanding that we are creatures whose minds are tied to our physical brains and whose perceptual experiences of the world are built of sensations. Sometimes new evidence does overturn our scientific preconceptions. Science, as we will see throughout this book, offers us surprises—
Most research psychologists and scientists are skeptical that paranormal phenomena exist. But reputable universities in Great Britain, the Netherlands, and Australia, have added faculty chairs or research units in parapsychology (Storm, 2010a,b; Turpin, 2005). These researchers perform scientific experiments searching for possible ESP and other paranormal phenomena. Before seeing how parapsychologists do research on ESP, let’s consider some popular beliefs.
Premonitions or Pretensions?
Can psychics see into the future? Although one might wish for a psychic stock forecaster, the tallied forecasts of “leading psychics” reveal meager accuracy. During the 1990s, the tabloid psychics were all wrong in predicting surprising events. (Madonna did not become a gospel singer, the Statue of Liberty did not lose both its arms in a terrorist blast, Queen Elizabeth did not abdicate her throne to enter a convent.) And the psychics have missed recent big-
After Amanda Berry went missing in Cleveland in 2003, her distraught and desperate mother turned to a famed psychic on a national television show for answers. “She’s not alive, honey,” the psychic told the devastated mom, who died without living to see her daughter rescued in 2013 (Radford, 2013). According to one analysis, this result brought that psychic’s record on 116 missing person and death cases to 83 unknown outcomes, 33 incorrect, and zero mostly correct. To researcher Ryan Shaffer (2013), that’s the record of a “psychic defective.”
The psychic visions offered to police departments have been no more accurate than guesses made by others (Nickell, 1994, 2005; Radford, 2010; Reiser, 1982). But their sheer volume does increase the odds of an occasional correct guess, which psychics can then report to the media. Police departments are wise to all this. When researchers asked the police departments of America’s 50 largest cities whether they ever had used psychics, 65 percent said No (Sweat & Durm, 1993). Of those that had, not one had found them helpful.
Psychics’ vague predictions sometimes sound correct when later interpreted (“retrofitted”) to match events that provide a perceptual set for “understanding” them. Nostradamus, a sixteenth-
273
Are the spontaneous “visions” of everyday people any more accurate? Do dreams, for example, foretell the future, as people from both Eastern and Western cultures tend to believe—
Given the billions of events in the world each day, and given enough days, some stunning coincidences are sure to occur. By one careful estimate, chance alone would predict that more than a thousand times a day someone on Earth will think of another person and then within the next five minutes will learn of that person’s death (Charpak & Broch, 2004). Thus, when explaining an astonishing event, we should “give chance a chance” (Lilienfeld, 2009). With enough time and people, the improbable becomes inevitable.
Putting ESP to Experimental Test
When faced with claims of mind reading or out-
This scientific attitude has led both believers and skeptics to agree that what parapsychology needs is a reproducible phenomenon and a theory to explain it. Parapsychologist Rhea White (1998) spoke for many in saying that “the image of parapsychology that comes to my mind, based on nearly 44 years in the field,
“To be sure of hitting the target, shoot first and call whatever you hit the target.”
Writer-
“A person who talks a lot is sometimes right.”
Spanish proverb
is that of a small airplane [that] has been perpetually taxiing down the runway of the Empirical Science Airport since 1882…its movement punctuated occasionally by lifting a few feet off the ground only to bump back down on the tarmac once again. It has never taken off for any sustained flight.”
How might we test ESP claims in a controlled, reproducible experiment? An experiment differs from a staged demonstration. In the laboratory, the experimenter controls what the “psychic” sees and hears. On stage, the psychic controls what the audience sees and hears.
The search for a valid and reliable test of ESP has resulted in thousands of experiments. After digesting data from 30 such studies, parapsychologist Lance Storm and his colleagues (2010a,b; 2013) concluded that, given participants with experience or belief in ESP, there is “consistent and reliable” parapsychological evidence. Psychologist Ray Hyman (2010), who has been scrutinizing parapsychological research since 1957, replied that if this is the best evidence, it fails to impress: “Parapsychology will achieve scientific acceptability only when it provides a positive theory with…independently replicable evidence. This is something it has yet to achieve after more than a century.”
Daryl Bem (2011), a respected social psychologist, has been a skeptic of stage psychics; he once quipped that “a psychic is an actor playing the role of a psychic” (1984). Yet he reignited hopes for replicable evidence with nine experiments that seemed to show people anticipating future events. In one, when an erotic scene was about to appear on a screen in one of two randomly selected positions, Cornell University participants guessed right 53.1 percent of the time (beating 50 percent by a small but statistically significant margin). In another, people viewed a set of words, took a recall test of those words, and then rehearsed a randomly selected subset of those words. People better remembered the rehearsed words—
Bem wonders if his “anomalous” findings reflect an evolutionary advantage to those who can precognitively anticipate future dangers. Critics scoff. “If any of his claims were true,” wrote cognitive scientist Douglas Hofstadter (2011), “then all of the bases underlying contemporary science would be toppled, and
“At the heart of science is an essential tension between two seemingly contradictory attitudes—
Carl Sagan (1987)
274
we would have to rethink everything about the nature of the universe.” Moreover, if future events retroactively affect present feelings, then why can’t people intuitively predict casino outcomes or stock market futures?
Despite the paper having survived critical reviews by a top-
Anticipating such skepticism, Bem has made his computer materials available to anyone who wishes to replicate his studies. Multiple attempts have since been made, without success (Galak et al., 2012; Ritchie et al., 2012). Regardless, science is doing its work. It has been open to a finding that challenges its own assumptions. And then, through follow-
One skeptic, magician James Randi, has had a longstanding offer of $1 million to be given “to anyone who proves a genuine psychic power under proper observing conditions” (Randi, 1999; Thompson, 2010). French, Australian, and Indian groups have made similar offers of up to 200,000 euros (CFI, 2003). Large as these sums are, the scientific seal of approval would be worth far more. To refute those who say there is no ESP, one need only produce a single person who can demonstrate a single, reproducible ESP event. (To refute those who say pigs can’t talk would take but one talking pig.) So far, no such person has emerged.
The ESP event would need to be reproduced in other scientific studies.
275
REVIEW | The Nonvisual Senses |
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
RETRIEVAL PRACTICE Take a moment to answer each of these Learning Objective Questions (repeated here from within this section). Then click the 'show answer' button to check your answers. Research suggests that trying to answer these questions on your own will improve your long-
6-
Sound waves are bands of compressed and expanded air. Our ears detect these changes in air pressure and transform them into neural impulses, which the brain decodes as sound. Sound waves vary in amplitude, which we perceive as differing loudness, and in frequency, which we experience as differing pitch.
6-
The outer ear is the visible portion of the ear. The middle ear is the chamber between the eardrum and cochlea. The inner ear consists of the cochlea, semicircular canals, and vestibular sacs. Through a mechanical chain of events, sound waves traveling through the auditory canal cause tiny vibrations in the eardrum. The bones of the middle ear amplify the vibrations and relay them to the fluid-filled cochlea. Rippling of the basilar membrane, caused by pressure changes in the cochlear fluid, causes movement of the tiny hair cells, triggering neural messages to be sent (via the thalamus) to the auditory cortex in the brain.
Sensorineural hearing loss (or nerve deafness) results from damage to the cochlea’s hair cells or their associated nerves. Conduction hearing loss results from damage to the mechanical system that transmits sound waves to the cochlea. Cochlear implants can restore hearing for some people.
6-
Loudness is not related to the intensity of a hair cell’s response. The brain interprets loudness from the number of activated hair cells.
Place theory explains how we hear high-
Sound waves strike one ear sooner and more intensely than the other. To locate sounds, the brain analyzes the minute differences in the sounds received by the two ears and computes the sound’s source.
6-
Our sense of touch is actually several senses—
6-
Pain reflects bottom-
Pain treatments often combine physical and psychological elements. Placebos can help by dampening the central nervous system’s attention and response to painful experiences. Distractions draw people’s attention away from painful stimulation. Hypnosis, which increases our response to suggestions, can also help relieve pain. Posthypnotic suggestion is used by some clinicians to control undesired symptoms.
6-
Taste and smell are both chemical senses. Taste is a composite of five basic sensations—
There are no basic sensations for smell. We smell something when molecules of a substance carried in the air reach a tiny cluster of 20 million receptor cells at the top of each nasal cavity. Odor molecules trigger combinations of receptors, in patterns that the olfactory cortex interprets. The receptor cells send messages to the brain’s olfactory bulb, then to the temporal lobe, and to parts of the limbic system.
6-
Through kinesthesia, we sense the position and movement of our body parts. We monitor our head’s (and thus our body’s) position and movement, and maintain our balance, with our vestibular sense.
6-
Our senses can influence one another. This sensory interaction occurs, for example, when the smell of a favorite food amplifies its taste. Embodied cognition is the influence of bodily sensations, gestures, and other states on cognitive preferences and judgments.
6-
Parapsychology is the study of paranormal phenomena, including extrasensory perception (ESP) and psychokinesis. The three most testable forms of ESP are telepathy (mind-
Skeptics argue that (1) to believe in ESP, you must believe the brain is capable of perceiving without sensory input, and (2) researchers have been unable to replicate ESP phenomena under controlled conditions.
TERMS AND CONCEPTS TO REMEMBER
RETRIEVAL PRACTICE Match each of the terms on the left with its definition on the right. Click on the term first and then click on the matching definition. As you match them correctly they will move to the bottom of the activity.
Use to create your personalized study plan, which will direct you to the resources that will help you most in .
TEST
YOUR-
SELF SENSATION AND PERCEPTION
Test yourself repeatedly throughout your studies. This will not only help you figure out what you know and don’t know; the testing itself will help you learn and remember the information more effectively thanks to the testing effect.
Basic Concepts of Sensation and Perception
2. The process by which we organize and interpret sensory information is called sNhxVmWhizK+x2YFvSEdqA== .
4. Another term for difference threshold is the lDuFfqgolFzWDweA cK4gdrMTyNiizh62SupyUw== cIetd8NqWiDGQnmyzFchew== .
Vision: Sensory and Perceptual Processing
8. The characteristic of light that determines the color we experience, such as blue or green, is DLMUGKxdC9Rgok/3dunpQA== .
14. The cells in the visual cortex that respond to certain lines, edges, and angles are called 5llCtSkxYB8gNPXC B0zMwbxg1zrBZAduSJOJWA== .
15. The brain’s ability to process many aspects of an object or a problem simultaneously is called l3vDQ6fqbYooRRnoHtYtHA== gQSngC56Q9bkvNMUcKYwcA== .
20. Two examples of /J3LLRQDuZpBSDGPIEgteA== depth cues are interposition and linear perspective.
23. In experiments, people have worn glasses that turned their visual fields upside down. After a period of adjustment, they learned to function quite well. This ability is called HdOE6Hj3ScL6nNi4RX4mXQ== zKM90kbxEpSxTO8C1G3dVA== .
277
The Nonvisual Senses
24. The snail-shaped tube in the inner ear, where sound waves are converted into neural activity, is called the LJfllkMB6Ks7STQ0 .
26. IdG4V7ZZiixXXpnP
theory explains how we hear high-
30. M24v9s4wUkHfzNENhWz+8A== is your sense of body position and movement. Your R0I1lxpj2/NF44hbQL/Ing== LR6yy6D6DUtrYEnl specifically monitors your head’s movement, with sensors in the inner ear.