Asked about the ideal wealth distribution in America, Democrats and Republicans were surprisingly similar. In the Democrats’ ideal world, the richest 20 percent would possess 30 percent of the wealth. Republicans preferred a similar 35 percent (Norton & Ariely, 2011).
43
In descriptive, correlational, and experimental research, statistics are tools that help us see and interpret what the unaided eye might miss. Sometimes the unaided eye misses badly. Researchers Michael Norton and Dan Ariely (2011) invited 5522 Americans to estimate the percent of wealth possessed by the richest 20 percent in their country. The average person’s guess—
When setting goals, we love big round numbers. We’re far more likely to want to lose 20 pounds than 19 or 21 pounds. We’re far more likely to retake the SAT if our verbal plus math score is just short of a big round number, such as 1200. By modifying their behavior, batters are nearly four times more likely to finish the season with a .300 average than with a .299 average (Pope & Simonsohn, 2011).
Accurate statistical understanding benefits everyone. To be an educated person today is to be able to apply simple statistical principles to everyday reasoning. One needn’t memorize complicated formulas to think more clearly and critically about data.
Off-
The point to remember: Doubt big, round, undocumented numbers. That’s actually a lesson we intuitively appreciate, by finding precise numbers more credible (Oppenheimer et al., 2014). When U.S. Secretary of State John Kerry sought to rally American support in 2013 for a military response to Syria’s apparent use of chemical weapons, his argument gained credibility from its precision: “The United States government now knows that at least 1429 Syrians were killed in this attack, including at least 426 children.”
Statistical illiteracy also feeds needless health scares (Gigerenzer et al., 2008, 2009, 2010). In the 1990s, the British press reported a study showing that women taking a particular contraceptive pill had a 100 percent increased risk of blood clots that could produce strokes. This caused thousands of women to stop taking the pill, leading to a wave of unwanted pregnancies and an estimated 13,000 additional abortions (which also are associated with increased blood-
1-
Once researchers have gathered their data, they may use descriptive statistics to organize that data meaningfully. One way to do this is to convert the data into a simple bar graph, as in FIGURE 1.8 below, which displays a distribution of different brands of trucks still on the road after a decade. When reading statistical graphs such as this, take care. It’s easy to design a graph to make a difference look big (Figure 1.8a) or small (Figure 1.8b). The secret lies in how you label the vertical scale (the y-
The point to remember: Think smart. When viewing graphs, read the scale labels and note their range.
Note how the y-axis of each graph is labeled. The range for the y-axis label in graph (a) is only from 95 to 100. The range for graph (b) is from 0 to 100. All the trucks rank as 95% and up, so almost all are still functioning after 10 years, which graph (b) makes clear.
mode the most frequently occurring score(s) in a distribution.
Measures of Central Tendency
mean the arithmetic average of a distribution, obtained by adding the scores and then dividing by the number of scores.
The next step is to summarize the data using some measure of central tendency, a single score that represents a whole set of scores. The simplest measure is the mode, the most frequently occurring score or scores. The most familiar is the mean, or arithmetic average—
median the middle score in a distribution; half the scores are above it and half are below it.
Measures of central tendency neatly summarize data. But consider what happens to the mean when a distribution is lopsided, when it’s skewed by a few way-
The average person has one ovary and one testicle.
44
The point to remember: Always note which measure of central tendency is reported. If it is a mean, consider whether a few atypical scores could be distorting it.
Measures of Variation
Knowing the value of an appropriate measure of central tendency can tell us a great deal. But the single number omits other information. It helps to know something about the amount of variation in the data—
range the difference between the highest and lowest scores in a distribution.
45
The range of scores—
The more useful standard for measuring how much scores deviate from one another is the standard deviation. It better gauges whether scores are packed together or dispersed, because it uses information from each score. The computation (see TABLE 1.4 for an example) assembles information about how much individual scores differ from the mean. If your college or university attracts students of a certain ability level, their intelligence scores will have a relatively small standard deviation compared with the more diverse community population outside your school.
standard deviation a computed measure of how much scores vary around the mean score.
normal curve (normal distribution) a symmetrical, bell-
You can grasp the meaning of the standard deviation if you consider how scores tend to be distributed in nature. Large numbers of data—
As FIGURE 1.10 shows, a useful property of the normal curve is that roughly 68 percent of the cases fall within one standard deviation on either side of the mean. About 95 percent of cases fall within two standard deviations. Thus, as Chapter 10 notes, about 68 percent of people taking an intelligence test will score within ±15 points of 100. About 95 percent will score within ±30 points.
46
For an interactive tutorial on these statistical concepts, visit LaunchPad’s PsychSim 6: Descriptive Statistics.
mean; mode; median; range; standard deviation
1-
Data are “noisy.” The average score in one group (children who were breast-
When Is an Observed Difference Reliable?
In deciding when it is safe to generalize from a sample, we should keep three principles in mind:
The point to remember: Smart thinkers are not overly impressed by a few anecdotes. Generalizations based on a few unrepresentative cases are unreliable.
47
When Is an Observed Difference Significant?
Perhaps you’ve compared men’s and women’s scores on a laboratory test of aggression, and found a gender difference. But individuals differ. How likely is it that the difference you observed was just a fluke? Statistical testing can estimate that.
Here is the underlying logic: When averages from two samples are each reliable measures of their respective populations (as when each is based on many observations that have small variability), then their difference is likely to be reliable as well. (Example: The less the variability in women’s and in men’s aggression scores, the more confidence we would have that any observed gender difference is reliable.) And when the difference between the sample averages is large, we have even more confidence that the difference between them reflects a real difference in their populations.
statistical significance a statistical statement of how likely it is that an obtained result occurred by chance.
In short, when sample averages are reliable, and when the difference between them is relatively large, we say the difference has statistical significance. This means that the observed difference is probably not due to chance variation between the samples.
For a 9.5-
In judging statistical significance, psychologists are conservative. They are like juries who must presume innocence until guilt is proven. For most psychologists, proof beyond a reasonable doubt means not making much of a finding unless the odds of its occurring by chance, if no real effect exists, are less than 5 percent.
When reading about research, you should remember that, given large enough or homogeneous enough samples, a difference between them may be “statistically significant” yet have little practical significance. For example, comparisons of intelligence test scores among hundreds of thousands of first-
The point to remember: Statistical significance indicates the likelihood that a result will happen by chance. But this does not say anything about the importance of the result.
The registrar’s office at the University of Michigan has found that usually about 100 students in Arts and Sciences have perfect marks at the end of their first term at the University. However, only about 10 to 15 students graduate with perfect marks. What do you think is the most likely explanation for the fact that there are more perfect marks after one term than at graduation (Jepson et al., 1983)?
Averages based on fewer courses are more variable, which guarantees a greater number of extremely low and high marks at the end of the first term.
Descriptive; inferential
48