Although we prefer to use -values rather than the reject-or-not view of the level significance test, the latter view is very important for planning studies and for understanding statistical decision theory. We discuss these two topics in this section.
Power
Level significance tests are closely related to confidence intervals—in fact, we saw that a two-sided test can be carried out directly from a confidence interval. The significance level, like the confidence level, says how reliable the method is in repeated use. If we use 5% significance tests repeatedly when is, in fact, true, we will be wrong (the test will reject ) 5% of the time and right (the test will fail to reject ) 95% of the time.
The ability of a test to detect that is false is measured by the probability that the test will reject when an alternative is true. The higher this probability is, the more sensitive the test is.
Power
The probability that a level significance test will reject when a particular alternative value of the parameter is true is called the power of the test to detect that alternative.
EXAMPLE 6.26 The Power to Detect Departure from Target
CASE 6.1 Case 6.1 considered the following competing hypotheses:
In Example 6.13 (page 317), we learned that for the filling process. Suppose that the bottler Bestea wishes to conduct tests of the filling process mean at a 1% level of significance. Assume, as in Example 6.13, that 20 bottles are randomly chosen for inspection. Bestea’s operations personnel wish to detect a 1-ml change in mean fill amount, either in terms of underfilling or overfilling. Does a sample of 20 bottles provide sufficient power?
344
We answer this question by calculating the power of the significance test that will be used to evaluate the data to be collected. Power calculations consist of three steps:
Let’s go through these three steps for Example 6.26.
Step 1. The null hypothesis is that the mean filling amount is at the 473-ml target level. The alternative is two-sided in that we wish to detect change in either direction from the target level. Formally, we have
In the possible values of the alternative, we are particularly interested in values at a minimal 1 ml from 472. This would mean that we are focusing on values of 472 or 474. We can proceed with the power calculations using either one of these values. Let’s pick the specific alternative of .
Step 2. The test statistic is
From Table D, we find that -values less than or greater than 2.576 would be viewed as signifcant at the 1% level. Consider first rejection above 2.576. We can rewrite the upper rejection rule in terms of :
We can do the same sort of rearrangement with the lower rejection rule to find rejection is also associated with:
Step 3. The power to detect the alternative is the probability that will be rejected when, in fact, . We calculate this probability by standardizing using the value , the population standard deviation , and the sample size . We have to remember that rejection can happen when either or These are disjoint events, so the power is the sum of their probabilities, computed assuming that the alternative is true. We find that
345
Figure 6.18 illustrates this calculation. Because the power is only about 0.37, we are not strongly confident that the test will reject when this alternative is true.
Increasing the power
Suppose that you have performed a power calculation and found that the power is too small. What can you do to increase it? Here are four ways:
Power calculations are important in planning studies. Using a significance test with low power makes it unlikely that you will find a signifcant effect even if the truth is far from the null hypothesis. A null hypothesis that is, in fact, false can become widely believed if repeated attempts to find evidence against it fail because of low power.
In Example 6.26, we found the power to be 0.37 for the detection of a 1-ml departure from the null hypothesis. If this power is unsatisfactory to the bottler, one option noted earlier is to increase the sample size. Just how large should the sample be? The following example explores this question.
EXAMPLE 6.27 Choosing Sample Size for a Desired Power
CASE 6.1 Suppose the bottler Bestea desires a power of 0.9 in the detection of the specific alternative of . From Example 6.26, we found that a sample size of 20 offers a power of only 0.37. Manually, we can repeat the calculations found in Example 6.26 for different values of larger than 20 until we find the smallest sample size giving at least a power of 0.9. Fortunately, most statistical software saves us from such tedium. Figure 6.19 shows Minitab output with inputs of 0.9 for power, 1% for sig-nifcance level, 2 for , and for the departure amount from the null hypothesis. From the output, we learn that a sample size of at least 60 is needed to have a power of at least 0.9. If we used a sample size of 59, the actual power would be a bit less than the target power of 0.9.
346
Inference as decision
We have presented tests of significance as methods for assessing the strength of evidence against the null hypothesis. This assessment is made by the -value, which is a probability computed under the assumption that is true. The alternative hypothesis (the statement we seek evidence for) enters the test only to help us see what outcomes count against the null hypothesis.
There is another way to think about these issues. Sometimes, we are really concerned about making a decision or choosing an action based on our evaluation of the data. The quality control application of Case 6.1 is one circumstance. In that application, the bottler needs to decide whether or not to make adjustments to the filling process based on a sample outcome. Consider another example. A producer of ball bearings and the consumer of the ball bearings agree that each shipment of bearings shall meet certain quality standards. When a shipment arrives, the consumer inspects a random sample of bearings from the thousands of bearings found in the shipment. On the basis of the sample outcome, the consumer either accepts or rejects the shipment. Let’s examine how the idea of inference as a decision changes the reasoning used in tests of significance.
Two types of error
Tests of significance concentrate on , the null hypothesis. If a decision is called for, however, there is no reason to single out . There are simply two hypotheses, and we must accept one and reject the other. It is convenient to call the two hypotheses and , but no longer has the special status (the statement we try to find evidence against) that it had in tests of significance. In the ball bearing problem, we must decide between
on the basis of a sample of bearings.
We hope that our decision will be correct, but sometimes it will be wrong. There are two types of incorrect decisions. We can accept a bad shipment of bearings, or we can reject a good shipment. Accepting a bad shipment leads to a variety of costs to the consumer (for example, machine breakdown due to faulty bearings or injury to end-product users such as skateboarders or bikers), while rejecting a good shipment hurts the producer. To help distinguish these two types of error, we give them specific names.
347
Type I and Type II Errors
If we reject (accept ) when in fact is true, this is a Type I error.
If we accept (reject ) when in fact is true, this is a Type II error.
The possibilities are summed up in Figure 6.20. If is true, our decision either is correct (if we accept ) or is a Type I error. If is true, our decision either is correct or is a Type II error. Only one error is possible at one time. Figure 6.21 applies these ideas to the ball bearing example.
Error probabilities
We can assess any rule for making decisions in terms of the probabilities of the two types of error. This is in keeping with the idea that statistical inference is based on probability. We cannot (short of inspecting the whole shipment) guarantee that good shipments of bearings will never be rejected and bad shipments will never be accepted. But by random sampling and the laws of probability, we can say what the probabilities of both kinds of error are.
348
Significance tests with fixed level give a rule for making decisions because the test either rejects or fails to reject it. If we adopt the decision-making way of thought, failing to reject means deciding to act as if is true. We can then describe the performance of a test by the probabilities of Type I and Type II errors.
EXAMPLE 6.28 Diameters of Bearings
The diameter of a particular precision ball bearing has a target value of 20 millimeters (mm) with tolerance limits of mm around the target. Suppose that the bearing diameters vary Normally with standard deviation of sixty-five hundred-thousandths of a millimeter, that is, mm. When a shipment of the bearings arrives, the consumer takes an SRS of five bearings from the shipment and measures their diameters. The consumer rejects the bearings if the sample mean diameter is signif-cantly different from 20 mm at the 5% significance level.
This is a test of the hypotheses
To carry out the test, the consumer computes the statistic:
and rejects if
A Type I error is to reject when in fact .
What about Type II errors? Because there are many values of in , we concentrate on one value. Based on the tolerance limits, the producer agrees that if there is evidence that the mean of ball bearings in the lot is 0.001 mm away from the desired mean of 20 mm, then the whole shipment should be rejected. So, a particular Type II error is to accept when in fact .
Figure 6.22 shows how the two probabilities of error are obtained from the two sampling distributions of , for and for . When , is true and to reject is a Type I error. When , accepting is a Type II error. We will now calculate these error probabilities.
349
The probability of a Type I error is the probability of rejecting when it is really true. In Example 6.28, this is the probability that when . But this is exactly the significance level of the test. The critical value 1.96 was chosen to make this probability 0.05, so we do not have to compute it again. The definition of “signifcant at level 0.05” is that sample outcomes this extreme will occur with probability 0.05 when is true.
Significance and Type I Error
The significance level of any fixed level test is the probability of a Type I error. That is, is the probability that the test will reject the null hypothesis when is in fact true.
The probability of a Type II error for the particular alternative in Example 6.28 is the probability that the test will fail to reject when has this alternative value. The power of the test for the alternative is just the probability that the test does reject when is true. By following the method of Example 6.26, we can calculate that the power is about 0.93. Therefore, the probability of a Type II error is equal to , or 0.07. It would also be the case that the probability of a Type II error is 0.07 if the value of the alternative is 19.999, that is, 0.001 less than the null hypothesis mean of 20.
Power and Type II Error
The power of a fixed level test for a particular alternative is 1 minus the probability of a Type II error for that alternative.
The two types of error and their probabilities give another interpretation of the significance level and power of a test. The distinction between tests of significance and tests as rules for deciding between two hypotheses lies, not in the calculations but in the reasoning that motivates the calculations. In a test of significance, we focus on a single hypothesis () and a single probability (the -value). The goal is to measure the strength of the sample evidence against . Calculations of power are done to check the sensitivity of the test. If we cannot reject , we conclude only that there is not sufficient evidence against , not that is actually true. If the same inference problem is thought of as a decision problem, we focus on two hypotheses and give a rule for deciding between them based on the sample evidence. We must therefore, focus equally on two probabilities—the probabilities of the two types of error. We must choose one or the other hypothesis and cannot abstain on grounds of insufficient evidence.
The common practice of testing hypotheses
Such a clear distinction between the two ways of thinking is helpful for understanding. In practice, the two approaches often merge. We continued to call one of the hypotheses in a decision problem . The common practice of testing hypotheses mixes the reasoning of significance tests and decision rules as follows:
350
Testing hypotheses may seem to be a hybrid approach. It was, historically, the effective beginning of decision-oriented ideas in statistics. An impressive mathematical theory of hypothesis testing was developed between 1928 and 1938 by Jerzy Neyman and Egon Pearson. The decision-making approach came later (1940s). Because decision theory in its pure form leaves you with two error probabilities and no simple rule on how to balance them, it has been used less often than either tests of significance or tests of hypotheses. Decision ideas have been applied in testing problems mainly by way of the Neyman-Pearson hypothesis-testing theory. That theory asks you first to choose, and the influence of Fisher has often led users of hypothesis testing comfortably back to or . Fisher, who was exceedingly argumentative, violently attacked the Neyman-Pearson decision-oriented ideas, and the argument still continues.