4.2 Sensation and Perception Are Distinct Activities

To us, sensation and perception appear to be one seamless event. However, they are two distinct activities.

You can enjoy a tempting ice cream sundae even if you do not know that its sweet taste depends on a complex process of transduction, in which molecules dissolved in saliva are converted to neural signals processed by the brain.
© ISTOCKPHOTO.COM/FOTOFLARE

As an example, your eyes are coursing across these sentences right now. The sensory receptors in your eyeballs are registering different patterns of light reflecting off the page. Your brain, however, is integrating and processing that light information into the meaningful perception of words, such as meaningful, perception, and words. Your eyes—the sensory organ—are not really seeing words; they are simply encoding different lines and curves on a page. Your brain—the perceptual organ—is transforming those lines and curves into a coherent mental representation of words and concepts.

What role does the brain play in what we see and hear?

If all of this sounds a little peculiar, it is because, from the vantage point of your conscious experience, it seems as if you are reading words directly. If you think of the discussion of brain damage in the Neuroscience and Behaviour chapter, you will recall that sometimes a person’s eyes can work just fine, yet the individual is still “blind” to faces she has seen for many years. Damage to the visual processing centres in the brain can interfere with the interpretation of information coming from the eyes: The senses are intact, but perceptual ability is compromised. Sensation and perception are related—but separate—events.

How do sensory receptors communicate with the brain? It all depends on the process of transduction, which occurs when sensors in the body convert physical signals from the environment into encoded neural signals sent to the central nervous system. In vision, light reflected from surfaces provides the eyes with information about the shape, colour, and position of objects. In audition, vibrations (from vocal cords or a guitar string, perhaps) cause changes in air pressure that propagate through space to a listener’s ears. In touch, the pressure of a surface against the skin signals its shape, texture, and temperature. In taste and smell, molecules dispersed in the air or dissolved in saliva reveal the identity of substances that we may or may not want to eat. In each case physical energy from the world is converted to neural energy inside the central nervous system (see TABLE 4.1). We will discuss in more detail how transduction works with each of the five primary senses—vision, hearing, touch, taste, and smell—in separate sections later in this chapter.

131

4.2.1 Psychophysics

Knowing that perception takes place in the brain, you might wonder if two people see the same colours in the sunset when looking at the evening sky. It is intriguing to consider the possibility that our basic perceptions of sights or sounds might differ fundamentally from those of other people. How can we measure such a thing objectively? Measuring the physical energy of a stimulus, such as the wavelength of a light, is easy enough: You can probably buy the necessary instruments online to do that yourself. But how do you quantify a person’s private, subjective perception of that light?

132

Table 4.1: Transduction

Why should a psychophysicist measure more than the strength of a stimulus?

A few early psychologists, such as Edward Titchener, tried using introspection to measure perceptual experiences (see the Psychology: Evolution of a Science chapter). They failed miserably at this task. After all, two people may both describe their experience of the sunset in the same words (“orange” and “beautiful”), but neither can directly perceive the other’s experience of the same event. In the mid-1800s, German scientist and philosopher Gustav Fechner (1801–1887) developed a new approach to measuring sensation and perception called psychophysics: methods that measure the strength of a stimulus and the observer’s sensitivity to that stimulus (Fechner, 1860/1966). In a typical psychophysics experiment, researchers ask people to make a simple judgment—whether or not they saw a flash of light, for example. The psychophysicist then relates the measured stimulus, such as the brightness of the light flash, to each observer’s yes-or-no response.

4.2.2 Measuring Thresholds

Figure 4.1: Absolute Threshold Some of us are more sensitive than others, and we may even detect sensory stimulation below our own absolute threshold. Absolute threshold is graphed here as the point where the increasing intensity of the stimulus enables an observer to detect it on 50 percent of the trials. As its intensity gradually increases, we detect the stimulation more frequently.

Psychophysicists begin the measurement process with a single sensory signal to determine precisely how much physical energy is required for an observer to become aware of a sensation. The simplest quantitative measurement in psychophysics is the absolute threshold the minimal intensity needed to just barely detect a stimulus in 50 percent of the trials. A threshold is a boundary. The doorway that separates the inside from the outside of a house is a threshold, as is the boundary between two psychological states (awareness and unawareness, for example). In finding the absolute threshold for sensation, the two states in question are perceiving and not perceiving some stimulus. TABLE 4.2 lists the approximate sensory thresholds for each of the five senses.

Table 4.2: Approximate Sensory Thresholds

To measure the absolute threshold for detecting a sound, for example, an observer sits in a soundproof room wearing headphones linked to a computer. The experimenter presents a pure tone (the sort of sound made by striking a tuning fork) using the computer to vary the loudness or the length of time each tone lasts and recording how often the observer reports hearing that tone under each condition. The outcome of such an experiment is graphed in FIGURE 4.1. Notice from the shape of the curve that the transition from not hearing to hearing is gradual rather than abrupt.

If we repeat this experiment for many different tones, we can observe and record the thresholds for tones ranging from very low to very high pitch. It turns out that people tend to be most sensitive to the range of tones corresponding to human conversation. If the tone is low enough, such as the lowest note on a pipe organ, most humans cannot hear it at all; we can only feel it. If the tone is high enough, we likewise cannot hear it, but dogs and many other animals can.

The absolute threshold is useful for assessing how sensitive we are to faint stimuli, but the human perceptual system is better at detecting changes in stimulation than the simple onset or offset of stimulation. When parents hear their infant’s cry, it is useful to be able to differentiate the “I am hungry” cry from the “I am cranky” cry from the “something is biting my toes” cry. The just noticeable difference (JND) is the minimal change in a stimulus that can just barely be detected. This is another kind of threshold: the difference threshold.

The JND is not a fixed quantity; rather, it depends on how intense the stimuli being measured are and on the particular sense being measured. Consider measuring the JND for a bright light. An observer in a dark room is shown a light of fixed intensity, called the standard (S), next to a comparison light that is slightly brighter or dimmer than the standard. When S is very dim, observers can see even a very small difference in brightness between the two lights: The JND is small. But if S is bright, a much larger increment is needed to detect the difference: The JND is larger.

133

What is the importance of proportion to the measurement of just noticeable difference?

When calculating a difference threshold, it is the proportion between stimuli that is important. This relationship was first noticed in 1834 by German physiologist Ernst Weber (Watson, 1978). Fechner applied Weber’s insight directly to psychophysics, resulting in a formal relationship called Weber's law: The just noticeable difference of a stimulus is a constant proportion despite variations in intensity. As an example, if you picked up a 20-g envelope, then a 40-g envelope, you would probably notice the difference between them. But if you picked up a 2-kg package, then a 2.02-kg package, you would probably detect no difference at all between them.

4.2.3 Signal Detection

Measuring absolute and difference thresholds requires a critical assumption: that a threshold exists! But much of what scientists know about biology suggests that such a discrete, all-or-none change in the brain is unlikely. Humans do not suddenly and rapidly switch between perceiving and not perceiving; in fact, recall that the transition from not sensing to sensing is gradual (see Figure 4.1). The very same physical stimulus, such as a dim light or a quiet tone, presented on several different occasions, may be perceived by the same person on some occasions but not on others. Remember, an absolute threshold is operationalized as perceiving the stimulus 50 percent of the time, which means the other 50 percent of the time it might go undetected.

How accurate and complete are our perceptions of the world?

Our accurate perception of a sensory stimulus, then, can be somewhat haphazard. Whether in the psychophysics lab or out in the world, sensory signals face a lot of competition, or noise, which refers to all the other stimuli coming from the internal and external environment. Memories, moods, and motives intertwine with what you are seeing, hearing, and smelling at any given time. This internal “noise” competes with your ability to detect a stimulus with perfect, focused attention. Other sights, sounds, and smells in the world at large also compete for attention; you rarely have the luxury of attending to just one stimulus apart from everything else. As a consequence of noise, you may not perceive everything that you sense, and you may even perceive things that you have not sensed. If you have ever been in the shower while eagerly anticipating a telephone call, you know how easy it is to “hear” a ringing telephone in the noise of the rushing water!

Crowds of people such as this one in a parade present our visual system with a challenging signal detection task.
BLICKWINKEL/ALAMY

An approach to psychophysics called signal detection theory holds that the response to a stimulus depends both on a person’s sensitivity to the stimulus in the presence of noise and on a person’s decision criterion. That is, observers consider the sensory evidence evoked by the stimulus and compare it to an internal decision criterion (Green & Swets, 1966; Macmillan & Creelman, 2005). If the sensory evidence exceeds the criterion, the observer responds by saying, “Yes, I detected the stimulus,” and if it falls short of the criterion, the observer responds by saying, “No, I did not detect the stimulus.” The four outcomes are summarized in FIGURE 4.2.

134

Signal detection theory allows researchers to quantify an observer’s response in the presence of noise. In a signal detection experiment, a stimulus, such as a dim light, is randomly presented or not. If you have ever taken an eye exam that checks your peripheral vision, you have an idea about this kind of setup: Lights of varying intensity are flashed at various places in the visual field, and your task is to respond anytime you see one. Observers in a signal detection experiment must decide whether they saw the light or not. If the light is presented and the observer correctly responds yes, the outcome is a hit. If the light is presented and the observer says no, the result is a miss. However, if the light is not presented and the observer nonetheless says it was, a false alarm has occurred. Finally, if the light is not presented and the observer responds no, a correct rejection has occurred: The observer accurately detected the absence of the stimulus.

Figure 4.2: Signal-Detection Experiment Outcomes This table represents the four possible outcomes of a signal-detection experiment. Either a signal is or is not present, and the observer can either detect it (“yes”) or not.

Signal detection theory proposes a way to measure perceptual sensitivity (how effectively the perceptual system represents sensory events) separately from the observer’s decision-making strategy. It explicitly takes into account observers’ response tendencies, such as liberally saying yes when there is any hint of a stimulus or conservatively reserving identifications only for obvious instances of the stimulus. Even when one person says yes much more often than another, both may be equally accurate in distinguishing between the presence or absence of a stimulus.

Signal detection theory has practical applications at home, school, work, and even while driving. For example, a radiologist may have to decide whether a mammogram shows that a woman has breast cancer. The radiologist knows that certain features, such as a mass of a particular size and shape, are associated with the presence of cancer. But noncancerous features can have a very similar appearance to cancerous ones. The radiologist may decide on a strictly liberal criterion and check every possible case of cancer with a biopsy. This decision strategy minimizes the possibility of missing a true cancer but leads to many unnecessary biopsies. A strictly conservative criterion will cut down on unnecessary biopsies but will miss some treatable cancers.

As another example, imagine that police are on the lookout for a suspected felon who they have reason to believe will be at a crowded soccer match. Although the law enforcement agency provided a fairly good description (white, 1.8 m (6 feet) tall, sandy brown hair, beard, glasses) there are still thousands of people to scan. Rounding up all men between 1.6 m (5′5″) and 2 m (6′5″) would probably produce a hit (the felon is caught) but at the expense of an extraordinary number of false alarms (many innocent people are detained and questioned).

These different types of errors have to be weighed against one another in setting the decision criterion. Signal detection theory offers a practical way to choose among criteria that permit decision makers to take into account the consequences of hits, misses, false alarms, and correct rejections (McFall & Treat, 1999; Swets, Dawes, & Monahan, 2000). For an example of a common everyday task that can interfere with signal detection, see The Real World box.

4.2.4 Sensory Adaptation

According to the theory of natural selection, inherited characteristics that provide a survival advantage tend to spread throughout the population across generations. Why might sensory adaptation have evolved? What survival benefits might it confer to a small animal trying to avoid predators? To a predator trying to hunt prey?
© CLÉMENT PHILIPPE/ARTERRA PICTURE LIBRARY/ALAMY

When you walk into a bakery, the aroma of freshly baked bread overwhelms you, but after a few minutes the smell fades. If you dive into cold water, the temperature is shocking at first, but after a few minutes you get used to it. When you wake up in the middle of the night for a drink of water, the bathroom light blinds you, but after a few minutes you no longer squint. These are all examples of sensory adaptation: Sensitivity to prolonged stimulation tends to decline over time as an organism adapts to current conditions.

135

What conditions have you already adapted to today? Sounds? Smells?

Sensory adaptation is a useful process for most organisms. Imagine what your sensory and perceptual world would be like without it. When you put on your jeans in the morning, the feeling of rough cloth against your bare skin would be as noticeable hours later as it was in the first few minutes. The stink of garbage in your apartment when you first walk in would never dissipate. If you had to be constantly aware of how your tongue feels while it is resting in your mouth, you would be driven to distraction. Sensory adaptation is generally due to neural fatigue—the cells that are firing to signal the stimulus get tired after a time, and stop firing—but as far as your body is concerned, the signal has stopped. If the cells have a chance to rest, they will signal again as strongly as ever. So after the smell of freshly baked bread has faded, if you leave the bakery for a few minutes, and then re-enter the bakery, the smell should be just as strong as it was at first, before fading again. Another phenomenon, called habituation, also results in sensations fading with time. But habituation is due to learning. If you move to an apartment on a busy street with a lot of traffic noise, your sleep might be disturbed for a few nights. But soon you are sleeping soundly again. Your perceptual system has learned to disregard the noise. You will learn more about habituation in the Learning chapter.

  • Sensation and perception are critical to survival. Sensation is the simple stimulation of a sense organ, whereas perception organizes, identifies, and interprets sensation at the level of the brain.

  • All of our senses depend on the process of transduction, which converts physical signals from the environment into neural signals carried by sensory neurons into the central nervous system.

  • In the nineteenth century, researchers developed psychophysics, an approach to studying perception that measures the strength of a stimulus and an observer’s sensitivity to that stimulus. Psychophysicists have developed procedures for measuring an observer’s absolute threshold, or the smallest intensity needed to just barely detect a stimulus, and the just noticeable difference (JND), or the smallest change in a stimulus that can just barely be detected.

  • Signal detection theory allows researchers to distinguish between an observer’s perceptual sensitivity to a stimulus and criteria for making decisions about the stimulus.

  • Sensory adaptation occurs because sensitivity to unchanging stimulation tends to decline over time due to neural fatigue.