Infancy is the stage of development that begins at birth and lasts between 18 and 24 months. Although infants seem to be capable of little more than squalling and squirming, research shows that they are much more sophisticated than they appear.
The stage of development that begins at birth and lasts between 18 and 24 months.
318
What do newborns see?
New parents like to stand around the crib and make goofy faces at the baby because they think the baby will be amused. What they don’t know is that newborns have a rather limited range of vision. The level of detail that a newborn can see at a distance of 20 feet is roughly equivalent to the level of detail that an adult can see at 600 feet (Banks & Salapatek, 1983), which is to say that they are missing out on a lot of the cribside shenanigans. On the other hand, when stimuli are 8 to 12 inches away (about the distance between a nursing infant’s eyes and its mother’s face), newborns can see pretty well. How do we know? In one study, newborns were shown a circle with diagonal stripes over and over again. The infants stared a lot at first and then less and less on each subsequent presentation. Recall from the Learning chapter that habituation is the tendency for organisms to respond less intensely to a stimulus the more frequently they are exposed to it, and infants habituate just like the rest of us do. But when the researchers rotated the circle 90°, the newborns once again stared intently, indicating that they had noticed the change in the circle’s orientation (Slater, Morison, & Somers, 1988).
Newborns are especially attentive to social stimuli. For example, in one study, researchers stood close to some newborns while sticking out their tongues and stood close to other newborns while pursing their lips. Newborns in the first group stuck out their own tongues more often than those in the second group did, and newborns in the second group pursed their lips more often than those in the first group did (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977). Indeed, newborns have been shown to mimic facial expressions in their very first hour of life (Reissland, 1988) and to mimic speech sounds as early as 12 weeks (Kuhl & Meltzoff, 1996).
Although infants can use their eyes right away, they must spend considerably more time learning how to use their other parts. Motor development is the emergence of the ability to execute physical actions such as reaching, grasping, crawling, and walking. Motor behavior starts with a small set of reflexes, which are specific patterns of motor response that are triggered by specific patterns of sensory stimulation. For example, the rooting reflex is the tendency for infants to move their mouths toward any object that touches their cheek, and the sucking reflex is the tendency to suck any object that enters their mouths. These two reflexes allow newborns to find their mother’s nipple and begin feeding—
The emergence of the ability to execute physical action.
Specific patterns of motor response that are triggered by specific patterns of sensory stimulation.
Why are infants born with reflexes?
In what order do infants learn to use parts of their bodies?
The development of more sophisticated motor behavior tends to obey two general rules. The first is the cephalocaudal rule (or the “top-
The “top-
The “inside-
319
What are the three essential tasks of cognitive development?
In the first half of the 20th century, a Swiss biologist named Jean Piaget noticed that when asked certain kinds of questions (e.g., “Does the big glass have more liquid in it than the small glass? Can Billy see what you see?”), children of the same age gave the same wrong answers. And as they aged, they started giving the right answers at about the same time. This led Piaget to suggest that children move through discrete stages of cognitive development, which is the emergence of the ability to think and understand. Between infancy and adulthood, children must come to understand three important things: (a) how the world works, (b) how their minds represent that world, and (c) how other minds represent that world. Let’s see how children accomplish these three essential tasks.
The emergence of the ability to think and understand.
320
Piaget (1954) suggested that cognitive development occurs in four stages which he called the sensorimotor stage, the preoperational stage, the concrete operational stage, and the formal operational stage (see TABLE 10.1). The sensorimotor stage is a period of development that begins at birth and lasts through infancy. As its name suggests, infants at this stage are mainly busy using their ability to sense and their ability to move to acquire information about the world. By actively exploring their environments with their eyes, mouths, and fingers, infants begin to construct schemas, which are theories about of the way the world works.
What happens at the sensorimotor stage?
Stage |
Characteristic |
---|---|
Sensorimotor (Birth– |
Infant experiences world through movement and sense, develops schemas, begins to act intentionally, and shows evidence of understanding object permanence. |
Preoperational (2– |
Child acquires motor skills but does not understand conservation of physical properties. Child begins this stage of thinking egocentrically but ends with a basic understanding of other minds. |
Concrete operational (6– |
Child can think logically about physical objects and events and understands conservation of physical properties. |
Formal operational (11 years and up) |
Child can think logically about abstract propositions and hypotheticals. |
A stage of development that begins at birth and lasts through infancy.
Theories about the way the world works.
As every scientist knows, the key advantage of having a theory is that it can be used to predict what will happen in novel situations. If an infant learns that tugging at a stuffed animal causes the toy to come closer, then that observation is incorporated into the infant’s theory about how physical objects behave when pulled, and the infant can later use that theory when he or she wants a different object to come closer, such as a rattle or a ball. Piaget called this assimilation, which is the process by which infants apply their schemas in novel situations. Of course, if the infant tugs the tail of the family cat, the cat is likely to sprint in the opposite direction. Because an infants’ theories about the world (“Things come closer if I pull them”) are occasionally disconfirmed, infants must occasionally adjust their schemas in light of new experiences (“Aha! Only inanimate things come closer when I pull them”). Piaget called this accommodation, which is the process by which infants revise their schemas to take new information into account.
The process by which infants apply their schemas in novel situations.
The process by which infants revise their schemas in light of new information.
Piaget suggested that infants lack some very basic understandings about the physical world and therefore must acquire them through experience. For example, when you put your shoes in the closet, you know that they exist even after you close the closet door, and you would be rather surprised if you opened the door a moment later and found the closet empty. But according to Piaget, this wouldn’t surprise an infant because infants do not have a theory of object permanence, which is the belief that objects exist even when they are not visible. But modern research suggests that infants may acquire a sense of object permanence much earlier than Piaget realized (Shinskey & Munakata, 2005).
The belief that objects exist even when they are not visible.
When do children acquire a theory of object permanence?
For instance, in one study, infants were shown a miniature drawbridge that flipped up and down (see FIGURE 10.3). Once the infants got used to this, they watched as a box was placed behind the drawbridge—
321
A Statistician in the Crib
A magician asks you to shuffle a deck of cards and then name your favorite. Then he dons a blindfold, reaches out his hand, and pulls your favorite card from the deck. You are astonished—
Would that trick astonish an infant? That’s pretty hard to imagine. After all, to appreciate the trick, one has to understand a basic rule of statistics—
In one study, researchers showed infants two boxes: One had mostly pink balls and just a few yellows; the other had mostly yellow balls with a few pinks. The infants then watched as an experimenter closed her eyes and reached into the mostly pink box, pulled out some balls, and deposited them in a little container in front of the infant. Sometimes she deposited four pinks and a yellow, and sometimes she deposited four yellows and a pink. What did the infants do?
When the experimenter pulled mainly pink balls from a mainly pink box, the infants glanced and then looked away. But when she pulled mainly yellow balls from a mainly pink box, they stared like bystanders at a train wreck. The fact that infants looked longer at the improbable sample than at the probable sample suggests that they found the former more astonishing; in other words, they had some basic understanding of how random sampling works.
This study—
322
The long period following infancy is called childhood, which is the stage of development that begins at about 18 to 24 months and lasts until about 11 to 14 years. According to Piaget, children enter childhood at the preoperational stage, which is the stage of cognitive development that begins at about 2 years and ends at about 6 years, during which children develop a preliminary understanding of the physical world. They then pass through the concrete operational stage, which is the stage of cognitive development that begins at about 6 years and ends at about 11 years, during which children learn how actions or “operations” can transform the “concrete” objects of the physical world.
The stage of development that begins at about 18 to 24 months and lasts until about 11 or 14 years.
The stage of cognitive development that begins at about 2 years and ends at about 6 years, during which children develop a preliminary understanding of the physical world.
The stage of cognitive development that begins at about 6 years and ends at about 11 years, during which children learn how actions or “operations” can transform the “concrete” objects of the physical world.
The difference between these stages is nicely illustrated by one of Piaget’s clever experiments in which he showed children a row of cups and asked them to place an egg in each. Preoperational children were able to do this, and afterward they readily agreed that there were just as many eggs as there were cups. Then Piaget removed the eggs and spread them out in a long line that extended beyond the row of cups. Preoperational children incorrectly claimed that there were now more eggs than cups, pointing out that the row of eggs was longer than the row of cups and hence there must be more of them. Concrete operational children, on the other hand, correctly reported that the number of eggs did not change when they were spread out in a longer line. They understood that quantity is a property of a set of concrete objects that does not change when an operation such as spreading out alters the set’s appearance (Piaget, 1954). Piaget called the child’s insight conservation, which is the notion that basic properties of an object do not change despite changes in the object’s appearance.
The notion that the quantitative properties of an object are invariant despite changes in the object’s appearance.
What distinguishes the preoperational and concrete operational stages?
The main reason why preoperational children do not fully grasp the notion of conservation is that they do not fully grasp the fact that they have minds and that these minds contain mental representations of the world. As adults, we naturally distinguish between appearances and realities. We realize that things aren’t always as they seem: A wagon can be red but look gray at dusk, and a highway can be dry but look wet in the heat. Visual illusions delight us precisely because we know that things look like this but are really like that. Preoperational children don’t make this distinction. When something looks gray or wet, they assume it is gray or wet.
But as children move from the preoperational to the concrete operational stage, they begin to realize that the way the world appears is not necessarily the way the world really is. For instance, concrete operational children can understand that when a ball of clay is rolled, stretched, or flattened, it is still the same amount of clay despite the fact that it looks larger in one form than in another. They can understand that when water is poured from a short, wide beaker into a tall, thin cylinder, it is still the same amount of water despite the fact that the water level in the cylinder is higher. Once children can make a distinction between objects and their mental representations of those objects, they begin to understand that certain operations—
Once children are at the concrete operational stage, they can readily solve physical problems involving egg-
The final stage of cognitive development that begins around the age of 11, during which children learn to reason about abstract concepts.
323
What is the essential feature of the formal operational stage?
As children develop, they discover their own minds, but they also discover the minds of others. Because preoperational children don’t fully grasp the fact that they have minds that mentally represent objects, they also don’t fully grasp the fact that other people have minds that often represent the same objects in different ways. As such, preoperational children generally expect others to see the world as they do. Egocentrism is the failure to understand that the world appears different to different people. Egocentrism is a hallmark of the preoperational stage, and it reveals itself in a variety of interesting ways. When 3-
The failure to understand that the world appears different to different observers.
What does the false-
Although very young children do not fully understand that others have different perceptions or beliefs than they do, they do seem to understand that others have different desires. For example, a 2-
Eventually, the vast majority of children come to understand that they and others have minds and that these minds represent the world in different ways. Once children understand these things, they are said to have acquired a theory of mind, which is the understanding that other people’s mental representations guide their behavior. The age at which children acquire a theory of mind appears to be influenced by a variety of factors, such as the number of siblings the child has, the frequency with which the child engages in pretend play, whether the child has an imaginary companion, and the socioeconomic status of the child’s family. But of all the factors researchers have studied, language seems to be the most important (Astington & Baird, 2005). Children’s language skills are an excellent predictor of how well they perform on false-
The understanding that other people’s mental representations guide their behavior.
324
Which children have special difficulty acquiring a theory of mind?
What did Piaget get wrong?
Two groups of children lag far behind their peers in acquiring a theory of mind. Children with autism (a disorder we’ll cover in more depth in the Disorders chapter) typically have difficulty communicating with other people and making friends, and some psychologists have suggested that this is because they have trouble acquiring a theory of mind (Frith, 2003). Although children with autism are typically normal on most intellectual dimensions—
Cognitive development is a complex journey, and Piaget’s ideas about it were nothing short of groundbreaking. Few psychologists have had such a profound impact on the field. Many of these ideas have held up quite well, but in the last few decades, psychologists have discovered two general ways in which Piaget got it wrong. First, Piaget thought that children graduated from one stage to another in the same way that they graduated from kindergarten to first grade: A child is in kindergarten or first grade, he is never in both, and there is an exact moment of transition to which everyone can point. Modern psychologists see development as more fluid and continuous—
325
Piaget saw the child as a lone scientist who made observations, developed theories, and then revised those theories in light of new observations. And yet, most scientists don’t start from scratch. Rather, they receive training from more experienced scientists. According to Piaget’s contemporary, the Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky, children do much the same thing. Vygotsky believed that cognitive development was largely the result of the child’s interaction with members of his or her own culture rather than his or her interaction with concrete objects (Vygotsky, 1978).
For example, in English, the names of numbers beyond 20 follow a logical pattern: twenty followed by a digit (twenty-
How does culture affect cognitive development?
Counting is just one of the many things that children learn from others. In fact, human beings are better than any other animal on earth at learning from other members of their own species, and that’s because they have three important skills that most other animals lack (Meltzoff et al., 2009; Striano & Reid, 2006).
Joint attention (“I see what you see”), imitation (“I do what you do”), and social referencing (“I think what you think”) are three of the basic abilities that allow infants to learn from other members of their species.
326
Walk This Way
Parents often complain that their children won’t take their advice. But research shows that even 18-
Researchers (Tamis-
So what did the infants do? Did they trust their mothers or did they trust their eyes? As you can see in the figure below, when the inclined plane was clearly safe or clearly risky, infants ignored their mothers. They typically trotted down the flat plane even when mom advised against it and refused to try the risky plane even when mom said it was okay. But when the plane was somewhere between safe and risky, the infants tended to follow mom’s advice.
These data show that infants use social information in a very sophisticated way. When their senses provide unambiguous information about the world, they ignore what people tell them. But when their senses leave them unsure about what to do, they readily accept parental advice. It appears that from the moment children start to walk, they know when to listen to their parents and when to shake their heads, roll their eyes, and do what they darn well please.
327
Unlike baby turtles, baby humans cannot survive without their caregivers. But what exactly do caregivers provide? Some obvious answers are warmth, safety, and food—
During World War II, psychologists studied infants who were living in orphanages while awaiting adoption. Although these children were warm, safe, and wellfed, many were developmentally impaired, both physically and psychologically (Spitz, 1949). A few years later, psychologist Harry Harlow (1958; Harlow & Harlow, 1965) discovered that infant rhesus monkeys that were warm, safe, and well fed but that were not allowed any social contact for the first 6 months of their lives developed a variety of behavioral abnormalities. They compulsively rocked back and forth while biting themselves, and if they were introduced to other monkeys, they avoided them entirely. These socially isolated monkeys turned out to be incapable of communicating with or learning from others of their kind, and when the females matured and became mothers, they ignored, rejected, and sometimes even attacked their own infants. Harlow also discovered that when socially isolated monkeys were put in a cage with two “artificial mothers”—one that was made of wire and dispensed food and one that was made of cloth and dispensed no food—
Few things are cuter than a string of ducklings following their mother. But how do they know who their mother is? In a series of studies, the biologist Konrad Lorenz discovered that ducklings don’t actually know who their mothers are at all. Rather, they simply follow the first moving object they see after they are born—
Psychiatrist John Bowlby was fascinated by Lorenz’s work, as well as by Harlow’s studies of rhesus monkeys reared in isolation and the work on children reared in orphanages, and he sought to understand how human infants form attachments to their caregivers (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980). Bowlby began by noting that from the moment they are born, ducks waddle after their mothers and monkeys cling to their mothers’ furry chests because the newborns of both species must stay close to their caregivers to survive. Human infants, he suggested, have a similar need, but because they are much less physically developed, they can’t waddle or cling. So instead they smile and cry. When an infant cries, gurgles, coos, makes eye contact, or smiles, most adults reflexively move toward the infant, and Bowlby suggested that this is why infants have been designed to emit these signals.
328
According to Bowlby, infants initially send these signals to anyone within visual or auditory range. For the first 6 months or so, they keep a “mental tally” of who responds most promptly and often to their signals, and soon they begin to target the best and fastest responder, also known as the primary caregiver. This person quickly becomes the emotional center of the infant’s universe. Infants feel secure in the primary caregiver’s presence and will happily crawl around, exploring their environments with their eyes, ears, fingers, and mouths. But if their primary caregiver gets too far away, infants begin to feel insecure, and they take action to decrease the distance between themselves and their primary caregiver, perhaps by crawling toward their caregiver or perhaps by crying until their caregiver moves toward them. Human infants, Bowlby suggested, are predisposed to form an attachment—that is, an emotional bond with a primary caregiver.
An emotional bond with a primary caregiver.
How does an infant “decide” to whom he or she should become attached?
Of course, not all emotional bonds are of the same kind. Research suggests that most infants show one of four basic patterns of attachment, known as attachment styles (Ainsworth et al., 1978), which can be inferred by watching how they respond when their caregivers leave them alone for a few minutes and then return:
Where do these attachment styles come from? Childrens’ attachment styles are determined in part by their temperament, or characteristic pattern of emotional reactivity (Thomas & Chess, 1977). Whether measured by parents’ reports or by physiological indices such as heart rate or cerebral blood flow, very young children vary in their tendency toward fearfulness, irritability, activity, positive affect, and other emotional traits (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). These temperamental differences among infants appear to result partly from innate biological differences (Baker et al., 2013). But culture also plays a role in determining attachment style. For example, although the secure attachment style is the most common one among infants of all cultures (van IJzendoorn & Kroonenberg, 1988), German children (whose parents tend to foster independence) are more likely to have avoidant than ambivalent attachment styles, and Japanese children (whose mothers typically stay home and do not leave them in the care of others) are more likely to have ambivalent than avoidant attachment styles (Takahashi, 1986).
Characteristic patterns of emotional reactivity.
Although biology and culture both play a role, for the most part a child’s attachment style is determined by interactions with his or her primary caregiver. Studies have shown that mothers of securely attached infants tend to be especially sensitive to signs of their child’s emotional state, especially good at detecting their infant’s “request” for reassurance, and especially responsive to that request (Ainsworth et al., 1978; De Wolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997). In contrast, mothers of infants with an ambivalent attachment style tend to respond inconsistently, only sometimes attending to infants who are showing signs of distress.
How do caregivers influence an infant’s attachment style?
329
As a result of their interactions with their caregivers, infants develop an internal working model of relationships, which is a set of beliefs about the self, the primary caregiver, and the relationship between them (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999). Secure infants seem certain that their primary caregiver will respond when they feel distressed; avoidant infants seem certain that their primary caregiver will not respond when they are distressed; and ambivalent infants seem uncertain about whether their primary caregiver will respond. Infants with a disorganized attachment style seem to be confused about their caregivers, which has led some psychologists to speculate that this style primarily characterizes children who have been abused (Carolson, 1998; Cicchetti & Toth, 1998).
A set of beliefs about the self, the primary caregiver, and the relationship between them.
Differences in how caregivers respond are largely due to differences in their ability to read their infants’ emotional states. Mothers who think of their infants as unique individuals with emotional lives and not just as creatures with urgent physical needs are more likely to have securely attached infants (Meins, 2003; Meins et al., 2001)? It appears so. Mothers whose infants were particularly irritable and difficult participated in a training program designed to sensitize them to their infants’ emotional signals and to encourage them to be more responsive to their infants. A year or two later, those infants whose mothers had received the training were considerably more likely to have a secure attachment style than were those whose mothers did not receive the training (van den Boom, 1994, 1995).
This is potentially good news, because children and adults who were securely attached as infants have greater psychological well-
From the moment of birth, human beings can make one distinction quickly and well, and that’s the distinction between pleasure and pain. Before they hit their very first diapers, infants can tell when something feels good or bad, and they can demonstrate to anyone within earshot that they strongly prefer the former. Over the next few years, they begin to notice that their pleasures (“Throwing food is fun”) are often someone else’s pains (“Throwing food makes Mom mad”), which is a bit of a problem because infants need these other people (and especially their caregivers) to survive. So they start to learn how to balance their needs and the needs of those around them, and they do this in part by developing the distinction between right and wrong.
330
How do children think about right and wrong? Piaget identified three ways in which children’s moral thinking shifts as they grow and develop (Piaget, 1932/1965).
According to Piaget, what three shifts characterize moral development?
Psychologist Lawrence Kohlberg used Piaget’s insights to produce a more detailed theory of the development of moral reasoning (Kohlberg, 1958, 1963, 1986). He based his theory on people’s responses to a series of dilemmas like this one:
A woman was near death from a special kind of cancer. The druggist in town had recently discovered a new drug that might save her. The druggist was charging $2,000 a dose, even though it only cost him $200 to make. The sick woman’s husband, Heinz, only had $1,000. He told the druggist that his wife was dying and asked him to sell the drug cheaper. But the druggist said: “No, I discovered the drug and I’m going to make money from it.” So Heinz got desperate and broke into the drugstore to steal the drug for his wife. Should Heinz have done that?
According to Kohlberg, people’s answers to this question reveal that moral reasoning develops in three stages:
A stage of moral development in which the morality of an action is primarily determined by its consequences for the actor.
A stage of moral development in which the morality of an action is primarily determined by the extent to which it conforms to social rules.
A stage of moral development at which the morality of an action is determined by a set of general principles that reflect core values.
331
What was Kohlberg right about and wrong about?
Research supports Kohlberg’s general claim that moral reasoning shifts from an emphasis on punishment to an emphasis on social rules and finally to an emphasis on ethical principles (Walker, 1988). But research also suggests that these stages are not quite as discrete as Kohlberg thought. For instance, a single person may use preconventional, conventional, and postconventional thinking in different circumstances, which suggests that the developing person does not “reach a stage” so much as “acquires a skill” that may or may not be used on a particular occasion. Others have criticized Kohlberg’s theory on the grounds that it doesn’t apply well to non-
Research on moral reasoning suggests that people are like judges in a court of law, using rational analysis—
You are standing on a bridge. Below you see a runaway trolley hurtling down the track toward five people who will be killed if it remains on its present course. You can save these people by flipping a lever that will switch the trolley onto a different track, where it will kill just one person instead of five. Is it morally permissible to divert the trolley and prevent five deaths at the cost of one?
Now consider a slightly different version of this problem:
You and a large man are standing on a bridge. Below you see a runaway trolley hurtling down the track toward five people who will be killed if it remains on its present course. You can save these people by pushing the large man onto the track, where his body will be caught up in the trolley’s wheels and stop it before it kills the five people. Is it morally permissible to push the large man and prevent five deaths at the cost of one?
These scenarios are illustrated in FIGURE 10.6. If you are like most people, you are more likely to think it is morally permissible to pull a switch than to push a man (Greene et al., 2001). And yet, both cases involve killing one person to save five, so how did your moral reasoning lead you to reach such different conclusions? The answer is that you probably didn’t reach these conclusions by moral reasoning at all. Rather, you simply had a strong negative emotional reaction to the thought of pushing another human being into the path of an oncoming trolley and watching him get sliced and diced, and that emotional reaction instantly led you to conclude that pushing him must be wrong. Sure, you came up with a few good arguments to support this conclusion (“What if he turned around and bit me?” or “I’d hate to get spleen all over my new shoes”), but those arguments probably followed your conclusion rather than preceding it (Greene, 2013).
332
Do moral judgments come before or after emotional reactions?
The way people respond to cases such as these has convinced some psychologists that moral judgments are the consequences—
What happens when we see others suffer?
The moral intuitionist perspective suggests that we consider it immoral to push someone onto the tracks simply because the idea of watching someone suffer makes us feel bad (Greene et al., 2001). In fact, research has shown that watching someone suffer activates many of the same brain regions that are activated when we suffer ourselves (Carr et al., 2003; see the discussion of mirror neurons in the Neuroscience and Behavior chapter). In one study, women received a shock or watched their romantic partners receive a shock on different parts of their bodies. The regions of the women’s brains that processed information about the location of the shock were activated only when the women experienced the shock themselves, but the regions that processed emotional information were activated whether the women received the shock or observed it (Singer et al., 2004). The fact that we can actually feel another person’s suffering may explain why even a small child who is incapable of sophisticated moral reasoning still considers it wrong when someone hurts someone else, especially when the person being hurt is similar to the child (Hamlin et al., 2013). Indeed, even very young children say that hitting is wrong even when an adult instructs someone to do it (Laupa & Turiel, 1986). It appears that from a very early age, other people’s suffering can become our suffering, and this leads us to conclude that the actions that caused the suffering are immoral.
333
1. | Piaget believed that infants construct ______, which are theories about the way the world works. |
c.
2. | Once children understand that human behavior is guided by mental representations, they are said to have acquired |
b.
3. | When infants in a new situation examine their mother’s face for cues about what to do, they are demonstrating an ability known as |
b.
4. | The capacity for attachment may be innate, but the quality of attachment is influenced by |
d.