Imagine writing a story about the people you know. To capture their special qualities, you might describe their traits: Keesha is friendly, aggressive, and domineering; Seth is flaky, humorous, and superficial. The trait approach to personality uses such trait terms to characterize differences among individuals. In attempting to create manageable and meaningful sets of descriptors, trait theorists face two significant challenges: narrowing down the almost infinite set of adjectives and answering the more basic question of why people have particular traits and whether they arise from biological or hereditary foundations.
354
One way to think about personality is as a combination of traits. This was the approach of Gordon Allport (1937), one of the first trait theorists, who believed people could be described in terms of traits just as an object could be described in terms of its properties. He saw a trait as a relatively stable disposition to behave in a particular and consistent way. For example, a person who keeps his books organized alphabetically in bookshelves, hangs his clothing neatly in the closet, and keeps a clear agenda in a daily planner can be said to have the trait of orderliness. This trait consistently manifests itself in a variety of settings.
A relatively stable disposition to behave in a particular and consistent way.
How might traits explain behavior?
The orderliness trait describes a person but doesn’t explain his or her behavior. Why does the person behave in this way? There are two basic ways in which a trait might serve as an explanation: The trait may be a preexisting disposition of the person that causes the person’s behavior, or it may be a motivation that guides the person’s behavior. Allport saw traits as preexisting dispositions, causes of behavior that reliably trigger the behavior. The person’s orderliness, for example, is an inner property of the person that will cause the person to straighten things up and be tidy in a wide array of situations. Other personality theorists suggested instead that traits reflect motives. Just as a hunger motive might explain someone’s many trips to the snack bar, a need for orderliness might explain the neat closet and alphabetically organized bookshelves (Murray & Kluckhohn, 1953). Researchers examining traits as causes have used personality inventories to measure them, whereas those examining traits as motives have more often used projective tests.
Picking a single trait such as orderliness and studying it in depth doesn’t get us very far in the search for the core of human character: the basic set of traits that defines how humans differ from one another. How have researchers tried to discover the core personality traits?
The study of core traits began with an exploration of how personality is represented in the store of wisdom we call language. Generation after generation, people have described people with words, so early psychologists proposed that core traits could be discerned by finding the main themes in all the adjectives used to describe personality. In one such analysis, a painstaking count of relevant words in a dictionary of English resulted in a list of over 18,000 potential traits (Allport & Odbert, 1936)! Attempts to narrow down the list to a more manageable set depend on the idea that traits might be related in a hierarchical pattern (see FIGURE 11.3), with more general or abstract traits at higher levels than more specific or concrete traits. The highest-
But how many factors are there? Different researchers have proposed different answers. Cattell (1950) proposed a 16-
355
Today, most researchers agree that personality is best captured by 5 factors rather than by 2, 3, 16, or 18,000 (John & Srivastava, 1999; McCrae & Costa, 1999). The Big Five, as they are affectionately called, are the traits of the five-
The traits of the five-
|
High on Trait |
Low on Trait |
---|---|---|
Openness to experience |
imaginative..................down- |
|
Conscientiousness |
organized........................disorganized |
|
Extraversion |
social.........................................retiring |
|
Agreeableness |
softhearted.............................ruthless |
|
Neuroticism |
worried..........................................calm |
|
Source: Data from McCrae & Costa, 1990, 1999. |
356
Personality on the Surface
When you judge someone as friend or foe, interesting or boring, how do you do it? It’s nice to think that your impressions of personality are based on solid foundations. You wouldn’t judge personality based on something as shallow as someone’s looks, or what’s on his Facebook page, would you? These things may seem to be flimsy bases for understanding personality, but it turns out that some remarkably accurate personality judgments can be made from exactly such superficial cues.
Recent studies have shown that extraverts smile more than others and appear more stylish and healthy (Naumann et al., 2009), and people high in openness to experience are more likely to have tattoos and other body modifications (Nathanson, Paulhus, & Williams, 2006). Findings like these suggest that people can manipulate their surface identities to try to make desired impressions on others and that surface signs of personality might therefore be false or misleading. However, one recent study of people’s Facebook pages, which are clearly surface expressions of personality intended for others to see, found that the personalities people project online are more highly related to their real personalities than to the personalities they describe as their ideals (Back et al., 2010). The signs of personality that appear on the surface may be more than skin deep.
Going one step further, it turns out that people’s Facebook activity is significantly associated with their self-
Stability of Personality Traits over Time
www.macmillanhighered.com/
What are the strengths of the five-
Interestingly, research on the Big Five has shown that people’s personalities tend to remain fairly stable through a lifetime: Scores at one time in life correlate strongly with scores at later dates, even decades later (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005). William James offered the opinion that “in most of us, by the age of thirty, the character has set like plaster, and will never soften again” (James, 1890, p. 121), but this turns out to be too strong a view. Some variability is typical in childhood, and though there is less in adolescence, some personality change can even occur in adulthood for some people (Srivistava et al., 2003).
Can we explain why a person has a stable set of personality traits? Many trait theorists have argued that unchangeable brain and biological processes produce the remarkable stability of traits over the life span. Brain damage certainly can produce personality change, as the classic case of Phineas Gage so vividly demonstrates (see the Neuroscience and Behavior chapter). You may recall that after the blasting accident that blew an iron rod through his frontal lobes, Gage showed a dramatic loss of social appropriateness and conscientiousness (Damasio, 1994). In fact, when someone experiences a profound change in personality, testing often reveals the presence of such brain pathologies as Alzheimer’s disease, stroke, or brain tumor (Feinberg, 2001). The administration of antidepressant medication and other pharmaceutical treatments that change brain chemistry also can trigger personality changes, making people, for example, somewhat more extraverted and less neurotic (Bagby et al., 1999; Knutson et al., 1998).
357
Some of the most compelling evidence for the importance of biological factors in personality comes from the domain of behavioral genetics. Simply put, the more genes you have in common with someone, the more similar your personalities are likely to be. For example, in one review of studies involving over 24,000 twin pairs (Loehlin, 1992), identical twins (who share 100% of their genes) proved markedly more similar to each other in personality than did fraternal twins (who share on average only 50% of their genes). And identical twins reared apart in adoptive families end up at least as similar in personality as those who grew up together (McGue & Bouchard, 1998; Tellegen et al., 1988). These and other studies suggest that simply growing up in the same family does not make people very similar. Rather, when two siblings are similar, this is thought to be largely due to genetic similarities.
Are There “Male” and “Female” Personalities?
Is there a typical female personality or a typical male personality? Researchers have found some reliable differences between men and women with respect to their self-
An evolutionary perspective on gender differences in personality holds that men and women have evolved different personality characteristics in part because their reproductive success depends on different behaviors. For instance, aggressiveness in men may have an adaptive value in intimidating sexual rivals; women who are agreeable and nurturing may have evolved to protect and ensure the survival of their offspring (Campbell, 1999) as well as to secure a reliable mate and provider (Buss, 1989).
In contrast, according to social role theory, personality differences between men and women result from cultural standards and expectations that assign the male and female roles (Eagly & Wood, 1999). Because of their physical size and their freedom from childbearing, men historically took roles of greater power—
Regardless of the source of gender differences in personality, the degree to which people identify personally with masculine and feminine stereotypes may tell us about important personality differences between individuals. Sandra Bem (1974) designed a scale (the Bem Sex Role Inventory) that assesses the degree of identification with stereotypically masculine and feminine traits. Bem suggested that psychologically androgynous people (those who adopt the best of both worlds and identify with positive feminine traits such as kindness and positive masculine traits such as assertiveness) might be better adjusted than people who identify strongly with only one sex role.
358
What do studies of twins tell us about personality?
People who share genes often have striking similarities in behavior and attitude. One study that examined 3,000 pairs of identical and fraternal twins found evidence for the genetic transmission of conservative views regarding topics such as socialism, church authority, the death penalty, and mixed-
What neurophysiological mechanisms might influence the development of personality traits? Eysenck (1967) speculated that extraversion and introversion might arise from individual differences in cortical arousal. Eysenck suggested that extraverts pursue stimulation because their reticular formation (the part of the brain that regulates arousal or alertness, as described in the Neuroscience and Behavior chapter) is not easily stimulated. To achieve greater cortical arousal and feel fully alert, Eysenck argued, extraverts seek out social interaction, parties, and other activities to achieve mental stimulation. In contrast, introverts may prefer reading or quiet activities because their cortex is very easily stimulated to a point higher than optimal alertness.
What neurological differences explain why extraverts pursue more stimulation than introverts?
Behavioral and physiological research generally supports Eysenck’s view. When introverts and extraverts are presented with a range of intense stimuli, introverts respond more strongly, including salivating more when a drop of lemon juice is placed on their tongues and reacting more negatively to electric shocks or loud noises (Bartol & Costello, 1976; Stelmack, 1990). This reactivity has an impact on the ability to concentrate: Extraverts tend to perform well at tasks that are done in a noisy, arousing context (such as bartending or teaching), whereas introverts are better at tasks that require concentration in tranquil contexts (such as the work of a librarian or nighttime security guard; Geen, 1984; Lieberman & Rosenthal, 2001; Matthews & Gilliland, 1999).
359
In a refined version of Eysenck’s ideas, Jeffrey Gray (1970) proposed that the dimensions of extraversion–
Recent studies have suggested that the core personality traits may arise from individual differences in the brain. For instance, self-
1. | A relatively stable disposition to behave in a particular and consistent way is a |
c.
2. | Which of the following is not one of the Big Five personality factors? |
d.
3. | Compelling evidence for the importance of biological factors in personality is best seen in studies of |
b.