11.5 The Social-Cognitive Approach: Personalities in Situations

Do researchers in social cognition think that personality arises from past experiences or from the current environment?

What is it like to be a person? The social-cognitive approach is an approach that views personality in terms of how the person thinks about the situations encountered in daily life and behaves in response to them. Bringing together insights from social psychology, cognitive psychology, and learning theory, this approach emphasizes how the person experiences and interprets situations (Bandura, 1986; Mischel & Shoda, 1999; Ross & Nisbett, 1991; Wegner & Gilbert, 2000).

social-cognitive approach

An approach that views personality in terms of how the person thinks about the situations encountered in daily life and behaves in response to them.

Researchers in social cognition believe that both the current situation and learning history are key determinants of behavior. These researchers focus on how people perceive their environments, how personality contributes to the way people construct situations in their own minds, and how people’s goals and expectancies influence their responses to situations.

Consistency of Personality across Situations

Mick Stevens © The New Yorker Collection/Cartoonbank.Com

At the core of the social-cognitive approach is a natural puzzle, the person-situation controversy, which focuses on the question of whether behavior is caused more by personality or by situational factors. This controversy began in earnest when Walter Mischel (1968) argued that measured personality traits often do a poor job of predicting individuals’ behavior. Mischel also noted that knowing how a person will behave in one situation is not particularly helpful in predicting the person’s behavior in another situation. For example, in classic studies, Hugh Hartshorne and M. A. May (1928) assessed children’s honesty by examining their willingness to cheat on a test and found that such dishonesty was not consistent from one situation to another. The assessment of a child’s trait of honesty in a cheating situation was of almost no use in predicting whether the child would act honestly in a different situation, such as when given the opportunity to steal money. Mischel proposed that measured traits do not predict behaviors very well because behaviors are determined more by situational factors than personality theorists were willing to acknowledge.

person–situation controversy

The question of whether behavior is caused more by personality or by situational factors.

366

Is a student who cheats on a test more likely than others to steal candy or lie to his grandmother? Social-cognitive research indicates that behavior in one situation does not necessarily predict behavior in a different situation.
Digital Vision/Getty Images

Is there no personality, then? Do we all just do what situations require? It turns out that information about both personality and situation are necessary to predict behavior accurately (Fleeson, 2004; Mischel, 2004). Some situations are particularly powerful, leading most everyone to behave similarly regardless of personality (Cooper & Withey, 2009). At a funeral, almost everyone looks somber, and during an earthquake, almost everyone shakes. But in more moderate situations, personality can come forward to influence behavior (Funder, 2001). Among the children in Hartshorne and May’s (1928) studies, cheating versus not cheating on a test was actually a fairly good predictor of cheating on a test later—as long as the situation was similar. Personality consistency, then, appears to be a matter of when and where a certain kind of behavior tends to be shown (see the Culture & Community box).

Does personality or the current situation predict a person’s behavior?

Culture & Community: Does your personality change according to which language you’re speaking?

Does your personality change according to which language you’re speaking? The personalities of people from different cultures often can diverge. For instance, in one study of personality tests taken by Americans and Mexicans, Americans reported being more extraverted, more agreeable, and more conscientious than Mexicans (Ramirez-Esparza et al., 2004). The authors suggested that this may be due to differences in how individualistic versus collectivistic people are in each culture. Individualistic cultures (like America’s) emphasize personal achievement, whereas collectivistic cultures (like Mexico’s) focus on the importance of family and community outcomes.

Data from Ramirez-Esparza et al., 2004.

Interestingly, however, when the researchers tested Spanish–English bilinguals in Texas, California, and Mexico in both languages, scores of the bilingual participants were more extraverted, agreeable, and conscientious when they took the test in English than when they took it in Spanish! The authors proposed that this difference is the result of cultural frame switching, which refers to the tendency of bi- or multicultural people to adjust their style of thinking, feeling, and behaving to more closely match the group with which they are currently interacting. Most important, the changes are pretty subtle (more toning it up or down rather than total personality transplant), but the appearance of such changes highlights the importance of considering culture and context when thinking about personality.

367

Personal Constructs

How can we understand differences in the way situations are interpreted? George Kelly (1955) long ago realized that these differences in perspective could be used to understand the perceiver’s personality. He suggested that people view the social world from differing perspectives and that these different views arise through the application of personal constructs, dimensions people use in making sense of their experiences. Consider, for example, different individuals’ personal constructs of a clown: One person may see him as a source of fun, another as a tragic figure, and yet another as so frightening that McDonald’s must be avoided at all costs.

personal constructs

Dimensions people use in making sense of their experiences.

Why doesn’t everyone love clowns?

Kelly proposed that different personal constructs are the key to personality differences and lead people to engage in different behaviors. Taking a long break from work for a leisurely lunch might seem lazy to you. To your friend, the break might seem an ideal opportunity for catching up with friends and wonder why you always choose to eat at your desk. Social-cognitive theory explains different responses to situations with the idea that people experience and interpret the world in different ways.

Are two of these people taller and one shorter? Are two bareheaded while one wears a hood? Or are two the daughters and one the mom? George Kelly held that the personal constructs we use to distinguish among people in our lives are basic elements of our own personalities.
David Bathgate/Corbis

Personal Goals and Expectancies

Some days you feel like a puppet on a string. If you have an external locus of control, you may feel that way most days.
A Sia Images/Superstock

Social-cognitive theories also recognize that a person’s unique perspective on situations is reflected in his or her personal goals, which are often conscious. In fact, people can usually tell you their goals, whether they are to find a date for this weekend, get a good grade in psych, establish a fulfilling career, or just get this darn bag of chips open. These goals often reflect the tasks that are appropriate to the person’s situation and stage of life (Cantor, 1990; Klinger, 1977; Little, 1983; Vallacher & Wegner, 1985). For instance, common goals for adolescents include being popular, achieving greater independence from parents and family, and getting into a good college. Common goals for adults include developing a meaningful career, finding a mate, securing financial stability, and starting a family.

People translate goals into behavior in part through outcome expectancies, a person’s assumptions about the likely consequences of a future behavior. Just as a laboratory rat learns that pressing a bar releases a food pellet, we learn that “if I am friendly toward people, they will be friendly in return” and “if I ask people to pull my finger, they will withdraw from me.” So we learn to perform behaviors that we expect will have the outcome of moving us closer to our goals. Outcome expectancies are learned through direct experience, both bitter and sweet, and through merely observing other people’s actions and their consequences. Outcome expectancies combine with a person’s goals to produce the person’s characteristic style of behavior. We do not all want the same things from life, clearly, and our personalities largely reflect the goals we pursue and the expectancies we have about the best ways to pursue them.

outcome expectancies

A person’s assumptions about the likely consequences of a future behavior.

What is the advantage of an internal locus of control?

People also differ in their expectancy for achieving goals. Some people seem to feel that they are fully in control of what happens to them in life, whereas others feel that the world doles out rewards and punishments to them irrespective of their actions. A person’s locus of control is the tendency to perceive the control of rewards as internal to the self or external in the environment (Rotter, 1966). People who believe they control their own destinies are said to have an internal locus of control, whereas those who believe that outcomes are random, determined by luck, or controlled by other people are described as having an external locus of control. These beliefs translate into individual differences in emotion and behavior. For example, people with an internal locus of control tend to be less anxious, achieve more, and cope better with stress than do people with an external orientation (Lefcourt, 1982). To get a sense of your standing on this trait dimension, choose one of the options for each of the sample items from the locus-of-control scale in TABLE 11.3.

locus of control

A person’s tendency to perceive the control of rewards as internal to the self or external in the environment.

Table : Table 11.3 Rotter’s Locus-of-Control Scale

For each pair of items, choose the option that most closely reflects your personal belief.

1.

a. Many of the unhappy things in people’s lives are partly due to bad luck.

b. People’s misfortunes result from the mistakes they make.

2.

a. I have often found that what is going to happen will happen.

b. Trusting to fate has never turned out as well for me as making a decision to take a definite course of action.

3.

a. Becoming a success is a matter of hard work; luck has little or nothing to do with it.

b. Getting a good job depends mainly on being in the right place at the right time.

4.

a. When I make plans, I am almost certain that I can make them work.

b. It is not always wise to plan too far ahead because many things turn out to be a matter of good or bad fortune anyhow.

Source: Information from Rotter, 1966.

Answer: A more internal locus of control would be reflected in choosing options 1b, 2b, 3a, and 4a.

368

SUMMARY QUIZ [11.5]

Question 11.12

1. Which of the following is NOT an emphasis of the social-cognitive approach?
  1. how personality and situation interact to cause behavior
  2. how personality contributes to the way people construct situations in their own minds
  3. how people’s goals and expectancies influence their responses to situations
  4. how people confront realities rather than embrace comforting illusions

d.

Question 11.13

2. According to social-cognitive theorists, ____________ are the dimensions people use in making sense of their experiences.
  1. personal constructs
  2. outcome expectancies
  3. loci of control
  4. personal goals

a.

Question 11.14

3. Tyler has been getting poor evaluations at work. He attributes this to having a mean boss who always assigns him the hardest tasks. This suggests that Tyler has
  1. external locus of control
  2. internal locus of control
  3. high performance anxiety
  4. poorly developed personal constructs

a.