4.1 Sensation and Perception Are Distinct Activities

Sensation is simple stimulation of a sense organ. It is the basic registration of light, sound, pressure, odor, or taste as parts of your body interact with the physical world. After a sensation registers in your central nervous system, perception takes place in your brain: the organization, identification, and interpretation of a sensation in order to form a mental representation. Sensation and perception are related—but distinct—activities.

sensation

Simple stimulation of a sense organ.

perception

The organization, identification, and interpretation of a sensation in order to form a mental representation.

As an example, your eyes are coursing across these sentences right now. The sensory receptors in your eyeballs are registering different patterns of light reflecting off the page. Your brain is integrating and processing that light information into the meaningful perception of words. Your eyes—the sensory organ—aren’t really seeing words; they’re simply encoding different lines and curves on a page. Your brain—the perceptual organ—is transforming those lines and curves into a coherent mental representation of words and concepts.

What role does the brain play in what we see and hear?

Sensory receptors communicate with the brain through transduction, which occurs when many sensors in the body convert physical signals from the environment into encoded neural signals sent to the central nervous system. In vision, light reflected from surfaces provides the eyes with information about the shape, color, and position of objects. In audition, vibrations (from vocal cords or a guitar string, perhaps) cause changes in air pressure that propagate through space to a listener’s ears. In touch, the pressure of a surface against the skin signals its shape, texture, and temperature. In taste and smell, molecules dispersed in the air or dissolved in saliva reveal the identity of substances that we may or may not want to eat. In each case, physical energy from the world is converted to neural energy inside the central nervous system (see TABLE 4.1).

transduction

What takes place when many sensors in the body convert physical signals from the environment into encoded neural signals sent to the central nervous system.

Table 4.1Transduction

Psychophysics

Knowing that perception takes place in the brain, you might wonder if two people see the same colors in the sunset when looking at the evening sky. It’s intriguing to consider the possibility that our basic perceptions of sights or sounds might differ fundamentally from those of other people. How can we measure such a thing objectively? Measuring the physical energy of a stimulus, such as the wavelength of a light, is easy enough: You can probably buy the necessary instruments online to do that yourself. But how do you quantify a person’s private, subjective perception of that light?

97

Why isn’t it enough for a psychophysicist to measure only the strength of a stimulus?

In the mid-1800s, German scientist Gustav Fechner (1801–1887) developed an approach to measuring sensation and perception called psychophysics: methods that measure the strength of a stimulus and the observer’s sensitivity to that stimulus (Fechner, 1860/1966). In a typical psychophysics experiment, researchers ask people to make a simple judgment—whether or not they saw a flash of light, for example. The psychophysicist then relates the measured stimulus, such as the brightness of the light flash, to each observer’s yes-or-no response.

psychophysics

Methods that measure the strength of a stimulus and the observer’s sensitivity to that stimulus.

You can enjoy a tempting ice cream sundae even if you do not know that its sweet taste depends on a complex process of transduction, in which molecules dissolved in saliva are converted to neural signals processed by the brain.
Fotoflare/Istockphoto

98

Measuring Thresholds

Psychophysicists begin the measurement process with a single sensory signal to determine precisely how much physical energy is required for an observer to become aware of a sensation. The simplest quantitative measurement in psychophysics is the absolute threshold, the minimal intensity needed to just barely detect a stimulus in 50% of the trials. A threshold is a boundary. The doorway that separates the inside from the outside of a house is a threshold, as is the boundary between two psychological states (awareness and unawareness, for example). In finding the absolute threshold for sensation, the two states in question are sensing and not sensing some stimulus. TABLE 4.2 lists the approximate sensory thresholds for each of the five senses.

absolute threshold

The minimal intensity needed to just barely detect a stimulus in 50% of the trials.

Table 4.2Approximate Sensory Thresholds

Figure 4.1: FIGURE 4.1 Absolute Threshold Absolute threshold is graphed here as the point where the increasing intensity of the stimulus enables an observer to detect it on 50% of the trials. As the intensity of a stimulus gradually increases, we detect the stimulation more frequently.

To measure the absolute threshold for detecting a sound, for example, an observer sits in a soundproof room wearing headphones linked to a computer. The experimenter presents a pure tone (the sort of sound made by striking a tuning fork) using the computer to vary the loudness or the length of time each tone lasts and recording how often the observer reports hearing that tone under each condition. The outcome of such an experiment is graphed in FIGURE 4.1. Notice from the shape of the curve that the transition from not hearing to hearing is gradual rather than abrupt.

If we repeat this experiment for many different tones, we can observe and record the thresholds for tones ranging from very low to very high pitch. It turns out that people tend to be most sensitive to the range of tones corresponding to human conversation. If the tone is low enough, such as the lowest note on a pipe organ, most humans cannot hear it at all; we can only feel it. If the tone is high enough, we likewise cannot hear it, but dogs and many other animals can.

The absolute threshold is useful for assessing how sensitive we are to faint stimuli, but the human perceptual system is better at detecting changes in stimulation than the simple onset or offset of stimulation. When parents hear their infant’s cry, it’s useful to be able to differentiate the “I’m hungry” cry from the “I’m cranky” cry from the “something is biting my toes” cry. The just noticeable difference (JND) is the minimal change in a stimulus that can just barely be detected.

just noticeable difference (JND)

The minimal change in a stimulus that can just barely be detected.

What is the importance of proportion to the measurement of just noticeable difference?

The JND is not a fixed quantity; rather, it is roughly proportional to the intensity of the stimulus. This relationship was first noticed in 1834 by German physiologist Ernst Weber (Watson, 1978) and is now called Weber’s law: The just noticeable difference of a stimulus is a constant proportion despite variations in intensity. As an example, if you picked up a 1-ounce envelope, then a 2-ounce envelope, you’d probably notice the difference between them. But if you picked up a 20-pound package and then a 20-pound, 1-ounce package, you’d probably detect no difference at all between them.

Weber’s law

The just noticeable difference of a stimulus is a constant proportion despite variations in intensity.

99

Signal Detection

Measuring absolute and difference thresholds requires a critical assumption: that a threshold exists! But humans don’t suddenly and rapidly switch between perceiving and not perceiving; in fact, the very same physical stimulus, such as a dim light or a quiet tone, presented on several different occasions, may be perceived by the same person on some occasions but not on others (see FIGURE 4.1). Remember, an absolute threshold is operationalized as perceiving the stimulus 50% of the time, which means the other 50% of the time it might go undetected.

Our accurate perception of a sensory stimulus, then, can be somewhat haphazard. Whether in the psychophysics lab or out in the world, sensory signals face a lot of competition, or noise, which refers to all the other stimuli coming from the internal and external environment. Memories, moods, and motives intertwine with what you are seeing, hearing, and smelling at any given time. This internal “noise” competes with your ability to detect a stimulus with perfect, focused attention. Other sights, sounds, and smells in the world at large also compete for attention. As a consequence, you may not perceive everything that you sense, and you may even perceive things that you haven’t sensed.

How accurate and complete are our perceptions of the world?

An approach to psychophysics called signal detection theory holds that the response to a stimulus depends both on a person’s sensitivity to the stimulus in the presence of noise and on a person’s decision criterion. That is, observers consider the sensory evidence evoked by the stimulus and compare it to an internal decision criterion (Green & Swets, 1966; Macmillan & Creelman, 2005). If the sensory evidence exceeds the criterion, the observer responds by saying, “Yes, I detected the stimulus,” and if it falls short of the criterion, the observer responds by saying, “No, I did not detect the stimulus.”

signal detection theory

The response to a stimulus depends both on a person’s sensitivity to the stimulus in the presence of noise and on a person’s response criterion.

Crowds of people such as this one in New York City’s Thanksgiving Day Parade present our visual system with a challenging signal detection task.
Blickwinkel/Alamy

Signal detection theory has practical applications at home, school, work, and even while driving. For example, a radiologist may have to decide whether a mammogram shows that a woman has breast cancer. The radiologist knows that certain features, such as a mass of a particular size and shape, are associated with the presence of cancer. But noncancerous features can have an appearance that is very similar to cancerous ones. The radiologist may decide on a strictly liberal criterion and check every possible case of cancer with a biopsy. This decision strategy minimizes the possibility of missing a true cancer but leads to many unnecessary biopsies. A strictly conservative criterion will cut down on unnecessary biopsies but will miss some treatable cancers. These different types of errors have to be weighed against one another in setting the decision criterion. For an example of a common everyday task that can interfere with signal detection, see the Real World box.

100

The Real World: Multitasking

Multitasking

By one estimate, using a cell phone while driving makes having an accident four times more likely (McEvoy et al., 2005). In response to statistics such as this, state legislatures are passing laws that restrict—and sometimes ban—using mobile phones while driving. You might think that’s a fine idea … for everyone else on the road. But surely you can manage to carry on a conversation while simultaneously driving in a safe and courteous manner. Right? In a word, wrong.

Shifting Attention Participants received fMRI scans as they performed tasks that required them to shift their attention between visual and auditory information. (a) When focusing on auditory information, a region in the superior (upper) temporal lobe involved in auditory processing showed increased activity (yellow/orange). (b) But when participants focused on auditory information, a visual region, the fusiform gyrus, showed decreased activity (blue).
Shomstein & Yantis, 2004

Talking on a cell phone while driving demands that you juggle two independent sources of sensory input—vision and audition—at the same time. This is problematic because research has found that when attention is directed to audition, activity in visual areas decreases (Shomstein & Yantis, 2004). This kind of multitasking creates problems when you need to react suddenly while driving. Researchers have tested experienced drivers in a highly realistic driving simulator, measuring their response times to brake lights and stop signs while they listened to the radio or carried on phone conversations about a political issue, among other tasks (Strayer, Drews, & Johnston, 2003). These experienced drivers reacted significantly more slowly during phone conversations than during the other tasks. This is because a phone conversation requires memory retrieval, deliberation, and planning what to say and often carries an emotional stake in the conversation topic. Tasks such as listening to the radio require far less attention.

Whether the phone was handheld or hands-free made little difference, and similar results have been obtained in field studies of actual driving (Horrey & Wickens, 2006). This suggests that laws requiring drivers to use hands-free phones may have little effect on reducing accidents. The situation is even worse when text messaging is involved: Compared with a no-texting control condition, drivers spent dramatically less time looking at the road when either sending or receiving a text message in the simulator, had a much harder time staying in their lane, missed numerous lane changes, and had greater difficulty maintaining an appropriate distance behind the car ahead of them (Hosking, Young, & Regan, 2009). A recent review concluded that the impairing effect of texting while driving is comparable to that of alcohol consumption and greater than that of smoking marijuana (Pascual-Ferrá, Liu, & Beatty, 2012).

So how well do you multitask in several thousand pounds of metal hurtling down the highway? Unless you have two heads with one brain each—one to talk and one to concentrate on driving—you would do well to keep your eyes on the road and not on the phone.

Sensory Adaptation

When you walk into a bakery, the aroma of freshly baked bread overwhelms you, but after a few minutes, the smell fades. If you dive into cold water, the temperature is shocking at first, but after a few minutes, you get used to it. When you wake up in the middle of the night for a drink of water, the bathroom light blinds you, but after a few minutes, you no longer squint. These are all examples of sensory adaptation: Sensitivity to prolonged stimulation tends to decline over time as an organism adapts to current conditions.

sensory adaptation

Sensitivity to prolonged stimulation tends to decline over time as an organism adapts to current conditions.

What conditions have you already adapted to today? Sounds? Smells?

Sensory adaptation is a useful process for most organisms. Imagine what your sensory and perceptual world would be like without it. (If you had to be constantly aware of how your tongue feels while it is resting in your mouth, you’d be driven to distraction.) Our sensory systems respond more strongly to changes in stimulation than to constant stimulation. A stimulus that doesn’t change usually doesn’t require any action; your car probably emits a certain hum all the time, one that you’ve gotten used to. But a change in stimulation often signals a need for action. If your car starts making different kinds of noises, you’re not only more likely to notice them, but you’re also more likely to do something about it.

visual acuity

The ability to see fine detail.

According to the theory of natural selection, inherited characteristics that provide a survival advantage tend to spread throughout the population across generations. Why might sensory adaptation have evolved? What survival benefits might it confer to a predator trying to hunt prey?
Clément Philippe/Arterra Picture Library/Alamy

101

SUMMARY QUIZ [4.1]

Question 4.1

1. Sensation involves _____________, whereas perception involves _____________.
  1. organization; coordination
  2. stimulation; interpretation
  3. identification; translation
  4. comprehension; information

b.

Question 4.2

2. What process converts physical signals from the environment into neural signals carried by sensory neurons into the central nervous system?
  1. representation
  2. identification
  3. propagation
  4. transduction

d.

Question 4.3

3. The smallest intensity needed to just barely detect a stimulus is called
  1. proportional magnitude.
  2. absolute threshold.
  3. just noticeable difference.
  4. Weber’s law.

b.