8.3 Motivation: The Wanting Machine

Leonardo is a robot, so he does what he is programmed to do and nothing more. Because he doesn’t have wants and urges—doesn’t crave friendship or desire chocolate or hate homework—he doesn’t initiate his own behavior. He can learn but not yearn. Motivation refers to the purpose or goal of an action, and human beings have two basic kinds of motivations: the biological and psychological. Let’s explore each in turn.

motivation

The purpose for or psychological cause of an action.

Biological Motivations

Humans are animals, and all animals must survive and reproduce. It is not surprising, then, that two of our most powerful biological motivations are toward food and sex. We are driven to make lunch and driven to make love, but as you will see, neither of these motivations is as simple as it sounds.

Survival: The Motivation for Food

When a thermostat detects that a room is too cold, it sends a signal that initiates a corrective action, namely, turning up the furnace. Bodies are a bit like thermostats in this way. To survive, the body must maintain precise levels of nutrition, warmth, and so on, and when these levels depart from optimality, the body sends signals to the brain, asking it to take corrective action. That signal is called a drive, which is an internal state that signals a physiological need.

drive

An internal state that signals a physiological need.

The most regular and familiar of your body’s drives is what you call hunger. At every moment, your body is sending signals to your brain about the amount of energy it has available. If your body needs energy, it sends a signal to tell your brain to switch hunger on, and if your body has sufficient energy, it sends a signal to tell your brain to switch hunger off (Gropp et al., 2005). No one knows precisely what these signals are or how they are sent and received, but research has identified a few candidates.

259

For example, ghrelin is a hormone that is produced in the stomach and appears to be one of the signals that tells the brain to switch hunger on (Inui, 2001; Nakazato et al., 2001). When people are injected with ghrelin, they become intensely hungry and eat about 30% more than usual (Wren et al., 2001). Another chemical called leptin is secreted by fat cells, and it appears to be one of the signals that tells the brain to switch hunger off, which makes food less rewarding (Farooqi et al., 2007). People who are born with a leptin deficiency have trouble controlling their appetites (Montague et al., 1997). Some researchers think this story is far too simple. They argue that there is no general drive called hunger, but rather, that there are many different hungers, each of which is a response to a unique nutritional deficit and each of which is switched on by a unique chemical messenger (Rozin & Kalat, 1971). For example, rats that are deprived of proteins will gladly eat proteins but will turn down fats and carbohydrates, suggesting that they are experiencing a specific “protein hunger” and not a general hunger (Rozin, 1968).

What makes us feel hungry?

Whether hunger is signaled by one chemical or many, the primary receiver of these signals is the hypothalamus (see FIGURE 8.10). The lateral hypothalamus receives the “hunger on” signals, and when it is destroyed, animals sitting in a cage full of food will starve themselves to death. The ventromedial hypothalamus receives the “hunger off” signals, and when it is destroyed, animals will gorge themselves to the point of illness and obesity (Miller, 1960; Steinbaum & Miller, 1965). These two structures were once thought to be the “hunger center” and “satiety center” of the brain, but it turns out to be much more complicated than that (Woods et al., 1998). These structures clearly play an important role in hunger, but no one yet knows exactly what the role is (Stellar & Stellar, 1985).

Figure 8.10: FIGURE 8.10 Hunger, Satiety, and the Hypothalamus The hypothalamus comprises many parts. In general, the lateral hypothalamus receives the signals that turn hunger on, and the ventromedial hypothalamus receives the signals that turn hunger off.
Bar Refaeili is one of Israel’s best known super-models. In 2012, Israel enacted a law banning models whose body mass index is under 18.5 from appearing in advertisements. So a 5’8” model must weigh at least 119 pounds.
Wireimage/Getty Images

EATING DISORDERS. Feelings of hunger tell us when to start eating and when to stop. But for the 10 to 30 million Americans who have eating disorders, eating is a much more complicated affair (Hoek & van Hoeken, 2003). For instance, bulimia nervosa is an eating disorder characterized by binge eating followed by purging. People with bulimia typically ingest large quantities of food in a relatively short period and then take laxatives or induce vomiting to purge the food from their bodies. These people are caught in a cycle: They eat to ease negative emotions such as sadness and anxiety, but then concern about weight gain leads them to experience negative emotions such as guilt and self-loathing, and these emotions then lead them to purge (Sherry & Hall, 2009; cf. Haedt-Matt & Keel, 2011).

bulima nervosa

An eating disorder characterized by binge eating followed by purging.

Anorexia nervosa is an eating disorder characterized by an intense fear of being fat and severe restriction of food intake. People with anorexia tend to have a distorted body image that leads them to believe they are fat when they are actually emaciated, and they tend to be high-achieving perfectionists who see their severe control of eating as a triumph of will over impulse. Remember ghrelin—the chemical that turns hunger on? Well, contrary to what you might expect, people with anorexia have extremely high levels of ghrelin in their blood, which suggests that their bodies are trying desperately to switch hunger on but that hunger’s call is somehow being suppressed, ignored, or overridden (Ariyasu et al., 2001).

anorexia nervosa

An eating disorder characterized by an intense fear of being fat and severe restriction of food intake.

Anorexia may have both cultural and biological causes (Klump & Culbert, 2007). For example, women with anorexia typically believe that thinness equals beauty (which isn’t a very surprising conclusion for women to draw when the average American fashion model is seven inches taller and 23 pounds lighter than the average American woman). But anorexia is not just “vanity run amok” (Striegel-Moore & Bulik, 2007, p. 193). Many researchers believe that there are as-yet-undiscovered biological and/or genetic components to the illness as well. For example, although anorexia primarily affects women, men have a sharply increased risk of becoming anorexic if they have a female twin who has the disorder (Procopio & Marriott, 2007), suggesting that anorexia may have something to do with prenatal exposure to female hormones.

260

What causes anorexia?

OBESITY. America’s most pervasive eating-related problem is obesity, which is defined as having a body mass index (BMI) of 30 or greater. TABLE 8.1 allows you to compute your BMI, and the odds are that you won’t like the number. Since 1999, Americans have collectively gained more than a billion pounds (Kolbert, 2009). In 2012, only one state (Colorado) had an obesity rate lower than 20% (see FIGURE 8.11).

Table : Table 8.1 Body Mass Index Table

 

Normal

Overweight      

Obese

Extreme Obesity      

BMI

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

42

43

44

45

46

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

Height (Inches)

Body Weight (pounds)

58

  91

  96

100

105

110

115

119

124

129

134

138

143

148

153

158

162

167

172

177

181

186

191

196

201

205

210

215

220

224

229

234

239

244

248

253

258

59

  94

  99

104

109

114

119

124

128

133

138

143

148

153

158

163

169

173

178

183

188

193

198

203

308

212

217

222

227

232

237

242

247

252

257

262

267

60

  97

102

107

112

116

123

128

133

138

143

148

153

156

163

168

174

179

184

189

194

199

204

209

215

220

225

230

235

240

245

250

256

261

266

271

278

61

100

108

111

116

122

127

132

137

143

148

153

156

164

169

174

180

186

190

195

201

206

211

217

222

227

232

238

243

248

254

259

264

269

275

280

285

62

104

109

115

120

126

131

138

142

147

153

158

164

169

175

180

186

191

196

202

207

213

218

224

229

235

240

248

251

258

262

267

273

278

264

289

295

63

107

113

118

124

130

135

141

148

152

158

163

169

175

180

188

191

197

203

208

214

220

225

231

237

242

248

254

260

265

270

278

282

287

293

299

304

64

110

118

122

128

134

140

145

151

157

163

169

174

180

188

192

197

204

209

215

221

227

232

238

244

250

258

262

267

273

279

285

291

298

302

308

314

65

114

120

128

132

138

144

150

156

162

168

174

180

186

192

193

204

210

218

222

228

234

240

246

252

258

264

270

278

282

288

294

300

308

312

318

324

66

118

124

130

138

142

148

155

161

167

173

179

186

192

198

204

210

216

223

229

235

241

247

253

260

266

272

278

284

291

297

303

309

315

322

328

334

67

121

127

134

140

146

153

159

166

172

178

185

191

198

204

211

217

223

230

238

242

249

256

261

268

274

280

287

293

299

308

312

319

325

331

338

344

68

125

131

138

144

151

158

164

171

177

184

190

197

203

210

216

223

230

236

243

249

256

262

269

278

282

289

295

302

303

315

322

328

335

341

348

354

69

128

135

142

149

155

162

169

178

182

189

195

203

209

218

223

230

236

243

250

257

263

270

277

284

291

297

304

311

318

324

331

338

345

351

358

365

70

132

139

146

153

160

167

174

181

188

195

202

209

216

222

229

236

243

250

257

264

271

278

285

292

299

308

313

320

327

334

341

348

355

362

369

378

71

138

143

150

157

166

172

179

186

193

200

208

215

222

229

235

243

250

257

265

272

279

288

293

301

308

315

322

329

338

343

351

358

365

372

379

388

72

140

147

154

162

169

177

184

191

199

208

213

221

228

235

242

250

258

265

272

279

287

294

302

309

316

324

331

338

346

353

361

368

375

383

390

397

73

144

151

159

166

174

182

189

197

204

212

219

227

236

242

250

257

266

272

280

288

295

302

310

318

326

333

340

348

355

363

371

378

388

393

401

408

74

148

155

163

171

179

188

194

202

210

218

225

233

241

249

258

264

272

280

287

295

303

311

319

328

334

342

350

358

365

373

381

389

398

404

412

420

75

152

160

166

178

184

192

200

208

216

224

232

240

248

256

264

272

279

287

295

303

311

319

327

335

343

351

359

367

375

383

391

399

407

415

423

431

76

158

164

172

180

189

197

205

213

221

230

238

246

254

263

271

279

287

295

304

312

320

328

338

344

353

361

369

377

385

394

402

410

418

428

436

443

Data from National Institutes of Health, 1998.

Times have changed. People today are often astonished to see that ads once promised to help young women gain weight to become popular.
Jordan L. Smith Collection
Figure 8.11: FIGURE 8.11 The Geography of Obesity This 2013 map of U.S. obesity rates shows that obesity is a problem everywhere, but especially in the Southeast.
© Gallup, Inc. All rights reserved. The content is used with permission. Gallup retains all rights of republication.

Why should you care? Every year, obesity-related illnesses cost our nation about $147 billion (Finkelstein et al., 2009) and about 3 million lives (Allison et al., 1999). In addition to these financial and physical costs, obese people tend to be viewed negatively by others, have lower self-esteem, and have a lower quality of life (Hebl & Heatherton, 1997; Kolotkin, Meter, & Williams, 2001). The stigma of obesity is so powerful that average-weight people are viewed negatively if they even have a relationship with someone who is obese (Hebl & Mannix, 2003).

261

What causes obesity? First, obesity is highly heritable (Allison et al., 1996) and may well have a genetic component. For example, obese people are often leptin-resistant, which is to say that their brains do not respond to the chemical message that shuts hunger off (Friedman & Halaas, 1998; Heymsfield et al., 1999). Second, obesity may have environmental causes. For example, some studies suggest that toxins in the environment can disrupt the functioning of the endocrine system and predispose people to obesity (Grün & Blumberg, 2006; Newbold et al., 2005), and other studies suggest that obesity can be caused by a dearth of “good bacteria” in the gut (Liou et al., 2013).

DATA VISUALIZATION

Historical BMI/Food Consumption Trends

www.macmillanhighered.com/launchpad/schacterbrief3e

But in most cases, obesity is simply the result of eating too much. We don’t breathe ourselves sick or sleep ourselves sick, so why do we eat ourselves sick? Blame the design. Hundreds of thousands of years ago, the main food-related problem facing our ancestors was starvation, and so our brains and bodies developed two strategies to avoid it. First, our brains developed a strong attraction to foods that provide large amounts of energy per bite (in other words, high calorie foods), which is why most of us prefer hamburgers and milkshakes to celery and water. Second, our bodies developed an ability to store excess food energy in the form of fat, which enabled us to eat more than we needed when food was plentiful and then live off our reserves when food was scarce. Our brains and bodies are beautifully engineered for a world in which high calorie foods are scarce, and the problem is that we don’t live in that world anymore. Instead, we live in a world in which the fatty miracles of modern technology—from chocolate cupcakes to sausage pizzas—are inexpensive and readily available.

The Real World: Jeet Jet?

Jeet Jet?

Does the amount of food placed in front of people influence how much they eat? Brian Wansink and colleagues (2005) sat research participants in front of a large bowl of tomato soup and told them to eat as much as they wanted. In one condition of the study, a server came to the table and refilled the participant’s bowl whenever it got down to about a quarter full. In another condition, unbeknownst to the participants, the bottom of the bowl was connected by a long tube to a large vat of soup, so whenever the participant ate from the bowl, it would slowly and almost imperceptibly refill itself.

Researcher Brian Wansink and his bottomless bowl of soup.
Bob Fila/Chicago Tribune/Newscom

What the researchers found was sobering. Participants who unknowingly ate from a “bottomless bowl” consumed a whopping 73% more soup than those who ate from normal bowls—and yet, they didn’t think they had consumed more and they didn’t report feeling any more full.

It seems that we find it easier to keep track of what we are eating than how much, and this can cause us to overeat even when we are trying our best to do just the opposite. For instance, one study showed that diners at an Italian restaurant often chose to eat butter on their bread rather than dipping it in olive oil because they thought that doing so would reduce the number of calories per slice. And they were right. What they didn’t realize, however, is that they would unconsciously compensate for this reduction in calories by eating 23% more bread during the meal (Wansink & Linder, 2003).

This and other research suggests that one of the best ways to reduce our waists is simply to count our bites.

262

We are designed to gain weight easily—and to lose it with great difficulty. The human body resists weight loss in two ways. First, when we gain weight, we experience an increase in both the size and the number of fat cells in our bodies (usually in our abdomens if we are male and in our thighs and buttocks if we are female). But when we lose weight, we experience a decrease in the size of our fat cells but no decrease in their number. Once our bodies have added a fat cell, that cell is pretty much there to stay. It may become thinner when we diet, but it is unlikely to die. Second, our bodies respond to dieting by decreasing our metabolism, which is the rate at which energy is used by the body. When our bodies sense that we are living through a famine (which is what they conclude when we refuse to feed them), they find more efficient ways to turn food into fat, which was a great trick for our ancestors but a real nuisance for us. The bottom line is that avoiding obesity is easier than overcoming it (Casazza et al., 2013).

metabolism

The rate at which energy is used by the body.

Why is dieting so difficult and ineffective?

And avoiding it isn’t as difficult as you might think. People eat when they are hungry, of course, but they also eat when cues in the environment tell them to—for instance, when they see other people eating or when the clock says it is time for lunch (Herman, Roth, & Polivy, 2003; Rozin et al., 1998; see the Real World box). If environmental cues can make us eat, can they also make us stop? Research suggests the answer is yes. In one study, snacking students ate fewer Pringles when every seventh chip was colored red, presumably because the color coding allowed them to keep track of how much they were eating (Geier, Wansink, & Rozin, 2012). In another study, people ate 22% less pasta with tomato sauce when they used a white plate instead of a red plate, presumably because the white plate provided a stark contrast that allowed them to see what they were eating (van Ittersum & Wansink, 2012). These and dozens of other studies show that small changes in our environments can prevent big changes in our waistlines.

One reason why obesity rates are rising is that “normal portions” keep getting larger. When researchers analyzed 52 depictions of The Last Supper that were painted between the years 1000 and 1800, they found that the average plate size increased by 66% (Data from Wansink & Wansink, 2010).
Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Wansink, B., & Wansink, C.S., The largest last supper: Depictions of food portions and plate size increased over the millennium.; International Journal of Obesity (34), 2010, 943–944 (permission obtained).
Scala/Art Resource, NY

263

Other Voices: Fat and Happy

Fat and Happy

Nobody wants to be fat. At least that’s what you might think. But as the novelist Alice Randall noted, in some cultures, being heavy isn’t just acceptable—it is desirable.

Alice Randall is a novelist whose books include The Wind Done Gone, Pushkin and the Queen of Spades, Rebel Yell, and Ada’s Rules.
© Sarah Krulwich/The New York Times/Redux Pictures

Four out of five black women are seriously overweight. One out of four middle-aged black women has diabetes. With $174 billion a year spent on diabetes-related illness in America and obesity quickly overtaking smoking as a cause of cancer deaths, it is past time to try something new.

What we need is a body-culture revolution in black America. Why? Because too many experts who are involved in the discussion of obesity don’t understand something crucial about black women and fat: many black women are fat because we want to be.

The black poet Lucille Clifton’s 1987 poem “Homage to My Hips” begins with the boast, “These hips are big hips.” She establishes big black hips as something a woman would want to have and a man would desire. She wasn’t the first or the only one to reflect this community knowledge. Twenty years before, in 1967, Joe Tex, a black Texan, dominated the radio airwaves across black America with a song he wrote and recorded, “Skinny Legs and All.” One of his lines haunts me to this day: “some man, somewhere who’ll take you baby, skinny legs and all.” For me, it still seems almost an impossibility.

Chemically, in its ability to promote disease, black fat may be the same as white fat. Culturally it is not.

How many white girls in the ’60s grew up praying for fat thighs? I know I did. I asked God to give me big thighs like my dancing teacher, Diane. There was no way I wanted to look like Twiggy, the white model whose boy-like build was the dream of white girls. Not with Joe Tex ringing in my ears.

How many middle-aged white women fear their husbands will find them less attractive if their weight drops to less than 200 pounds? I have yet to meet one.

But I know many black women whose sane, handsome, successful husbands worry when their women start losing weight. My lawyer husband is one.

Another friend, a woman of color who is a tenured professor, told me that her husband, also a tenured professor and of color, begged her not to lose “the sugar down below” when she embarked on a weight-loss program….

I live in Nashville. There is an ongoing rivalry between Nashville and Memphis. In black Nashville, we like to think of ourselves as the squeaky-clean brown town best known for our colleges and churches. In contrast, black Memphis is known for its music and bars and churches. We often tease the city up the road by saying that in Nashville we have a church on every corner and in Memphis they have a church and a liquor store on every corner. Only now the saying goes, there’s a church, a liquor store and a dialysis center on every corner in black Memphis.

The billions that we are spending to treat diabetes is money that we don’t have for education reform or retirement benefits, and what’s worse, it’s estimated that the total cost of America’s obesity epidemic could reach almost $1 trillion by 2030 if we keep on doing what we have been doing.

We have to change. …

Randall suggests that if we really want to solve the obesity problem, we must first understand that some people don’t see it as a problem at all. So what should society do? Should we try to change the belief that “fat is beautiful” through education and advertising? Would that alleviate the problem Randall identifies? Or would it just serve to stigmatize overweight people, who already suffer from prejudice and discrimination? How can you discourage obesity without hurting people who are obese?

Abridged version of “Black Women and Fat” from the New York Times, May 5, 2012. © 2012 The New York Times. All rights reserved. Used by permission and protected by the Copyright Laws of the United States. The printing, copying, redistribution, or retransmission of this Content without express written permission is prohibited. http://www.nytimes.com/2012/05/06/opinion/sunday/why-black-women-are-fat.html

Reproduction: The Motivation for Sex

Food motivates us because it is essential to our survival. But sex is also essential to our survival—or at least to the survival of our DNA—which is why evolution has wired a sex drive into almost every human brain.

Bernard Schoenbaum/The New Yorker Collection/cartoonbank.com

The chemistry behind that drive is fairly well understood. The hormone dihydroepiandosterone (DHEA) seems to be involved in the initial onset of the sex drive. Both boys and girls begin producing this slow-acting hormone at about the age of 6, which may explain why boys and girls both experience their initial sexual interest at about the age of 10. Two other hormones have more gender-specific effects. Both males and females produce testosterone and estrogen, but males produce more of the former and females produce more of the latter. As you will learn in the Development chapter, these two hormones are largely responsible for the physical and psychological changes that characterize puberty.

264

The red coloration on the female gelada’s chest (left) indicates that she is in estrus and amenable to sex. The sexual interest of a female human being (right) is not limited to a particular time in her monthly cycle.
Michael K. Nichols/National Geographic/Getty Images
Brand X Pictures/Jupiter Images

But are they also responsible for the waxing and waning of the sex drive in adults? The answer appears to be yes—as long as those adults are rats. Testosterone increases the sex drive of male rats, and estrogen increases the sex drive of female rats. But the story for human beings is far more interesting. The females of most mammalian species (e.g., dogs, cats, and rats) are only interested in sex when their estrogen levels are high, which happens when they are ovulating (i.e., when they are “in estrus” or “in heat”). But female humans can be interested in sex at any point in their monthly cycles. Although the level of estrogen in a woman’s body changes dramatically over the course of her monthly menstrual cycle, studies suggest that sexual desire changes little, if at all.

Why do people have sex?

If estrogen is not the chemical basis of women’s sex drives, then what is? Two pieces of evidence suggest that the answer is testosterone—the same hormone that provides the chemical basis of men’s sex drives. First, when women are given testosterone, their sex drives increase. Second, men naturally have more testosterone than women do, and they generally have stronger sex drives. Men are more likely than women to think about sex, have sexual fantasies, seek sex and sexual variety (whether positions or partners), masturbate, want sex at an early point in a relationship, and complain about low sex drive in their partners (Baumeister, Cantanese, & Vohs, 2001). All of this suggests that testosterone is the chemical basis of the sex drive in both men and women.

Although men and women have on average different levels of sex drive, they have sex for similar reasons. Sex is a prerequisite for reproduction, of course, but the vast majority of sexual acts are not meant to produce babies. College students, for example, are rarely aiming to get pregnant, but they often have sex, and when they do it is because of physical attraction (“The person had beautiful eyes”), as a means to an end (“I wanted to be popular”), to increase emotional connection (“I wanted to communicate at a deeper level”), and to alleviate insecurity (“It was the only way my partner would spend time with me”; Meston & Buss, 2007). Although men are more likely than women to report having sex for purely physical reasons, TABLE 8.2 shows that men and women don’t differ dramatically in this regard. We will have much more to say about sexual attraction and relationships in the Social Psychology chapter.

Table : Table 8.2 Reasons for Having Sex

Top Ten Reasons Why Men and Women Report Having sex

 

Women

Men

1

I was attracted to the person.

I was attracted to the person.

2

I wanted to experience the physical pleasure.

It feels good.

3

It feels good.

I wanted to experience the physical pleasure.

4

I wanted to show my affection to the person.

It’s fun.

5

I wanted to express my love for the person.

I wanted to show my affection to the person.

6

I was sexually aroused and wanted the release.

I was sexually aroused and wanted the release.

7

I was “horny.”

I was “horny.”

8

It’s fun.

I wanted to express my love for the person.

9

I realized I was in love.

I wanted to achieve an orgasm.

10

I was “in the heat of the moment.”

I wanted to please my partner.

Source: Information from Meston & Buss, 2007.

Psychological Motivations

Survival and reproduction are every animal’s first order of business, so it is no surprise that we are strongly motivated by food and sex. But we are motivated by other things too. Yes, we crave kisses of both the chocolate and romantic variety, but we also crave friendship and respect, security and certainty, wisdom and meaning, and a whole lot more. The psychologist Abraham Maslow (1954) argued that all human motivations (which he called needs) could be arranged in a hierarchy (see FIGURE 8.12) with biological motivations at the bottom and psychological motivations at the top. He suggested that until motivations at one level were satisfied, motivations at higher levels were ignored. In other words, when people are hungry or exhausted, they don’t worry too much about intellectual fulfillment and moral clarity (see FIGURE 8.13).

Figure 8.12: FIGURE 8.12 Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs Human beings are motivated to satisfy a variety of needs. Psychologist Abraham Maslow thought these needs formed a hierarchy, with physiological needs forming a base and self-actualization needs forming a pinnacle. He suggested that people don’t experience higher needs until the needs below them have been met.
Figure 8.13: FIGURE 8.13 When Do Higher Needs Matter? Maslow was right. A recent study of 77,000 people in the world’s 51 poorest nations (Martin & Hill, 2012) showed that if people have their basic needs met, then autonomy (i.e., freedom to make their own decisions) increases their satisfaction with their lives. But when people do not have their basic needs met, autonomy makes little difference. (Data from Martin & Hill, 2012.)

265

Maslow was correct in observing that psychological motivations are usually more deferrable than biological motivations. But these two kinds of motivations differ in other ways as well. For example, although we share our biological motivations with most other animals, our psychological motivations are uniquely human. Chimps and rabbits and robins and turtles are all motivated to have sex, but only human beings are motivated to imbue the act with meaning. In addition, although our biological motivations are few—food, sex, oxygen, sleep, and a small handful of other things—our psychological motivations are so numerous and varied that no psychologist has ever been able to make a complete list (Hofmann, Vohs, & Baumeister, 2012). Nonetheless, even if you looked at an incomplete list, you’d quickly notice that psychological motivations vary on three key dimensions: extrinsic versus intrinsic, conscious versus unconscious, and approach versus avoidance. Let’s examine each of these.

Intrinsic vs. Extrinsic

Taking a psychology exam and eating a French fry are different in many ways. One makes you tired and the other makes you chubby, one requires that you move your lips and the other requires that you don’t, and so on. But the key difference between these activities is that one is a means to an end and one is an end in itself. An intrinsic motivation is a motivation to take actions that are themselves rewarding. When we eat a French fry because it tastes good, dance because it feels good, or listen to music because it sounds good, we are intrinsically motivated. These activities don’t have a payoff: They are a payoff. Conversely, an extrinsic motivation is a motivation to take actions that lead to reward. When we floss our teeth so we can avoid gum disease (and get dates), when we work hard for money so we can pay our rent (and get dates), and when we take an exam so we can get a college degree (and get money to get dates), we are extrinsically motivated. None of these things directly brings pleasure, but all may lead to pleasure in the long run.

intrinsic motivation

A motivation to take actions that are themselves rewarding.

extrinsic motivation

A motivation to take actions that lead to reward.

In 2010, Mohamed Bouazizi set himself on fire to protest his treatment by the Tunisian government, and his dramatic suicide ignited the revolution that came to be known as “Arab Spring.” Clearly, psychological needs—such as the need for justice—can be powerful.
Fethi Belaid/AFP/Getty Images

266

Extrinsic motivation gets a bad rap. Americans tend to believe that people should “follow their hearts” and “do what they love,” and we feel sorry for students who choose courses just to please their parents and for parents who choose jobs just to make a living. But the fact is that our ability to engage in behaviors that are unrewarding in the present because we believe they will bring greater rewards in the future is one of our species’ most significant talents, and no other species can do it quite as well as we can (Gilbert, 2006). In research on the ability to delay gratification (Ayduk et al., 2007; Mischel et al., 2004), people are typically faced with a choice between getting something they want right now (e.g., a scoop of ice cream) or waiting and getting more of what they want later (e.g., two scoops of ice cream). Waiting for ice cream is a lot like taking an exam or flossing: It isn’t much fun, but you do it because you know you will reap greater rewards in the end. Studies show that 4-year-old children who can delay gratification are judged to be more intelligent and socially competent 10 years later and have higher SAT scores when they enter college (Mischel, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989). In fact, the ability to delay gratification is a better predictor of a child’s grades in school than is the child’s IQ (Duckworth & Seligman, 2005). Apparently there is something to be said for extrinsic motivation.

Why should people delay gratification?

There is a lot to be said for intrinsic motivation too (Patall, Cooper, & Robinson, 2008). People work harder when they are intrinsically motivated, they enjoy what they do more, and they do it more creatively. Both kinds of motivation have advantages, which is why many of us try to build lives in which we are both intrinsically and extrinsically motivated by the same activity—lives in which we are paid for doing exactly what we like to do best. Who hasn’t fantasized about becoming an artist or an athlete or a wealthy celebrity’s personal party planner? Alas, research suggests that it is difficult to get paid for doing what you love and still end up loving what you do because extrinsic rewards can undermine intrinsic interest (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Henderlong & Lepper, 2002). For example, in one study, college students who were intrinsically interested in a puzzle either were paid to complete it or completed it for free, and those who were paid were less likely to play with the puzzle later on (Deci, 1971). It appears that under some circumstances, people take rewards to indicate that an activity isn’t inherently pleasurable (“If they had to pay me to do that puzzle, it couldn’t have been very much fun”); thus rewards can cause people to lose their intrinsic motivation.

Why do rewards sometimes backfire?

Just as the promise of rewards can undermine intrinsic motivation, the threat of punishment can create it. In one study, children who had no intrinsic interest in playing with a toy suddenly gained an interest when the experimenter threatened to punish them if they touched it (Aronson, 1963). And when a group of day care centers got fed up with parents who arrived late to pick up their children, some of them instituted a financial penalty for tardiness. As FIGURE 8.14 shows, the financial penalty caused an increase in late arrivals (Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000). Why? Because parents are intrinsically motivated to fetch their kids and they generally do their best to be on time. But when the day care centers imposed a fine for late arrival, the parents became extrinsically motivated to fetch their children—and because the fine wasn’t particularly large, they decided to pay a small financial penalty in order to leave their children in day care for an extra hour. When punishments and rewards change intrinsic motivation into extrinsic motivation, unexpected consequences can follow.

Figure 8.14: FIGURE 8.14 When Threats Backfire Threats can cause behaviors that were once intrinsically motivated to become extrinsically motivated. Day care centers that instituted fines for late-arriving parents saw an increase in the number of parents who arrived late. (Data from Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000.)

267

In most elections, only about a third of the eligible voters in Arizona bother to cast a ballot. That’s what made Mark Osterloh propose the Arizona Voter Reward Act, which would award $1 million to a randomly selected voter in every election. Given what you know about intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, what consequences might such an act have?
Jeff Topping/The New York Times/Redux Pictures

Conscious versus Unconscious

When prizewinning artists or scientists are asked to explain their achievements, they typically say things like, “I wanted to liberate color from form” or “I wanted to cure diabetes.” They almost never say, “I wanted to exceed my father’s accomplishments, thereby proving to my mother that I was worthy of her love.” People clearly have conscious motivations, which are motivations of which people are aware, but they also have unconscious motivations, which are motivations of which people are not aware (Aarts, Custers, & Marien, 2008; Bargh et al., 2001; Hassin, Bargh, & Zimerman, 2009).

conscious motivations

Motivations of which people are aware.

unconscious motivations

Motivations of which people are not aware.

For example, research suggests that people vary in their need for achievement, which is the motivation to solve worthwhile problems (McClelland et al., 1953). Some researchers have argued that this basic motivation is unconscious. For example, when words such as achievement are presented on a computer screen so rapidly that people cannot consciously perceive them, those people will work especially hard to solve a puzzle (Bargh et al., 2001) and will feel especially unhappy if they fail (Chartrand & Kay, 2006).

need for achievement

The motivation to solve worthwhile problems.

What makes people aware of their motivations?

What determines whether we are conscious of our motivations? Most actions have more than one motivation, and the ease or difficulty of performing the action may determine which of these motivations we will be aware of (Vallacher & Wegner, 1985, 1987). When actions are easy (e.g., screwing in a lightbulb), we are aware of our most general motivations (e.g., to be helpful), but when actions are difficult (e.g., wrestling with a lightbulb that is stuck in its socket), we are aware of our more specific motivations (e.g., to get the threads aligned). For example, participants in an experiment drank coffee either from a normal mug or from a mug that had a heavy weight attached to the bottom, which made the mug difficult to manipulate. When asked what they were doing, those who were drinking from the normal mug explained that they were “satisfying needs,” whereas those who were drinking from the weighted mug explained that they were “swallowing” (Wegner et al., 1984).

Approach versus Avoidance

The poet James Thurber (1956) wrote, “All men should strive to learn before they die/What they are running from, and to, and why.” The motivation to “run to” pleasure is called approach motivation, which is a motivation to experience a positive outcome, and the motivation to “run from” pain is called avoidance motivation, which is a motivation not to experience a negative outcome. Pleasure is not just the lack of pain, and pain is not just the lack of pleasure. They are independent experiences that occur in different parts of the brain (Davidson et al., 1990; Gray, 1990).

approach motivation

A motivation to experience a positive outcome.

avoidance motivation

A motivation not to experience a negative outcome.

Michael Phelps is clearly high—in need for achievement, that is—which is one of the reasons why he ultimately became the most decorated Olympic athlete of all time.
AP Photo/The Canadian Press, Ryan Remiorz

268

Research suggests that, all else being equal, avoidance motivations tend to be more powerful than approach motivations. Most people will turn down a chance to bet on a coin flip that would pay them $10 if it came up heads but would require them to pay $8 if it came up tails, because they believe that the pain of losing $8 will be more intense than the pleasure of winning $10 (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). On average, avoidance motivation is stronger than approach motivation, but the relative strength of these two tendencies does differ somewhat from person to person. TABLE 8.3 shows the kind of question that has been used to measure the relative strength of a person’s approach and avoidance tendencies (Carver & White, 1994). Research shows that people who are described by the high-approach items are happier when rewarded than those who are not, and that those who are described by the high-avoidance items are more anxious when threatened than those who are not (Carver, 2006). Just as some people seem to be more responsive to rewards than to punishments (and vice versa), some people tend to think about their behaviors as attempts to get a reward rather than to avoid punishment (and vice versa; Higgins, 1997).

Table : Table 8.3 Scale for Measuring the Behavioral Inhibition System and Behavioral Activation System

To what extent do each of these items describe you? The items in red measure the strength of your avoidance tendency, and the items in green measure the strength of your approach tendency.

  • Even if something bad is about to happen to me, I rarely experience fear or nervousness. (LOW AVOIDANCE)
  • I go out of my way to get things I want. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • When I’m doing well at something, I love to keep at it. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • I’m always willing to try something new if I think it will be fun. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • When I get something I want, I feel excited and energized. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • Criticism or scolding hurts me quite a bit. (HIGH AVOIDANCE)
  • When I want something, I usually go all-out to get it. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • I will often do things for no other reason than that they might be fun. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • If I see a chance to get something I want, I move on it right away. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • I feel pretty worried or upset when I think or know somebody is angry at me. (HIGH AVOIDANCE)
  • When I see an opportunity for something I like, I get excited right away. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • I often act on the spur of the moment. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • If I think something unpleasant is going to happen, I usually get pretty “worked up.” (HIGH AVOIDANCE)
  • When good things happen to me, it affects me strongly. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • I feel worried when I think I have done poorly at something important. (HIGH AVOIDANCE)
  • I crave excitement and new sensations. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • When I go after something, I use a “no holds barred” approach. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • I have very few fears compared to my friends. (LOW AVOIDANCE)
  • It would excite me to win a contest. (HIGH APPROACH)
  • I worry about making mistakes. (HIGH AVOIDANCE)

Source: Information from Carver & White, 1994.

269

A “credit card surcharge” and a “cash discount” are precisely the same thing. But they sure don’t feel that way! RyanAir customers were outraged in 2012 when the airline imposed a 2% surcharge on customers who paid with credit cards. Would the airline have been wiser to raise its ticket prices by 2% and then offer a 2% discount to customers who paid with cash?
imagebroker.net/Superstock

Which is stronger—avoidance or approach?

Avoidance motivation is powerful, and of all the things that people want to avoid, death is pretty high on everyone’s list. All animals strive to avoid death, but only human beings realize that this striving is ultimately in vain. Some psychologists have suggested that our knowledge of death’s inevitability creates a sense of “existential terror,” and that much of our behavior is merely an attempt to manage it. Terror Management Theory is a theory about how people respond to knowledge of their own mortality, and it suggests that one of the ways that people cope with their existential terror is by developing a “cultural world-view”—a shared set of beliefs about what is good and right and true (Greenberg, Solomon, & Arndt, 2008; Solomon et al., 2004). These beliefs allow people to see themselves as more than mortal animals because they inhabit a world of meaning in which they can achieve symbolic immortality (e.g., by leaving a great legacy or having children) and perhaps even literal immortality (e.g., by being pious and earning a spot in the afterlife). According to this theory, our cultural worldview is a shield that buffers us against the anxiety that the knowledge of our own mortality creates.

terror management theory

A theory about how people respond to knowledge of their own mortality.

SUMMARY QUIZ [8.3]

Question 8.8

1. The hedonic principle states that
  1. emotions provide people with information.
  2. people are motivated to experience pleasure and avoid pain.
  3. people use their moods as information about the likelihood of succeeding at a task.
  4. motivations are acquired solely through experience.

b.

Question 8.9

2. According to the early psychologists, an unlearned tendency to seek a particular goal is called
  1. an instinct.
  2. a drive.
  3. a motivation.
  4. a corrective action.

a.

Question 8.10

3. According to Maslow, our most basic needs are
  1. self-actualization and self-esteem.
  2. biological.
  3. unimportant until other needs are met.
  4. belongingness and love.

b.

Question 8.11

4. Which of the following is NOT a dimension on which psychological motivations vary?
  1. intrinsic–extrinsic
  2. conscious–unconscious
  3. avoid–approach
  4. appraisal–reappraisal

d.

270

Question 8.12

5. Which of the following activities is most likely the result of extrinsic motivation?
  1. completing a crossword puzzle
  2. pursuing a career as a musician
  3. having ice cream for dessert
  4. flossing one’s teeth

d.