15.1 Theories of Late Adulthood

Still Helping Virginia Ryder, seen here helping Betty Baldwin, is a lifelong helper. She cared for younger children when she was an adolescent, and she has been a senior companion for the past 19 years, often for younger people. She is 89 years old.
AP PHOTO/COLUMBUS DAILY DISPATCH, ERIC ALBRECHT

Development in late adulthood may be more diverse than at any other age: Some elderly people run marathons and lead nations, whereas others can no longer walk or talk. Moreover, individuals vary within themselves in almost every measure from day to day, with some days much better than others (Krenk et al., 2012). Many social scientists try to understand the significance and origin of these variations and to describe the general course of old age.

Some theories of late adulthood have been called self theories because they focus on individuals’ perceptions of themselves and their ability to meet challenges to their identity. Other theories are called stratification theories because they describe the ways in which societies place people on a particular life path.

Self Theories

It can be said that people become more truly themselves as they grow old, an idea captured by Anna Quindlen:

It’s odd when I think of the arc of my life from child to young woman to aging adult. First I was who I was, then I didn’t know who I was, then I invented someone and became her, then I began to like what I’d invented, and finally I was what I was again. It turned out I wasn’t alone in that particular progression.

[Quindlen, 2012, ix]

531

The essential self is protected and rediscovered, despite all the changes that may occur. Self theories emphasize how people negotiate their personal challenges (Sneed & Whitbourne, 2005). Such negotiation is particularly crucial when older adults are confronted with multiple challenges like illness, retirement, and the death of loved ones.

A central idea of self theories is that each person ultimately depends on himself or herself. As one woman explained:

I actually think I value my sense of self more importantly than my family or relationships or health or wealth or wisdom. I do see myself as on my own, ultimately.…Statistics certainly show that older women are likely to end up being alone, so I really do value my own self when it comes right down to things in the end.

[quoted in J Kroger, 2007]

Studies of personality over the life span confirm the continuity of self. An individual’s high or low level on each of the Big Five personality traits (see Chapter 13) tends to remain the same, not only throughout the earlier adult years, but even when people are in their 80s (Mõttus et al., 2012).

IntegrityThe most comprehensive self theory was from Erik Erikson. His eighth and final stage of development is called integrity versus despair, a period in which older adults seek to integrate their unique experiences with their vision of community (Erikson et al., 1986). The word integrity is often used to mean honesty, but it also means a feeling of being whole, not scattered, and comfortable with oneself. (Integrity comes from the same root word as integer, a math term meaning “a whole number, not a fraction.”)

As an example of integrity, many older people express pride and contentment regarding their personal history. They are proud of their past, even when it includes events that an outsider might not consider worthy of pride—such as skipping school, taking drugs, or escaping arrest. Psychologists sometimes call this the “sucker to saint” phenomenon—that is, people interpret their experiences as signs of their nobility (saintliness), not their stupidity (Jordan & Monin, 2008).

As Erikson (1963) explains it, such self-glorifying distortions are far better than losing hope, or “feeling that the time is now short, too short for the attempt to start another life” (p. 269). As with every crisis, tension occurs between the two opposing aspects of development. Past crises, particularly Erikson’s fifth crisis of identity versus role confusion, reappear when the usual pillars of the self-concept (such as employment or good looks) crumble. One 70-year-old said, “I know who I’ve been, but who am I now?” (quoted in J. Kroger, 2007).

Fiercely Independent In the first half of his life, Nelson Mandela led the fight against apartheid in his native South Africa—until he was convicted of sabotage and sentenced to life in prison. Remarkably, he stayed true to his beliefs; released 27 years later to lead once again, he was elected president and served from 1994 to 1999. Still his true self, he next formed The Elders and pledged to be “a fiercely independent force for good.” Pictured here at age 92, he sat with two other Elders, Desmond Tutu (72) and Jimmy Carter (79). Mandela died 3 years later, at age 95, on December 5, 2013.
AP PHOTO/JEFF MOORE, POOL

This tension helps advance the person toward a more complete self-concept. In this last stage,

life brings many, quite realistic reasons for experiencing despair: aspects of the present that cause unremitting pain; aspects of a future that are uncertain and frightening. And, of course, there remains inescapable death, that one aspect of the future which is both wholly certain and wholly unknowable. Thus, some despair must be acknowledged and integrated as a component of old age.

[Erikson et al, 1986]

That integration of death and the self is an important accomplishment of this stage. The life review (explained in Chapter 14) and the acceptance of death (to be explained in the Epilogue) are crucial aspects of the integrity envisioned by Erikson (Zimmermann, 2012).

532

Holding on to One’s SelfMost older people consider their personalities and attitudes quite stable over their life span, even as they acknowledge the physical changes of their bodies (Fischer et al., 2008). One 103-year-old woman observed, “My core has stayed the same. Everything else has changed” (quoted in Troll & Skaff, 1997). Sometimes it is a struggle to maintain a strong sense of self. As bodies and social relationships change, adults may need to revise their self theory about their identity.

The need to maintain the self may explain behaviour that younger people might consider foolish. For example, many elders hate to give up driving a car because the independence that driving offers them has become an integral part of their view of self, especially for men (Davidson, 2008). Also, objects and places become more precious in late adulthood than they were earlier, as people seek a way to hold on to identity (J. Kroger, 2007; Whitmore, 2001).

The tendency to cling to familiar possessions may be problematic if it leads to compulsive hoarding. While younger relatives may complain that the accumulation of old papers, furniture, and mementos takes up space and is a fire hazard, for the elderly, compulsive hoarding can be seen as maintaining the self: Possessions become a part of self-expression, and the elderly resist self-destruction (Ayers et al., 2010).

Similarly, many older people refuse to move from drafty and dangerous dwellings into smaller, safer apartments because abandoning familiar places means abandoning personal history. Likewise, they may avoid surgery or reject medicine because they fear anything that might distort their thinking or emotions: Their priority is self-protection, even if it means shortening life (S. W Miller, 2011–2012).

The need to preserve one’s self explains why many of the elderly strive to maintain childhood cultural and religious practices. For instance, grandparents may painstakingly teach a grandchild a language that is rarely used in their current community, or encourage the child to repeat rituals and prayers they themselves learned as children. In cultures that emphasize youth and novelty, the elderly worry that their old values will be lost and, thus, that they themselves will disappear from memory.

The importance of self-validation is particularly apparent among older immigrants. Many grew up with customs unlike those where they and their descendants now live. They may become depressed if they think their culture is not appreciated and struggle to reconcile their traditional values with those of their children (Abu-Bader et al., 2011; H. Y. Lee et al., 2011).

The Positivity EffectAs you remember from Chapter 14, some people cope successfully with the changes of late adulthood through selective optimization with compensation. This is central to self theories. Individuals set their personal goals, assess their own abilities, and figure out how to accomplish those goals despite limitations. For some people, simply maintaining identity correlates with well-being (Ebner et al., 2006).

One strategy for selective optimization is known as the positivity effect. Elderly people are more likely to perceive, prefer, and remember positive images and experiences than negative ones (Carstensen et al., 2006). Compensation occurs via selective recall: Unpleasant experiences are reinterpreted as inconsequential. For example, with age, stressful events (economic loss; serious illness; the death of friends, family, or, in the future, oneself) are less likely to be considered central to one’s identity. That enables the elderly to be unperturbed by much of what happens; they maintain emotional health via positive self-perception (Boals et al., 2012).

The positivity effect is evident in many nations. After taking into account income, education, and gender, adults everywhere report gradually increasing happiness from age 50 on (Blanchflower et al., 2008).

533

A VIEW FROM SCIENCE

No Regrets

FIGURE 15.1 Let Bygones Be Bygones Young adults, depressed older adults, and emotionally healthy older adults played a computer game in which they could continue opening a series of boxes that they hoped would have money in them or risk finding a cartoon picture of the devil, which caused them to lose all their money and ended the game. Young adults and depressed older adults who discovered, at the end of a round, that they could have opened more boxes safely were more disappointed than were emotionally healthy, older adults. This was evident in their behaviour—they took more risks in subsequent rounds—and in increased activity in areas of the brain (the ventral striatal) that are activated when a person feels regret.

Many researchers have studied the positivity effect. They have found not only that a positive world view usually increase with age but also that it correlates with believing that life is meaningful (Hicks et al., 2012). Those elders who are highest in positivity (e.g., feeling happy, not frustrated or depressed) are likely to agree strongly that their life has a purpose (e.g., “I have a system of values that guides my daily activities” and “I am at peace with my past”) (Hicks et al, 2012).

Researchers have measured expressed attitudes and memories as well as brain and body reactions to disappointment. For example, Brassen and colleagues (2012) explored the reactions of adults of various ages to questions such as “When something doesn’t happen as you wish, are you likely to take a greater risk the next time, or are you likely to let go of your disappointment?” Emerging adults might take a greater risk, but emotionally healthy older adults are not likely to. The emotionally healthy elderly react to disappointment by letting it go and thinking positively about going forward (see Figure 15.1).

In this and many other experiments, older adults are found to be able to let bygones be bygones, leading to greater emotional well-being from middle age onward and fewer experiences of anger. In this particular study, the brain activity and heart rate of healthy older adults (average age 66) show a different response to disappointment.

FIGURE 15.2 Keep the Peace When someone does something mean or unpleasant, what is your goal in your interaction with that person? If your goal is to maintain goodwill, as was the case for a majority of older adults studied, you are likely to be quicker to forgive and forget.

This study also included a group of older adults who had been diagnosed with late-life depression. Their brains, bodies, and behaviour were more like those of the younger adults. The researchers concluded that those who are emotionally healthy have reduced responses to regretful events.

Other research has also found that depressed older adults are neurologically impaired, particularly in the anterior cingulate cortex, a brain region crucial for processing conflicting emotions and thoughts (Ochsner et al., 2009). Because of the positivity effect, anger, sadness, and disappointment are limited in healthy older people—and they are happier because of it.

The positivity effect is not always dramatic. Consider the details of a different study (Werheid et al., 2010). In four experiments, a total of 132 individuals—60 young (average age 25) and 72 old (average age 66)—looked at photographs of happy, neutral, or angry faces. They were then shown the same faces and an equal number of new faces either one day later (experiments 1, 2, and 3) or two weeks later (experiment 4), and were asked which faces they had seen before.

All four experiments showed the positivity effect somewhat, but not dramatically. For instance, in the first experiment, the older adults were slightly better than the younger ones at recognizing happy faces (74 versus 70 percent) and worse at recognizing angry faces (81 versus 84 percent) or neutral faces (65 versus 69 percent). They were also likely to mistakenly claim that the new happy faces were familiar (22 versus 7 percent).

The presence of even a mild positivity effect suggests that elders ignore or move past certain negative realities, such as expressions of anger and frustration. For example, adults in one study were asked about recent instances of personal confrontation (Sorkin & Rook, 2006). More than one-third (39 percent) of the adults older than 65 could not think of any negative social exchanges. Of those who remembered unpleasant encounters, most of the elders, unlike the younger participants, said that their primary goal after the event was to maintain goodwill. Only a few sought to change the other person’s behaviour (see Figure 15.2).

534

Since their goal was to achieve harmony, the elderly were more likely to compromise instead of insist that they were correct. This led to a happier outcome. As the researchers stated,

[p]articipants whose primary coping goal was to preserve goodwill reported the highest levels of perceived success and the least intense and shortest duration of distress. In contrast, participants whose…goal was to change the other person reported the lowest levels of perceived success and the most intense and longest lasting distress.

[Sorkin & Rook, 2006]

Stratification Theories

A second set of theories, called stratification theories, emphasizes societal forces that place each person in a social strata or level. Such stratification makes it difficult for a person to earn more income, have better health, or live longer than people of their group. Stratification begins from day one, as babies are conceived in societies that are already stratified, or differentiated, by various dimensions such as gender, ethnicity, and SES (Lynch & Brown, 2011).

The problem with stratification is that it results in inequality for reasons beyond the individual. As the decades of life go by, stratification by age, gender, ethnicity, and income become increasingly burdensome, causing double, triple, or even quadruple jeopardy. We describe each of these in turn.

Stratification By AgeAgeism is, of course, stratification by age. Age affects a person’s life in many ways, including income and health. For example, seniority builds in the workplace, increasing income up to a certain point; then employment may stop when a person reaches a given age, with that person perhaps earning a pension but never as much income as before.

This is just one example of the many ways in which industrialized nations segregate elderly people, gradually shutting them out of the mainstream of society as they grow older (Achenbaum, 2005). That harms all generations because it limits social experiences: Younger as well as older people have a narrower perspective on life if they interact only with people their own age.

Respected Twins Ageism takes many forms. Some cultures are youth-oriented, devaluing the old, while others are the opposite. These twin sisters were born in rural China in 1905. Pictured here, at age 103, they were venerated for their experience and wisdom.
IMAGINECHINA/AP IMAGES

The most controversial version of age-stratification theory is disengagement theory (Cumming & Henry, 1961). This theory holds that, as people age, traditional roles become unavailable or unimportant, the social circle shrinks, co-workers stop asking for help, and adult children turn away to focus on their own children. According to this theory, disengagement is a mutual process, chosen by both older and younger generations. Children want to be with other children, adults with other adults, and older adults with one another or by themselves. Thus, younger people disengage from the old, who themselves voluntarily disengage from younger adults. They relinquish past roles, withdrawing from life’s action.

Disengagement theory provoked a storm of protest because people feared it justified ageism and social isolation. Many gerontologists insisted that older people need and want new involvements. Some developed an opposing theory, called activity theory, which holds that the elderly seek to remain active with relatives, friends, and community groups. Activity theorists contend that if the elderly disengage, they do so unwillingly (J. R. Kelly, 1993; Rosow, 1985).

Later research has found that elders who are more active are happier, intellectually more alert, and less depressed. This is true at younger ages as well, although some studies find that activity is particularly likely to correlate with high functioning at older ages (Bielak, 2010; Bielak et al., 2012).

535

Both disengagement and activity theories need to be applied with caution, however. Disengagement in one aspect of life (e.g., retiring from employment) does not necessarily mean disengagement overall: Many retirees disengage from work but find new roles and activities to participate in (Freund et al., 2009). The positivity effect, just described, may mean that an older person disengages from emotional events that cause anger, regret, and sadness, while actively enjoying other experiences (Brassen et al., 2012).

A cautionary note comes from research in China. One study found that among the Chinese young-old, activity correlated with health, particularly if the activities involved social interactions. But among the oldest-old, activity did not correlate with longevity: For some of the oldest Chinese, disengagement was more closely associated with health (R. Sun & Liu, 2008). Both theories—that all the elderly want to withdraw and that they all should stay active—may arise from cultural stereotypes.

Never Too Old One would not expect an 80-year-old to skydive, but that is exactly what this active Israeli woman did!
ITAY BARSHADSKI/ISRAEL SUN/LANDOV

Stratification By GenderFeminist theory draws attention to stratification that puts males and females on separate tracks through life. From pink or blue blankets for newborns’ bassinets to flowers or stripes for nursing home bedsheets, gender is signalled throughout life. These signals alert everyone—caregivers, family members, and strangers—to treat males and females differently. Such stratification, when combined with ageism, makes people expect older women to be, for instance, either warm and giving (grandmotherly, providing kindness and cookies) or sickly and dependent. Older men might be seen, for example, as elder statesmen or as grumpy and curmudgeonly.

The implications of gender divergence are illustrated by a study of caregiving among older married couples. Both sexes provided care if their spouse became needy, but they did so in opposite ways: Women quit their jobs, whereas men worked longer. To be specific, employed women whose husbands needed care were five times more likely to retire than were other employed women. By contrast, employed husbands whose wife needed care retired only half as often as other men (Dentinger & Clarkberg, 2002).

A Nobel Prize Winner As a younger adult, author Alice Munro won three Governor General Awards (in 1968, 1978, and 1986), then as an older adult she won two Giller Prizes (in 1998 and 2004) and the Mann Booker International Prize (in 2009). In 2013, at the age of 82, she became the 13th woman, and 1st Canadian woman, to win the Nobel Prize in literature, in 2013. She was named “master of the contemporary short story.”
CHRIS YOUNG/CP IMAGES

Note, however, that both partners sacrifice. Men stayed on the job so they could keep insurance and hire household help, and women quit to provide personal care. Both sexes followed the gender stratification of decades earlier: Women were socialized to be caregivers, and men’s employment patterns (not part-time, more seniority) typically included higher salaries and better insurance than women’s patterns (more interruptions, and hence less pay and benefits). In this situation, past stratification disadvantages the older women: Their caregiving response leads to poverty and loneliness.

Irrational, gender-based fear may also lead to unequal experiences for older men and women. For example, because adult children want to protect their mothers, they are more likely to persuade their widowed mothers to live with them than they are their widowed fathers, thus limiting their mothers’ independence. The children’s fear for the mother’s safety is not based on evidence. Men living alone are more likely than women to have a sudden health crisis and are more likely to be the victim of a violent crime (5 percent versus 2 percent) (P. Klaus, 2005).

536

In another example of stratification, women typically marry men a few years older than they are and then outlive them. Especially if they lived in rural areas (as most North Americans did until about 1950), married women often relied on their husbands to drive, to manage money, and to keep up with politics. Then, if the husband died, past gender stratification led to isolation, poverty, and dependence among the oldest-old widows.

Men, too, may be harmed. For instance, boys are taught that males are stoic, repressing emotions and avoiding medical care. Is that why, in every nation, adult women outlive men? This could be biological (protective hormones), but it could result from lifelong stratification, making men more vulnerable in old age.

Stratification By EthnicityLike age and gender, ethnic background affects every aspect of development throughout life, including education, health, place of residence, and employment. Stratification theory suggests that these factors accumulate, creating large discrepancies by old age that leave ethnic minority seniors at risk of becoming marginalized (National Advisory Council On Aging, 2005).

When one considers Canadian seniors, stratification by ethnicity is probably most evident among immigrants, especially those who arrived recently, and among Aboriginal peoples. In 2006, about 30 percent of Canadian seniors were immigrants, many of them from visible minorities (Ng, 2010). Aboriginal peoples accounted for 1 percent of Canada’s seniors (Chief Public Health Officer, 2010).

ESPECIALLY FOR Social Scientists The various social science disciplines tend to favour different theories of aging. Can you tell which theories would be more acceptable to psychologists and which to sociologists?

Common problems faced by ethnic minority and Aboriginal seniors include greater rates of poverty and illness and, for Aboriginal peoples, shorter life expectancies. In 2001, 13 percent of Aboriginal seniors, compared to 7 percent of non-Aboriginal seniors, were living in low-income households. Similarly, 50 percent of Aboriginal seniors not living with family were in low-income situations compared to 40 percent of non-Aboriginal seniors in the same living situation (Chief Public Health Officer, 2010). This income discrepancy is largely due to higher levels of unemployment among the Aboriginal population, higher percentages of Aboriginals working part time, lower levels of participation in the labour force (i.e., employed or looking for work), and lower wages for those employed (Turcotte & Schellenberg, 2006).

Higher poverty rates are also present among elderly immigrants, particularly those who are recent immigrants as compared to those who have been in the country longer than 10 years (Eigersma, 2010). For example, in 2003, 26 percent of recent immigrant seniors were in the lowest income quartile compared with 12 percent of long-term immigrant seniors and 15 percent of non-immigrant seniors (Eigersma, 2010). Statistics Canada reports that, in general, recent immigrant seniors experience higher unemployment rates, lower earnings, and greater difficulties in matching their qualifications to their employment (Turcotte & Schellenberg, 2006). Difficulties in finding a suitable job consequently impair new immigrants’ efforts to accumulate sufficient funds for retirement. As well, when recent immigrants do retire, they are often ineligible for Old Age Security benefits since that program requires a 10-year residency in Canada before a person can collect benefits (Eigersma, 2010; Jackson, 2001). In a culture where workers are expected to retire and live on pension income, many older immigrants (without a host country work history) are left unemployed, poor, and lonely.

A particular form of ethnic stratification may affect immigrant elders who find themselves dependent on their adult children within a culture stratified against both the old and the immigrant. Many traditional cultures expect younger generations to care for the elderly personally, but North American families tend to be nuclear families, not extended ones.

For some cultures, the traditional notion of adult children caring for their aging parents remains strong. In some Chinese families, for example, the thought of placing an ailing and aged parent in a nursing home may conflict with their sense of responsibility. One recent response to this problem has been the appearance of ethnoculturally focused nursing homes such as the Yee Hong Geriatric Care Centre that services the Chinese community in Mississauga, Ontario. About 10 Asian-focused nursing homes now exist in Canada, mainly for the Chinese and South Asian communities, but demand far outstrips availability. The average wait time for a space in a mainstream seniors’ facility in Ontario is 113 days; at the Yee Hong Centre, it can take 10 years to find a place (Bascaramurty, 2012).

537

Since poverty often has negative impacts on a person’s health, it comes as no surprise that in 2001—the same year that 13 percent of Aboriginal seniors, compared to 7 percent of non-Aboriginal seniors, were living in low-income households—only 24 percent of non-reserve Aboriginal seniors reported that their health was very good to excellent (O’Donnell & Tait, 2003), compared to 36.4 percent among seniors in the general population (Statistics Canada, 2002). In 2003—the same year that 26 percent of recent immigrant seniors were in the lowest income quartile compared with 12 percent of long-term immigrant seniors and 15 percent of nonimmigrant seniors—only 28 percent of recent immigrant seniors reported that their health was very good to excellent, compared with 36 percent of long-term immigrants and 38 percent of non-immigrant seniors (Turcotte & Schellenberg, 2006).

A 2008 report for Statistics Canada found discrepancies in the mortality rates among various ethnic groups but especially for Aboriginal peoples. First Nations, Métis, and Inuit had substantially higher mortality rates than non-Aboriginal Canadians (see Figure 15.3). The highest rate of all was among registered Indians, a subgroup of First Nations (Wilkens et al., 2008). Another government report found that compared with other Canadian seniors, Aboriginal seniors tend to have double or triple the prevalence of certain chronic conditions that often end in death, such as heart disease, hypertension, diabetes, and arthritis (Health Canada in collaboration with the Interdepartmental Committee on Aging and Seniors Issues, 2002). Remember that living in poverty can place repeated stresses on a person that result in conditions such as hypertension and obesity as well as the other 8 to 14 indicators of allostatic load. Over the years, this can take a toll on a person’s health and well-being.

FIGURE 15.3 Lower Life Expectancies While the life expectancies of Aboriginal peoples have improved over time, they remain lower than that of the total population.

OBSERVATION QUIZ

Which Aboriginal group has the lowest life expectancy? Why might this be the case?

The Inuit. They are less likely to have access to health care services; their rates of smoking are higher than those of other populations; they are more likely to live in poorer conditions (crowded homes, unsafe drinking water); their levels of education are lower, which correlates with health and well-being, and so on.

Stratification By SESFinally, the pivotal influence on the well-being of the elderly may be financial, not directly gender, ethnicity, or age. Income correlates with those three but is not caused by them (Bird et al., 2010). A child of a family with low SES will have less education, worse health, and a poorer work history (and thus more unemployment, fewer benefits, no pensions). He or she will experiences stress of all kinds, accumulating disadvantages that are increasingly limiting as the years go by (Bowen & González, 2010).

538

The problem may begin even before birth, since epigenetic factors—themselves affected by maternal health—shape the genetic expression (Shanahan & Hofer, 2011). Those who are born into low-SES families risk late-life illnesses, such as diabetes, as well as disabilities. Obviously, poverty among elders should be alleviated, but mitigating poverty early in life may be critical for well-being in late adulthood (Herd et al., 2011).

In 2010, 5.3 percent of seniors in Canada were living in poverty—that is, below what Statistics Canada calls the Low-Income Cut-Off (LICO) (see the discussion on LICO in Chapter 1) (Conference Board of Canada, 2013a). This represents a significant decrease in the LICO rate among Canadian seniors over the last almost 40 years; in 1976, almost 30 percent of seniors found themselves below LICO (see Figure 15.4). At least one expert, economics professor Lars Osberg of Dalhousie University, has hailed this drop in poverty rates among seniors as “the major success story of Canadian social policy in the twentieth century” (Osberg, 2001).

FIGURE 15.4 Low-Income Rates Among Seniors The number of seniors living below LICO has decreased dramatically since the 1970s, largely because of payments from the Canada Pension Plan and Quebec Pension Plan, as well as from private pension plans.

Perhaps the main reason for the decrease was the institution of the Canada Pension Plan and Quebec Pension Plan in 1966. Canadian seniors can also access two other government transfer programs:

  1. Old Age Security. Almost all Canadians older than 65 receive OAS, which is an income supplement that averages about $450 a month.
  2. Guaranteed Income Supplement. This provides additional funds to low-income seniors who are enrolled in OAS and meet certain other requirements.

In addition, some seniors have access to their own savings or private pension plans that they enrolled in at work.

Despite the drop in poverty rates among seniors, one group that remains vulnerable to low income in old age is women, especially those older than 75 and who have been widowed or are living alone for other reasons. Women often receive less than men from government pension plans because the benefits are usually linked to a person’s employment history. In the period between 2006 and 2010, the number of Canadian seniors living below LICO increased by about 160 000, and 60 percent of them were women (Conference Board of Canada, 2013a).

One important financial problem for the elderly is that inflation makes retirement income worth less than half of what it was when that money was first set aside. Those middle-aged adults who have adequate savings for retirement are in the minority: Typically, they are already wealthy, another example of SES stratification.

539

Internationally, stratification by income for the elderly varies a great deal (see Figure 15.5). Some nations provide free health care and heavily subsidized senior residences for everyone. One of these is Denmark, which also has the highest proportion of happy seniors. By contrast, other nations provide nothing at all, expecting family members to care for the elderly. In several Asian nations, it is illegal for adult children not to provide for their parents.

FIGURE 15.5 When Below Average Is a Good Thing Canada has done relatively well in reducing poverty rates among its seniors population compared with other industrialized countries in the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD). On average, 12.8 percent of people over the age of 65 in OECD countries live in poverty, which is defined as having an income below half the national median. Canada’s poverty rate is 7.2 percent.

This is a developmental issue to which applying a cross-cultural approach would be useful. Every nation has had unexpectedly large increases in the number of older adults, and every nation is cutting government assistance to the economically disadvantaged. The resulting dilemma is more economically disadvantaged elders than in the past. Some nations have found better solutions than others, but nations can learn from one another.

KEY points

  • Self theories of late adulthood stress that people try to remain themselves, achieving integrity and not despairing.
  • The positivity effect protects the self, as elders take pleasure and pride in who they are.
  • The disengagement theory holds that as people age, they relinquish past roles and withdraw from life’s action.
  • Activity theory suggests that, if the aged disengage, it is not by choice. Contrary to disengagement theory, it suggests societies should encourage activity in old age.
  • Gender, ethnicity, and economic strata all place people on particular paths for life: This stratification may be particularly harmful in late adulthood.