Thinking, Fast and Slow

The body changes just reviewed are dramatic, but even more life-changing are cognitive advances. Teenagers no longer think like children, but they do not yet think like adults. We begin our discussion with the neurological changes of adolescence and then explore the intellectual ones.

Brain Development

Like the rest of the body, parts of the brain grow at various rates. Myelination and maturation occur in sequence, proceeding from the lower and inner parts of the brain to the cortex, and from the back to the frontal and then the prefrontal cortex (Sowell et al., 2007). That means that the limbic system (site of fear and anxiety) matures before regions where planning, emotional regulation, and impulse control occur (see Figure 9.3).

image
Figure 9.3: FIGURE 9.3 Same People, But Not the Same Brain These brain scans are part of a longitudinal study that repeatedly compared the proportion of gray matter from childhood through adolescence. (Gray matter refers to the cell bodies of neurons, which are less prominent with age as some neurons are unused.) Gray matter is reduced as white matter increases, in part because pruning during the teen years (the last two pairs of images here) allows intellectual connections to build. As the authors of one study that included this chart explained, teenagers may look “like an adult, but cognitively, they are not really there yet” (Powell, 2006, p. 865).

Furthermore, hormones directly target the amygdala (a key component of the limbic system) (Romeo, 2013), but full functioning of the cortex requires time more than hormones, which means that the prefrontal cortex is not mature until years of experience beyond the teen years.

LaunchPad

image

Video Activity: Brain Development: Adolescence features animations and illustrations of the changes that occur in the teenage brain.

This developmental mismatch is a particular problem in the twenty-first century when the hormones of puberty begin much earlier than social maturation. Teenagers are fully grown a decade before they are ready for adult roles. The result: Emotional rushes are unchecked by caution, reflection, or planning.

327

Teens seek excitement and pleasure, especially the joy of a peer’s admiration (Galván, 2013). In fact, when others are watching, teens find it thrilling to take daredevil risks that produce social acclaim (Albert et al., 2013). Especially when they are with friends, the thrill of new experiences and sensations can overpower the caution that their parents have tried to instill. Fatal car accidents, with many passengers, are one result.

The normal sequence of brain maturation (limbic system at puberty, prefrontal cortex sometime in the early 20s) combined with the earlier onset of puberty means that, for contemporary teenagers, emotions rule behavior for a decade.

When stress, arousal, passion, sensory bombardment, drug intoxication, or depri-vation occur, the adolescent brain is flooded with impulses that might shame adults. Teenagers brag about being so drunk that they were “wasted,” “bombed,” “smashed”—a state most adults try to avoid and would be ashamed to admit.

The parts of the brain dedicated to analysis are immature until long after the first hormonal rushes and sexual urges. Sadly, teenagers have access to fast cars, lethal weapons, and dangerous drugs before their brains are mature. My friend said to his neighbor, who gave his son a red convertible for high school graduation, “Why didn’t you just give him a loaded gun?” The mother of the 20-year-old who killed 20 children and 7 adults in Newtown, Connecticut, did just that. He killed her.

A more common example comes from teens sending text messages while they are driving. In one survey, among U.S. high school seniors who have their driver’s license, 60 percent texted while driving within the past month, although that is illegal almost everywhere (MMWR, June 13, 2014). More generally, despite quick reflexes and better vision, far more teenagers die in motor-vehicle accidents than older drivers do. Thoughtless impulses and poor decisions are to blame.

Any decision, from whether to eat a peach to where to go to college, requires balancing risk and reward, caution and attraction. Experiences, memories, emotions, and the prefrontal cortex help people choose to avoid some actions and perform others. Since the reward parts of adolescents’ brains are stronger than the inhibition parts (Luna et al., 2013), many adolescents act in ways that seem foolhardy to adults.

328

A VIEW FROM SCIENCE

The Pleasures of the Adolescent Brain

Some risk-taking experiments show notable differences in brain activity (specifically in the ventral striatum) between adolescents and adults. When they are with other adults, the adults’ brains give more signals of caution (inhibition)–unlike adolescents’ brains when they are with peers (Albert et al., 2013).

When peers are watching, the adolescent brain’s pleasure at a risky win apparently outweighs the brain’s disappointment at a risky loss. This is demonstrated with simulated driving, when a teen is at the wheel (on a computer) with two friends nearby, and a traffic light turns from green to yellow. Teens—but not adults with other adults—often gun the accelerator.

The joy of beating the light activates the pleasure centers of their brains, and failure—a crash—does not impact their brains as much. The opposite is true for adults (Gardner & Steinberg, 2005; Chein et al., 2011). Teenagers are likely to say “nothing ventured, nothing gained” and adults, “better safe than sorry.”

That this peer effect involves brain pleasure, rather than merely response to the encouragement of friends, was shown in another study. Gambling choices with known odds were presented to 15- to 17-year-olds (Smith et al., 2014). Half of the teens (the experimental group) were told that another teen was watching them on a closed-circuit TV. The other half (the control group) were told that no one was watching. Risk-taking was tallied and brain activity was noted.

When the odds were good, almost all the teens gambled, no matter whether they thought a peer was watching or not. But when odds of a loss outnumbered those of a win by 2:1, 40 percent nonetheless took the chance when a peer was watching. Only 15 percent did so without the observer.

image
Figure 9.4: FIGURE 9.4 Look Before You Leap As you can see, adolescents become less impulsive as they mature, but they still enjoy the thrill of a new sensation.

The researchers suggest that when a peer observes, the pleasure centers in the brain are most strongly activated when the odds are against success. This is discouraging to any adult who thinks that warning a teenager about the likelihood of HIV/AIDS, addiction, or death will stop risk taking: Greater risk may lead to greater pleasure.

This study had only 52 participants. Indeed, most such experiments involve relatively few participants, in part because brain scans are expensive. As you remember from Chapter 1, scientists often use other methods to confirm results found in one type of study. That has happened with this research.

One longitudinal survey of more than 7,000 adolescents, from age 12 through age 24, assessed their ideas, activities, and plans (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011). Some questions were about sensation seeking (e.g., “I enjoy new and exciting experiences”). Increases were notable from ages 12 to 14 (see Figure 9.4). Some questions were about impulsivity, as in “I often get in a jam because I do things without thinking.”

On average, sensation seeking accelerated rapidly at puberty, and both sensation seeking and impulsivity slowly declined with maturation. However, trajectories varied individually: Sensation seeking did not necessarily correlate with impulsivity. Thus, the limbic system was not necessarily linked to the prefrontal cortex (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011). Both affect behavior: Risky sex correlates with sensation seeking and with impulsivity (Charnigo et al., 2013), but each has an independent impact.

The results were “consistent with neurobiological research indicating that cortical regions involved in impulse control and planning continue to mature through early adulthood . . . [and that] subcortical regions that respond to emotion, novelty, and reward are more responsive in middle adolescence than in either children or adults” (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011, p. 743).

The same idea is presented in the diagram. Two experts in cognitive development (Duckworth & Steinberg, 2015) agree that impulse control (the prefrontal cortex) continues to mature throughout childhood and adolescence. The problem is that sensation seeking suddenly escalates at puberty, temporarily outstripping growth of the parts of the brain that keep emotional impulses in check.

What can be concluded from all the brain science on adolescents? Young brains do not react as older ones do. As with adventuresome toddlers (the “little scientists”) who might run toward exploration and danger, the admirable curiosity and bravery of the young human is a path toward learning as well as pleasure. Adults must ensure that adventure leads to wisdom, not harm.

329

BENEFITS OF ADOLESCENT BRAIN DEVELOPMENT It is easy for adults to be critical of adolescent behavior and blame it on hormones, peers, culture, or the latest chosen culprit, brains. Exaggerated metaphors have changed from blaming puberty as the cause of “storm and stress” to blaming brains for “all gas and no brakes” (Payne, 2012).

image
Yes, Not No Diving into cold water with your friends is thrilling if you are a teenage boy and a girl is watching. Adult prohibition increases the joy.

Yet remember that difference is not always deficit. Gains as well as losses are part of every developmental period, and continuity from one life period to another is evident even as each stage includes specific problems. What might be the benefits of the adolescent brain?

Increasing myelination and slower inhibition allows lightning fast reactions, making adolescent athletes quick and fearless as they steal a base, tackle a fullback, or sprint when their lungs feel about to burst. Ideally, coaches channel such bravery.

Furthermore, as the brain’s reward areas activate positive neurotransmitters, teenagers become happy. A new love, a first job, a college acceptance, or even an A on a term paper can produce a rush of joy, to be remembered and cherished lifelong.

Teenagers need to take risks and learn new things, because “[t]he fundamental task of adolescence—to achieve adult levels of social competence—requires a great deal of learning about the complexities of human social interactions” (Peper & Dahl, 2013, p. 135). That is exactly what their brains enable adolescents to do.

Synaptic growth enhances moral development as well. Adolescents question their elders and forge their own standards. This is an asset if adolescent values are less self-centered and more relevant to current conditions than those of older generations. Values embraced during adolescence are more likely to endure than those acquired later, after brain connections are firmly established.

THINK CRITICALLY: Given the nature of adolescent brain development, how should society respond to adolescent thoughts and actions?

Thus, the developing prefrontal cortex “confers benefits as well as risks. It helps explain the creativity of adolescence and early adulthood, before the brain becomes set in its ways” (Monastersky, 2007, p. A17). The emotional intensity of adolescents “intertwines with the highest levels of human endeavor: passion for ideas and ideals, passion for beauty, passion to create music and art” (Dahl, 2004, p. 21).

Formal Operational Thought

image
Brains and Bodies Can you imagine yourself in their shoes? People of all ages play basketball but these boys display all the signs of adolescence. Note the long, skinny legs and arms, growing before the heads and torsos, and the impulsive brain that provoked the shooter to try the long shot, and the other two to risk injury by jumping so high.

formal operational thought

In Piaget’s theory, the fourth and final stage of cognitive development, characterized by systematic logical thinking and by understanding abstractions.

Adolescents move past concrete operational thinking (discussed in Chapter 7) and consider abstractions. Jean Piaget described a shift to formal operational thought, including “assumptions that have no necessary relation to reality” (Piaget, 1950/2010, p. 148).

EGOCENTRISM For many adolescents, the first step toward abstract thought is very personal, although unrealistic, thought. They think deeply about themselves. One reason adolescents spend so much time talking on the phone, e-mailing, and texting is that they like to ruminate about each nuance of whatever they have done, might have done, and could do.

image
All Eyes on Me Egocentrism and obsession with appearance are hallmarks of adolescence, as shown by these high school cheerleaders. Given teenage thinking, it is not surprising that many boys and girls seek stardom, sometimes with fierce competition within teams and between schools. Cooperation and moderation are more difficult.

330

adolescent egocentrism

A characteristic of adolescent thinking that leads young people to believe in their own uniqueness, and to imagine that other people are also focused on them.

They also imagine what others—teachers, parents, friends, even classmates who seem to ignore them—think of them. Together these two aspects of thought are called adolescent egocentrism, first described by David Elkind (1967).

Egocentrism leads adolescents to interpret everyone else’s behavior as if it were a judgment on them. A stranger’s frown or a teacher’s critique could make a teenager conclude that “No one likes me” and then deduce that “I am unlovable” or even claim that “I can’t leave the house.”

More positive casual reactions—a smile from a barista or an extra-big hug from a younger brother—could lead to “I am great” or “Everyone loves me” or similarly distorted self-perception. Given the rapid mood changes of adolescence, such conclusions are usually short-lived, susceptible to reversal with another offhand remark.

personal fable

The belief that one’s own emotions, experiences, and destiny are unique, more wonderful or awful than anyone else’s.

invincibility fable

The fantasy that a person cannot be harmed by anything that might defeat a normal mortal, such as unprotected sex, drug abuse, or high-speed driving.

Several aspects of adolescent egocentrism are named, among them the personal fable and the invincibility fable, which often appear together (Alberts et al., 2007). The personal fable is the belief that one is unique, destined to have a heroic, fabled, legendary life. Some 12-year-olds expect to play in the NBA, or become billionaires, or cure cancer. Invincible means impervious to harm.

Some adolescents see no contradiction between the personal fable and invincibility. Some believe that they will not be hurt by fast driving, unprotected sex, or addictive drugs unless fate wills it. If they survive unscathed from a dangerous risk, they may feel invincible, not relieved; special, not unlucky.

imaginary audience

The other people who, in an adolescent’s egocentric belief, watch his or her appearance, ideas, and behavior.

Egocentrism creates an imaginary audience in the minds of many adolescents. They believe they are at center stage, with all eyes on them, and they imagine how others might react to their appearance and behavior.

image
Duck, Duck, Goose Far more teens are injured in bicycle accidents than hunting ones, because almost all young people ride bicycles and relatively few are hunters. However, especially when no adult is present, young hunters are less likely to wear blaze orange, to attend safety classes, and be licensed to hunt. Most likely these boys will return home safe, without the duck they seek. Nonetheless, guns and off-road vehicles are leading causes of death under age 18, so this scene is not a comforting one.

331

The imaginary audience can cause teenagers to enter a crowded room as if they are the most attractive human beings alive. They might put studs in their lips or blast music for all to hear, calling attention to themselves. The reverse is sometimes evident: Many a 12-year-old refuses to leave the house with a bad haircut or the wrong shoes.

image
A Proud Teacher “Is it possible to train a cockroach?” This hypothetical question, an example of formal operational thought, was posed by 15-year-old Tristan Williams of New Mexico. In his award-winning science project, he succeeded in conditioning Madagascar cockroaches to hiss at the sight of a permanent marker. (His parents’ logic about sharing their home with 600 cockroaches is unknown.)

PIAGET’S EXPERIMENTS Egocentrism can coexist with more logical and abstract intelligence. Piaget and his colleagues devised tasks to assess formal operational thought (Inhelder & Piaget, 1958/2013b).

In one experiment (diagrammed in Figure 9.5), children of many ages balance a scale by hooking weights onto the scale’s arms. To master this task, they must realize that the weights’ heaviness and distance from the center interact reciprocally to affect balance.

image
Figure 9.5: FIGURE 9.5 How to Balance a Scale Piaget’s balance-scale test of formal reasoning, as it is attempted by (a) a 4-year-old, (b) a 7-year-old, (c) a 10-year-old, and (d) a 14-year-old. The key to balancing the scale is to make weight times distance from the center equal on both sides of the center; the realization of that principle requires formal operational thought.

The concept of balancing was completely beyond the 3- to 5-year-olds. By age 7, children could balance the scale by putting the same amount of weight on each arm, but they didn’t realize that the distance from the center mattered.

By age 10, children thought about location but used trial and error, not logic. Finally, by about age 13 or 14, some children hypothesized and tested the reciprocal relationship between weight and distance and developed the correct formula (Piaget & Inhelder, 1972).

In balancing, as in all of Piaget’s experiments, “in contrast to concrete operational children, formal operational adolescents imagine all possible determinants . . . [and] systematically vary the factors one by one, observe the results correctly, keep track of the results, and draw the appropriate conclusions” (P. Miller, 2011, p. 57).

LaunchPad

Video Activity: The Balance Scale Task shows children of various ages completing the task and gives you an opportunity to try it as well.

HYPOTHETICAL-DEDUCTIVE REASONING One hallmark of formal operational thought is the capacity to think of possibility, not just reality. “Here and now” is only one of many alternatives, including “there and then,” “long, long ago,” “nowhere,” “not yet,” and “never.” As Piaget said:

332

The adolescent . . . thinks beyond the present and forms theories about everything, delighting especially in considerations of that which is not. . . .

[Piaget, 1950/2010, p. 147]

image
Triple Winners Sharing the scholarship check of $100,000, these high school students are not only high achievers, but they also have learned to collaborate within a comprehensive public school (Hewlett). They learned much more than formal operational logic.

hypothetical thought

Reasoning that includes propositions and possibilities that do not reflect reality.

Adolescents are primed to engage in hypothetical thought, reasoning about if–then propositions that do not reflect reality. For example, consider this question (adapted from De Neys & Van Gelder, 2009):

If all mammals can walk,

And whales are mammals,

Can whales walk?

Younger adolescents often answer “No!” They know that whales swim, not walk, so the logic escapes them. Some adolescents answer “Yes.” They understand if.

Possibility no longer appears merely as an extension of an empirical situation or of actions actually performed. Instead, it is reality that is now secondary to possibility.

[Inhelder & Piaget, 1958/2013b, p. 251; emphasis in original]

Because of this new ability, many adolescents sharply criticize their parents, their school, their society. They “naively underestimate the practical problems involved in achieving an ideal future for themselves or for society” (P. Miller, 2011, p. 59). Why doesn’t everyone get good health care; why don’t the Palestinians and Israelis agree; why doesn’t the teacher realize that my question is brilliant; why doesn’t Dad let me stay out past midnight?

deductive reasoning

Reasoning from a general statement, premise, or principle, through logical steps, to figure out (deduce) specifics. (Also called top-down reasoning.)

In developing the capacity to think hypothetically, adolescents gradually become capable of deductive reasoning, or top-down reasoning. Deductive reasoning begins with an abstract idea or premise and then uses logic to draw specific conclusions. This ability improves during early adolescence (Markovits, 2014).

inductive reasoning

Reasoning from specific experiences or facts to reach (induce) a general conclusion. (Also called bottom-up reasoning.)

By contrast, during the primary school years, children accumulate facts and personal experiences (the knowledge base), asking what and why. The result is inductive reasoning, or bottom-up reasoning, with many specific examples leading to general conclusions (see Figure 9.6).

333

image
Figure 9.6: FIGURE 9.6 Bottom Up or Top Down? Children, as concrete operational thinkers, are likely to draw conclusions on the basis of their own experiences and what they have been told. This is called inductive, or bottom-up, reasoning. Adolescents can think deductively, from the top down.

One example of moving past personal experience in reasoning ability is the understanding of prejudice. Almost every adolescent knows that racism exists—and opposes it. However, children tend to think the core problem is the attitudes of individuals. Using inductive reasoning, they think that the remedy is to increase tolerance among individuals who are racist.

By contrast, adolescents begin to think, deductively, that policy solutions need to attack racism. That is formal operational thinking.

This interpretation arises from a study of adolescent agreement or disagreement with policies to reduce racial discrimination (Hughes & Bigler, 2011). Not surprisingly, most students in an interracial U.S. high school recognized disparities between African and European Americans and believed that racism was a major cause. What was surprising to the researchers is that age made a difference.

Among those who were aware of marked inequalities, more older adolescents (age 16 to 17) supported systemic solutions (e.g., affirmative action and desegregation) than did younger adolescents (age 14 to 15). The researchers wrote: “during adolescence, cognitive development facilitates the understanding that discrimination exists at the social-systemic level . . . racism awareness begins to inform views of race-conscious policies during middle adolescence” (Hughes & Bigler, 2011, p. 489).

In thinking about race and many other issues, neither adolescents nor adults necessarily reason at the formal operational level. Piaget probably overestimated the prevalence of this fourth period of intelligence. As now discussed, many contemporary scholars believe there are two modes of thinking, and that most people, most of the time, do not use formal operational thought (Barrouillet, 2011).

Two Modes of Thinking

dual-process model

The idea that two modes of thinking exist within the human brain, one for intuitive emotional responses and one for analytical reasoning.

Advanced logic in adolescence is counterbalanced by the increasing power of intuitive thinking. A dual-process model of cognition has been formulated (Albert & Steinberg, 2011) (see Visualizing Development, p. 322). This echoes what you have learned about the brain: Sensation seeking temporarily outweighs impulse control (Duckworth & Steinberg, 2015).

INTUITION VERSUS ANALYSIS Cognitive psychologists use various terms for and descriptions of two modes of human cognition (Evans & Stanovich, 2013). The terms include: intuitive/analytic, implicit/explicit, creative/factual, contextualized/decontextualized, unconscious/conscious, gist/quantitative, emotional/intellectual, experiential/rational, personal/impersonal, hot/cold, systems 1 and 2. Although these two modes interact and can overlap, each mode is distinct (Kuhn, 2013).

334

image

335

Question 9.16

OBSERVATION QUIZ

Melissa seems to working by herself, but what sign do you see that suggests she is part of a team who built this robot?

The flag on the robot matches her T-shirt. Often teenagers wear matching shirts to signify their joint identity.

image
Impressive Connections This robot is about to compete in the Robotics Competition in Atlanta, Georgia, but much more impressive are the brains of the Oregon high school team (including Melissa, shown here) who designed the robot.

In describing adolescent cognition, here we use the terms intuitive and analytic.

intuitive thought

Thought that arises from an emotion or a hunch, a “gut feeling” influenced by past experiences and cultural assumptions.

The thinking described by the first half of each pair is easier and quicker, preferred in everyday life. We are all “predictably irrational” at times (Ariely, 2009). Sometimes, however, the second mode is needed, when deeper thought is demanded.

The discrepancy between the maturation of the limbic system and of the prefrontal cortex tilts adolescents toward the more emotional, personal, experiential mode. Adolescents tend to be “fast and furious” intuitive thinkers, unlike their teachers and parents, who prefer slower, more analytic thinking.

Experiences and role models influence choices, not only in deciding what to do but also in choosing which intellectual process to use in making a decision. Conversations, observations, and debate all advance cognition and lead to a deeper consideration of the facts (Kuhn, 2013). As they grow older, adolescents sometimes gain in logic and sometimes regress; the social context and training in statistics are major influences on adolescent cognition (Klaczynski & Felmban, 2014).

To test yourself on intuition and analysis, answer the following three problems:

  1. Question 9.17

    A bat and a ball cost $1.10 in total. The bat costs $1 more than the ball. How much does the ball cost?

    Intuitive Analytic
    10 cents 5 cents
  2. Question 9.18

    If it takes 5 minutes for 5 machines to make 5 widgets, how long would it take 100 machines to make 100 widgets?

    Intuitive Analytic
    100 minutes 5 minutes
  3. Question 9.19

    In a lake, there is a patch of lily pads. Every day the patch doubles in size. If it takes 48 days for the patch to cover the entire lake, how long would it take for the patch to cover half the lake?

    Intuitive Analytic
    24 days 47 days

[From Gervais & Norenzayan, 2012]

336

Although many researchers find that logic gradually overcomes biased reasoning during adolescence, whether or not such analysis is used depends on specifics. One instance when being older did not improve logic occurred when adolescents were asked to decide how likely a hypothetical girl named Jennifer, described as lazy and not well liked, was to be obese. Among those teenagers who idealized thinness, the older ones more often jumped to the illogical conclusion that Jennifer was probably obese (Klaczynski & Felmban, 2014).

In dozens of studies, being smarter as measured by an intelligence test does not advance logic as much as having more experience, in school and in life. Students can learn to use statistics and to respect expert opinion, and that helps them think more rationally—but not always (Kail, 2013). Many parents of smart adolescents are surprised to realize this, as in the following.

A CASE TO STUDY

“What Were You Thinking?”

Laurence Steinberg is a noted expert on adolescence (e.g., Steinberg, 2014). He is also a father.

When my son, Benjamin, was 14, he and three of his friends decided to sneak out of the house where they were spending the night and visit one of their girlfriends at around two in the morning. When they arrived at the girl’s house, they positioned themselves under her bedroom window, threw pebbles against her windowpanes, and tried to scale the side of the house. Modern technology, unfortunately, has made it harder to play Romeo these days. The boys set off the house’s burglar alarm, which activated a siren and simultaneously sent a direct notification to the local police station, which dispatched a patrol car. When the siren went off, the boys ran down the street and right smack into the police car, which was heading to the girl’s home. Instead of stopping and explaining their activity, Ben and his friends scattered and ran off in different directions through the neighborhood.

. . . After his near brush with the local police, Ben had returned to the house out of which he had snuck, where he slept soundly until I awakened him with an angry telephone call, telling him to gather his clothes and wait for me in front of his friend’s house. On our drive home, after delivering a long lecture about what he had done and about the dangers of running from armed police in the dark when they believe they may have interrupted a burglary, I paused.

“What were you thinking?” I asked.

“That’s the problem, Dad,” Ben replied, “I wasn’t.”

[Steinberg, 2004, pp. 51, 52]

Steinberg realized that his son was right: When emotions are intense, especially when friends are nearby, the logical part of the brain shuts down. This shutdown is not reflected in questionnaires that require teenagers to respond to paper-and-pencil questions regarding hypothetical dilemmas. In fact, when strong emotions are not activated, teenagers may be more logical than adults (Casey & Caudle, 2013). They remember facts they have learned in biology or health class about sex and drugs. However,

the prospect of visiting a hypothetical girl from class cannot possibly carry the excitement about the possibility of surprising someone you have a crush on with a visit in the middle of the night. It is easier to put on a hypothetical condom during an act of hypothetical sex than it is to put on a real one when one is in the throes of passion . . . It is easier to just say no to a hypothetical beer than it is to a cold frosty one on a summer night.

[Steinberg, 2004, p. 53]

Ben reached adulthood safely. Some other non-thinking teenagers, with less cautious police or less diligent parents, do not.

PREFERRING EMOTIONS Why do high school students, well aware of the logic of the science, sometimes not use formal operational thinking? Dozens of experiments and extensive theorizing have found some answers (Albert & Steinberg, 2011).

Essentially, analysis is more difficult than intuition. It requires questioning ideas that are comforting and familiar. Once people of any age reach an emotional conclusion (sometimes called a “gut feeling”), they resist changing their minds.

337

Furthermore, weighing alternatives and thinking of possibilities can be paralyzing. The systematic, analytic thought that Piaget called formal operational is slow and costly, not fast and frugal, wasting precious time when a young person wants to act.

One characteristic of adolescents—suspicion of authority—may advance analysis, because teenagers use logic to argue against tradition. But, the same trait may work in the opposite direction. One of my students used logic (quoting the U.S. Constitution) to argue with a police officer who was questioning her young cousin: When the officer grabbed the cousin, she impulsively bit his hand—and spent months in jail.

It is helpful to realize that, even when they are analytical, teenagers might analyze a situation and reach a conclusion other than the one their parents would. A 15-year-old who is offered a cigarette, for example, might rationally choose peer acceptance over the distant risk of cancer. How likely is a teenager who wants to be “cool” or “bad” to say, “No thank you, my mother wouldn’t approve”?

THINK CRITICALLY: When might an emotional response to a problem be better than an analytic one?

More generally, adults want adolescents to take care of their health, which means avoiding cigarettes, junk food, and promiscuity. But emotional thinking pushes in the opposite direction, and some analytic arguments (about the risk of cancer, for instance) are dismissed (Gibbons et al., 2012a). That is why effective messages to halt teen smoking discuss smelly breath, stained fingers, and yellow teeth.

Digital Natives

Adults over age 40 grew up without the Internet, instant messaging, Twitter, Snapchat, blogs, cell phones, smartphones, MP3 players, tablets, or digital cameras. In contrast, today’s teenagers have been called digital natives, although if that term implies that they know everything about digital communication, it is a misnomer (boyd, 2014).

A huge gap between those with and without computers was bemoaned a decade ago; it divided boys from girls and rich from poor (Dijk, 2005; Norris, 2001). Now that digital divide is shrinking, and a new one is evident, between the old and the young.

image
Not All Thumbs After two days of competition among 22 qualified contestants, with tests of texting speed, clarity, and knowledge, 15-year-old Kate Moore of Des Moines, Iowa, was declared champion. She won a trophy and $50,000. She has texted hundreds of friends for years.

338

As costs tumble, the smartphone has been particularly important among low-SES adolescents of every ethnic group, used primarily to connect with friends (Madden et al., 2013). Connection to peers has always been important to teenagers, and that has always been feared by adults—who in earlier generations feared that the automobile, or the shopping mall, or rock and roll would lead their children astray.

TECHNOLOGY AND COGNITION In general, educators accept, even welcome, students’ facility with technology. There are “virtual” schools in which students earn all their credits online, never entering a school building, and in several school districts all the students have tablets.

Video: The Impact of the Media on Adolescent Development

Remember that research before the technology explosion found that direct instruction, practice, conversation, and experience advance adolescent thought. Social networking via technology may speed up this process, as teens communicate daily with dozens—perhaps hundreds or thousands—of “friends” via texting or instant messaging.

Most secondary students check facts, read explanations, view videos, and thus grasp concepts they would not have understood without technology. For some adolescents, the Internet is their only source of information about health and sex. Almost every high school student in the United States uses the Internet for research, finding it quicker and more extensive than books on library shelves.

Educators claim that the most difficult aspect of technology is teaching students how to evaluate sources, some reputable, some nonsensical. To this end, teachers explain the significance of .com, .org, .edu, and .gov (O’Hanlon, 2013).

ABUSE AND ADDICTION? Parents worry about sexual abuse via the Internet. Research is reassuring: Although sexual predators lurk online, most teens never encounter them. Sexual abuse is a serious problem, but if sexual abuse is defined as a perverted older stranger taking advantage of an innocent teenager met online, it is “extremely rare” (Mitchell et al., 2013, p. 1226).

The data show that the percent of teenagers who say that someone online tried to get them to talk about sex declined since 2000 from 10 to 1. Those 1 percent were almost always solicited by another young person whom the teenager already knew—a Facebook friend, for instance (Mitchell et al., 2013).

For some adolescents, chat rooms, video games, and Internet gambling may be considered addictive, taking time from active play, schoolwork, and friendship. Internet addiction is considered a problem in many nations (Tang et al., 2014; Y–H. Lee et al., 2015). For example, among high school students, 15 percent in Turkey and 12 percent in India are said to be addicted to computer use (S¸as¸maz et al., 2014; Yadav et al., 2013).

However, some scholars worry that adults tend to pathologize normal teen behavior, particularly in China, where rehabilitation centers are strict—some would say abusive—in keeping teenagers from Internet use (Bax, 2014). Parents may make the same mistake elsewhere (boyd, 2014).

The North American psychiatrists who wrote the new DSM-5 decided against including Internet use as an addiction. Instead, they wrote that further study was needed.

cyberbullying

Bullying that occurs when one person attacks and harms another via technology (e.g., e-mails, text messages, or cell phones).

CYBER DANGER When a person is bullied via electronic devices, usually via social media, text messages, or smartphone videos, that is cyberbullying. The adolescents involved in cyberbullying are usually already bullies or victims or both, with bully-victims especially likely to engage in, and suffer from, cyberbullying. Technology does not create bullies; it gives them another means to act and a larger audience, which multiplies the harm (Kowalski et al., 2014).

339

image
Fake Face in Georgia Alex stands behind a phony Facebook page that portrays her as a racist, lesbian drug abuser. She is 14, a late developer, which may be one reason she became a cyberbullying target. Also shown are her parents, Amy and Chris Boston, who sued her classmates for libel. The case was dismissed, but then the Court of Appeals of the State of Georgia sent the case back to the local court, suggesting that the parents of the cyberbullies should face trial, since they did not take the site down until a month after the first lawsuit. No matter what happens in court, the worst has already happened: Alex thought the bullies were her friends.

Texted and posted rumors and insults can “go viral,” reaching thousands, transmitted day and night. Some adolescents take videos of others drunk, naked, or crying and send them to dozens of others, who may send them to yet others, who may post them on YouTube or Vine. Since young adolescents are impulsive and low on judgment, cyberbullying is particularly prevalent and cruel between ages 11 and 14.

Although the causes of all forms of bullying are similar, each form has its own sting. Cyberbullying is most harmful if the victim believes in the imaginary audience, if sexual impulses are new, and if impulsive thoughts precede analytic ones. All that means that young adolescent victims of cyberbullying are likely to suffer from depression, even suicide (Bonanno & Hymel, 2013).

image
Something Worth Sharing But what is it? Is it the same as boys everywhere, or is it something specific to their culture? The four are in England: We do not know if they see a football (soccer) score, a prime minister’s proclamation, or a naked female.

The vulnerability of adolescence was tragically evident for a California 15-year-old (Sulek, 2013). At a sleepover, Audrie Pott and her friends found alcohol. She got so drunk that she blacked out, or passed out. On the next school day, three boys in her school were bragging that they had had sex with her, showing cell-phone pictures to prove it. The next weekend, Audrie hanged herself. Only then did her parents learn what had happened.

One aspect of this tragedy will come as no surprise to adolescents: sexting, as sending sexual photographs is called. As many as 30 percent of adolescents report having received sext photos. Many teens send their own sexy “selfies” and are happy to receive sext messages (Temple et al., 2014). As with Internet addiction, researchers have yet to agree on how to measure sexting or how harmful it is.

340

There are two dangers: (1) Pictures may be forwarded without the person’s knowledge, and (2) those who send erotic self-images risk serious depression if the reaction is negative (Temple et al., 2014). Many teens have distorted self-concepts—no wonder sexting is fraught with trouble.

The danger of all forms of technology lies not in the equipment but in the cognition of the user. That is the conclusion of a national survey which found that 7 percent of all teens willingly sent sext photos of themselves, yet sexting correlated with risky sex, depression, and low self-esteem (Ybarra & Mitchell, 2014).

As is true of many aspects of adolescence (puberty, eating disorders, brain development, egocentric thought, use of contraception, and so on), context, adults, peers, and the adolescent’s own personality and temperament “shape, mediate, and/or modify effects” of technology (Oakes, 2009, p. 1142).

One careful observer claims that instead of being native users of technology, many teenagers are naive users—believing they have privacy settings that they do not have, trusting sites that are markedly biased, misunderstanding how to search for and verify information (boyd, 2014).

THINK CRITICALLY: The older people are, the more likely they are to be critical of social media. Is that wisdom or ignorance? Why?

Educators can help with all this—but only if they themselves understand technology and teens. Teens are intuitive, impulsive, and egocentric, often unaware of the impact of what they send or overestimating the validity of what they read. Adults should know better, but this discussion makes it apparent that people of all ages are sometimes illogical and emotional about technology.

341

WHAT HAVE YOU LEARNED?

Question 9.20

1. Why does the limbic system develop before the prefrontal cortex?

This happens because myelination and maturation begin in the lower and inner parts of the brain to the cortex, and from the back to the frontal cortex and then the prefrontal cortex. Hormones also play a part: they have a much greater affect on the amygdala than on the prefrontal cortex, which needs time to mature.

Question 9.21

2. How might seeking sensations lead an adolescent to trouble?

Because the frontal lobe is still maturing, emotions rule adolescent behavior and impulsivity reigns relatively unchecked by logical thought. Adolescents take risks, such as driving fast and texting while they are driving. Thoughtless impulses and poor decisions are almost always to blame when teenagers die in motor-vehicle accidents.

Question 9.22

3. What are some of the benefits of adolescent brain function?

(1) Faster reaction time; (2) increased positive emotions; (3) easier acquisition of new ideas; (4) enhanced moral development; and 5) willingness to question tradition and try new things.

Question 9.23

4. How does adolescent egocentrism differ from early-childhood egocentrism?

In early childhood, egocentrism refers to the inability to take another person’s perspective. Young adolescents not only think intensely about themselves but also think about what others think of them. Adolescents regard themselves as unique, special, and much more socially significant than they actually are.

Question 9.24

5. What perceptions arise from belief in the imaginary audience?

Adolescents believe that everyone is watching them, which makes them very self-conscious. They may become shy and awkward or try to fade into the background so no one notices them. Others may seek to be the center of attention and do things to draw attention to themselves.

Question 9.25

6. What are the characteristics of formal operational thinking?

As concrete operational thinkers, children draw conclusions on the basis of their own experiences and what they have been told. Formal operational adolescents imagine all possible determinants and systematically vary the factors one by one, observe the results, keep track of the results, and draw the appropriate conclusions. Formal operational thinking enables systematic, logical thinking as well as the ability to understand and manipulate abstract concepts. Teens become capable of deductive reasoning and hypothetical thinking.

Question 9.26

7. What is the difference between inductive reasoning and deductive reasoning?

In inductive reasoning (which is more likely to be used by children), many specific examples lead to a general conclusion. In deductive reasoning (of which adolescents are more capable), an abstract idea or premise is the starting point, and then logic is employed to draw a specific conclusion. Inductive reasoning is quick and automatic, but also more prone to error than deductive reasoning.

Question 9.27

8. When might intuition and analysis lead to opposite conclusions?

Intuition begins with a belief, assumption, or general rule. It is quick and powerful—a gut reaction. Analysis is formal, logical, hypothetical thinking. It involves rational analysis of many factors. Intuition causes one to focus on the most interesting part of the problem, whereas analysis leads one to consider all aspects of the problem. Decisions based on intuition may lead one to take risks or jump to conclusions that analysis would help avoid.

Question 9.28

9. Why do most people prefer intuitive thinking, not analytic thought?

People tend to use intuition in their everyday lives because it is faster and easier to rely on preexisting beliefs, assumptions, and rules than it is to reason deliberately.

Question 9.29

10. What benefits come from adolescents’ use of technology?

Research conducted before the technology explosion found that with education, conversation, and experience, adolescents move past egocentric thought. Social networking may speed up this process, as teens communicate daily with dozens of friends via e-mail, texting, and cell phones.

Question 9.30

11. What problems do adults fear from teenage use of the Internet?

Parents worry that their teenagers will be victimized by online sexual predators, even though in reality this is quite rare. They may also fear more likely dangers—becoming distracted by or even addicted to chat rooms and message boards, video games, and Internet gambling.

Question 9.31

12. Why is cyberbullying particularly harmful during adolescence?

Adolescents, especially those between ages 11 and 14, are often impulsive and low on judgment and believe in the imaginary audience. Consequently, they are more vulnerable to depression and suicide if they are victims of cyberbullying.

Question 9.32

13. Why might sexting be a problem?

(1) Pictures might be forwarded without the person’s knowledge, and (2) those who send erotic self-images risk serious depression if the reaction is negative.

Question 9.33

14. How is the term digital native misleading?

The term “native” implies that adolescents are adept with technology, whereas many are actually naive—they believe Web sites have privacy settings that do not exist or trust sites that are biased or misleading.