15.2 Two Modes of Thinking

dual-process model The notion that two networks exist within the human brain, one for emotional and one for analytical processing of stimuli.

Advanced logic in adolescence is counterbalanced by the increasing power of intuitive thinking. A dual-process model of cognition has been formulated (Dustin & Steinberg, 2011) (see Visualizing Development, p. 435).

435

VISUALIZING DEVELOPMENT

Thinking in Adolescence

We are able to think both intuitively and analytically, but adolescents tend to rely more on intuitive thinking than do adults. As we age, we move toward more analytical processing.

CDC NATIONAL VITAL STATISTICS SYSTEM, 2010.
JUPITERIMAGES/THINKSTOCK

As people age, their thinking tends to move from intuitive processing to more analytical processing. Virtually all cognitive psychologists note these two alternative processes and describe a developmental progression toward more dispassionate logic with maturity. However, the terms used and the boundaries between the two vary. They are roughly analogous to Kahneman’s System 1 (which “operates automatically and quickly”) and System 2 (“the conscious, reasoning self”) (Kahneman, 2011, pp. 20–21), as well as to the traditional distinction between inductive and deductive reasoning, and to Piaget’s concrete operational versus formal operational thought. Although experts vary in their descriptions, and individuals vary in when and how they use these two processes, overall adolescents tend to favor intuitive rather than analytic thinking.

436

Intuition Versus Analysis

At least two modes characterize reasoning, which we refer to here as intuitive and analytical. Although they interact and can overlap, each is independent of the other (Kuhn, 2013). Although most cognitive psychologists recognize that there are two modes of thought, the terms they use vary, as do some of the specific examples. The terms include: intuitive/analytic, implicit/explicit, creative/factual, systems 1 and 2, contextualized/decontextualized, unconscious/conscious, gist/quantitative, emotional/intellectual, experiential/rational, hot/cold.

The thinking described by the first half of each pair is easier, preferred in everyday life. Sometimes, however, circumstances necessitate the second mode, when deeper thought is demanded. The discrepancy between the maturation of the limbic system and the prefrontal cortex reflects this duality. [Lifespan Link: Timing differences in maturation of various parts of the brain was discussed in Chapter 14.]

The Irrational Adolescent

Particularly in describing adolescent cognition, the terms often used to describe these two modes of thinking are intuitive/analytic:

Adolescents are quick thinkers; their reaction time is shorter than at any other time of life. That typically makes them “fast and furious” intuitive thinkers, unlike their teachers and parents, who prefer slower, more analytic thinking.

Of course, when the two modes of thinking conflict, people of all ages sometimes use one mode and sometimes the other: We are all “predictably irrational” at times (Ariely, 2009). Although older adults may prefer the more thoughtful responses, it is possible to overthink a decision, to become so entangled in possibilities that no action is taken. Adolescents are impatient with adult logic, but they also can overthink. They more often err in another direction, however, with quick illogical action.

Observation Quiz Above, Melissa seems to working by herself, but what sign do you see that suggests she is part of a team who built this robot?

Answer to Observation Quiz: The flag on the robot matches her T-shirt. Often teenagers wear matching shirts to signify their joint identity.

Impressive Connections This robot is about to compete in the Robotics Competition in Atlanta, Georgia, but much more impressive are the brains of the Oregon high school team (including Melissa, shown here) who designed the robot.
AP PHOTO/GREGORY SMITH

Experiences and role models influence choices, not only what action to take but also what intellectual process to use to decide what to do. Conversations, observations, and debate all move thinking forward, leading to conclusions that consider more of the facts (Kuhn, 2013).

Paul Klaczynski has conducted dozens of studies comparing the thinking of children, young adolescents, and older adolescents (usually 9-, 12-, and 15-year-olds) (Holland & Klaczynski, 2009; Klaczynski, 2001, 2011; Klaczynski et al., 2009). In one study he presented 19 logic problems to children and adolescents. For example:

Timothy is very good-looking, strong, and does not smoke. He likes hanging around with his male friends, watching sports on TV, and driving his Ford Mustang convertible. He’s very concerned with how he looks and with being in good shape. He is a high school senior now and is trying to get a college scholarship.

437

Based on this [description], rank each statement in terms of how likely it is to be true…. The most likely statement should get a 1. The least likely statement should get a 6.

________ Timothy has a girlfriend.

________ Timothy is an athlete.

________ Timothy is popular and an athlete.

________ Timothy is a teacher’s pet and has a girlfriend.

________ Timothy is a teacher’s pet.

________ Timothy is popular.

In ranking these statements, most adolescents (73 percent) made at least one analytic error, ranking a double statement (e.g., popular and an athlete) as more likely than either of the single statements included in it (popular or an athlete). They intuitively jumped to the more inclusive statement rather than sticking to logic. Klaczynski found that almost every adolescent was analytical and logical on some of the 19 problems but not on others, with some scoring high on the same questions that others scored low on. Logical thinking improved with age and education, although not with IQ.

In other words, being smarter as measured by an intelligence test did not advance logic as much as did having more experience, in school and in life. Klaczynski (2001) concluded that, even though teenagers can use logic, “most adolescents do not demonstrate a level of performance commensurate with their abilities” (p. 854).

Preferring Emotions

What would motivate adolescents to use—or fail to use—their formal operational thinking? Klaczynski’s participants had all learned the scientific method in school, and they knew that scientists use empirical evidence and deductive reasoning. But they did not always think like scientists. Why not?

Dozens of experiments and extensive theorizing have found some answers (Albert & Steinberg, 2011). Essentially, logic is more difficult than intuition, and it requires questioning ideas that are comforting and familiar. Once people of any age reach an emotional conclusion (sometimes called a “gut feeling”), they resist changing their minds. Prejudice does not quickly disappear, because it is not seen as prejudice.

As people gain experience in making decisions and thinking things through, they become better at knowing when analysis is needed (Milkman et al., 2009). For example, in contrast to younger students, when judging whether a rule is legitimate, older adolescents are more suspicious of authority and more likely to consider mitigating circumstances (Klaczynski, 2011). Both suspicion of authority and awareness of context signify advances in reasoning, but both also complicate simple issues—sometimes to the exasperation of their parents.

Rational judgment is especially difficult when egocentrism dominates. One psychologist discovered this personally when her teenage son phoned to be picked up from a party that had “gotten out of hand.” The boy heard

his frustrated father lament “drinking and trouble—haven’t you figured out the connection?” Despite the late hour and his shaky state, the teenager advanced a lengthy argument to the effect that his father had the causality all wrong and the trouble should be attributed to other covariates, among them bad luck.

[Kuhn & Franklin, 2006, p. 966]

Better Thinking

Sometimes adults conclude that more mature thought processes are wiser, since they lead to caution (as in the father’s connection between “drinking and trouble”). Adults are particularly critical when egocentrism leads an impulsive teenager to risk future addiction by experimenting with drugs, or to risk pregnancy and AIDS by not using a condom.

438

But adults may themselves be egocentric in making such judgments if they assume that adolescents share their values. Parents want healthy, long-lived children, so they think that adolescents use faulty reasoning when they risk their lives. Adolescents, however, value social warmth and friendship, and their hormones and brains are more attuned to those values than to long-term consequences (Crone & Dahl, 2012).

A 15-year-old who is offered a cigarette, for example, might rationally choose social acceptance and maybe romance over the distant risk of cancer. Think of a teenager who wants to be “cool” or “bad,” and then think of one who says “my mother wouldn’t approve.”

Weighing alternatives and thinking of possibilities can be paralyzing. The systematic, analytic thought that Piaget described is slow and costly, not fast and frugal, wasting precious time when a young person wants to act. Some risks are taken impulsively and foolishly, but others are premeditated, leading adolescents to make choices unlike those the parent would wish (Maslowsky et al., 2011).

As the knowledge base increases and the brain matures, as impulses become less insistent and past experiences accumulate, both modes of thought become more forceful. With maturity, education, and conversation with those who disagree, adolescents are neither paralyzed by too much analysis nor plummeted into danger via intuition. Logic increases from adolescence to adulthood (and then decreases somewhat in old age) (De Neys & Van Gelder, 2009; Kuhn, 2013).

Dual Processing and the Brain

The brain maturation process described in Chapter 14 seems to be directly related to the dual processes just explained (Steinberg, 2010). Because the limbic system is activated by puberty but the prefrontal cortex matures more gradually over time, it is easy to understand why adolescents are swayed by their intuition instead of by analysis.

Since adolescent brains respond quickly and deeply to social rejection, it is not surprising that teens might readily follow impulses that will bring social approval. Consider the results of experiments in which adults and adolescents, alone or with peers, played a video game in which taking risks might lead to crashes or gaining points. Compared to the adults, the adolescents were much more likely to take risks and crash, especially when they were with peers (Albert et al., 2013) (see Figure 15.3).

Losing Is Winning In this game, risk-taking led to more crashes and fewer points. As you see, adolescents were strongly influenced by the presence of peers, so much so that they lost points they would have kept if they had played alone. In fact, sometimes they laughed when they crashed, instead of bemoaning their loss. Note the contrast with emerging adults, who were more likely to take risks when alone.

This explains why motor vehicle accidents in adolescence result in more deaths per vehicle than crashes in adulthood—teenage drivers seek the admiration of their passengers by speeding, by passing trucks, by trying to beat the train at the railroad crossing. Chances are about 10 times greater that the driver in a fatal motor vehicle accident is under 20 than over the age of 25 (Winston et al., 2008). Don’t blame it only on inexperience; blame it on the need for acclaim. Some states now prohibit teen drivers from transporting other teenagers, a law that reduces deaths.

In the risk-taking video experiment, brain activity, (specifically in that part of the brain called the ventral striatum) showed an marked discrepancy between the adolescents and the adults: The adults’ brains gave more signals of caution (inhibition) when with other adults—the opposite reaction of adolescents’ brains when they were with peers.

Because experiments that include brain scans are expensive, they rarely include many participants. However, other research methods confirm these results. One longitudinal survey repeatedly queried over than 7,000 adolescents, beginning at age 12 and ending at age 24. The results were “consistent with neurobiological research indicating that cortical regions involved in impulse control and planning continue to mature through early adulthood [and that] subcortical regions that respond to emotional novelty and reward are more responsive in middle adolescence than in either children or adults” (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011, p. 743).

439

Specifically, this longitudinal survey traced sensation-seeking (e.g., “I enjoy new and exciting experiences”) from early adolescence to the mid-20s. Increases were notable from ages 12 to 14 (see Figure 15.4). Sensation-seeking leads to intuitive thinking, direct from the gut to the brain. The researchers also studied impulsivity, as indicated by agreement with statements such as “I often get in a jam because I do things without thinking.” A decline in impulsive action occurs as analytic thinking increases.

Look Before You Leap As you can see, adolescents become less impulsive as they mature, but they still enjoy the thrill of a new sensation.

A burst of sensation seeking at puberty and the slow decline of impulsivity over the years of adolescence were the general trends in this study. However, trajectories varied individually: The decline in sensation seeking did not correlate with the decline in impulsivity. Thus, biology (the HPA system) is not necessarily linked to experience (which affects decision making of the prefrontal cortex) (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011).

For example, hormone rushes in two adolescents might produce intense and identical drives for sex, but one teenager might have had experiences (direct or via role models) that taught him or her to curb that desire, while the other has had the opposite experiences. For the first, impulsivity would decline rapidly, as practice at saying no increases. This would not be true for the second adolescent, who would seek sexual pleasure as he or she had seen on a video, heard from a friend, or had experienced before. Thus both might experience equal sensation-seeking impulses, but how they act on those impulses would vary.

440

SUMMING UP

Current research recognizes that there are at least two modes of cognition, here called intuitive reasoning and analytical thought. Intuitive thinking is experiential, quick, and impulsive, unlike formal operational thought. Both forms develop during adolescence, although sometimes intuitive processes crowd out analytic ones, because emotions overwhelm logic, especially when adolescents are together. Each form of thinking is appropriate in some contexts. The capacity for logical, reflective thinking increases with neurological maturation, as the prefrontal cortex matures.