23.1 Prejudice and Predictions

ageism A prejudice whereby people are categorized and judged solely on the basis of their chronological age.

Prejudice about late adulthood is common among people of all ages, including young children and older adults. That is a reflection of ageism, the idea that age determines who you are. Stereotyping makes ageism “a social disease, much like racism and sexism … [causing] needless fear, waste, illness, and misery” (Palmore, 2005, p. 90).

668

Ageism can target people of any age, but it is not recognized as readily as racism or sexism. Consider curfew laws that require every teenager to be off the streets by 10 P.M.: that is ageist. Imagine the outcry if a curfew targeted all non-whites or all males. Why is ageism accepted, especially in regard to the old?

One expert contends that

there is no other group like the elderly about which we feel free to openly express stereotypes and even subtle hostility…. Most of us … believe that we aren’t really expressing negative stereotypes or prejudice, but merely expressing true statements about older people when we utter our stereotypes (Nelson, 2011, p. 40).

Nelson believes that ageism is institutionalized in our culture and has become pervasive in the media, employment, and retirement communities.

Another reason people accept ageism is that it often seems complimentary (“young lady”) or solicitous (Bugental & Hehman, 2007). However, the effects of ageism, whether benevolent or not, are insidious, seeping into and eroding the older person’s feelings of competence. The resulting self-doubt fosters anxiety, morbidity, and even mortality.

Believing the Stereotype

Parents protect their children from racism via racial socialization, teaching them to recognize and counter bias while encouraging them to be proud of who they are. However, when children express an ageist thought, few people teach them otherwise. Later on, their long-standing prejudice is “extremely resistant to change,” undercutting their own health and intellect (Golub & Langer, 2007, pp. 12–13).

For example, in one study, adults younger than age 50 expressed opinions about the elderly, such as agreeing or not that “old people are helpless.” Thirty years later, those who were least ageist were half as likely to have serious heart disease compared to those who were most ageist (Levy et al., 2009). Attitudes about aging vary from culture to culture, with deadly consequences.

Not in the West Korea (left) and Viet Nam (right) are more than two thousand miles apart, each with a distinct culture, but their people share appreciation of exercise and of the elderly. These women are among many doing Tai Chi each morning in Hanoi. Can you picture dozens of women exercising in a public plaza in a Western City?
© TIM CLAYTON/CORBIS
SEONGJOON CHO/LONELY PLANET IMAGES/GETTY IMAGES

Attitudes toward aging may be one reason longevity varies markedly depending on where a person lives. (The estimated life span for people born in various nations was shown in Figure 20.4). Japan has the highest life expectancy in the world—a newborn girl in 2013 is expected to live well into her eighties (United Nations, 2013)—perhaps because of cultural practices such as Respect for the Aged Day.

669

Many contend, however, that ageism is now prevalent worldwide, even in Japan (North & Fiske, 2012). Perhaps only nations with few old people can afford to venerate them.

Ageist Elders

Ageism becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy, a prediction that comes true because people believe it. For example, most people older than 70 believe they are doing better than other people their age—who, they say, have worse problems and are too self-absorbed (Cruikshank, 2009; Townsend et al., 2006). Underlying that belief is the idea that age destroys vitality.

If the elderly attribute their problems to the inevitability of age, they will not try to change themselves or the situation. For example, if they forget something, they laugh it off as a “senior moment,” not realizing the ageism of that reaction. When hearing an ageist phrase—“dirty old man” or “second childhood,” or patronizing compliments such as “spry” or “having all her marbles”—elders themselves miss the insult.

Asked how old they feel, typical 80-year-olds lop a decade or more off their age (Pew Research, 2009). Yet if most 80-year-olds feel like they imagine the average 70-year-old feels, then that feeling is, in fact, typical for 80-year-olds. Thus they reject their own ageist stereotype of 80-year-olds, although they feel the same way most 80-year-olds actually do.

It is illogical for all the 80-year-olds to feel like 70-year-olds, but in an ageist culture thinking you feel younger than your chronological age is protective. Indeed, “feeling youthful is more strongly predictive of health than any other factors including commonly noted ones like chronological age, gender, marital status and socioeconomic status” (Barrett, 2012, p. 3).

Stereotype threat can be as debilitating for the aged as for other groups (Hummert, 2011). [Lifespan Link: Stereotype threat was discussed in detail in Chapter 18.] If the elderly fear they are losing their minds, that fear itself may undermine cognitive competence (Hess et al., 2009). Internalized ageism is much worse than an ageist comment from someone else, as the following view from science illustrates.

A VIEW FROM SCIENCE

When You Think of Old People …

The effect of internalized ageism was apparent in a classic study (Levy & Langer, 1994). The researchers selected three groups. Two were thought to be less exposed to ageism: residents of China, where the old were traditionally venerated, and North Americans who were deaf lifelong. The third group was composed of North Americans with typical hearing, who presumably had heard ageist comments all their lives. In each of these three groups, half the participants were young and half were old.

Memory tests were given to everyone, six clusters in all. Elders in all three groups (Chinese, deaf Americans, and hearing Americans) scored lower than their younger counterparts. This was expected; age differences are common in laboratory tests of memory, as will be explained in detail in Chapter 24.

The purpose of this study, however, was not to replicate earlier research, but to see if ageism affected memory. It did. The gap in scores between younger and older hearing North Americans (most exposed to ageism) was double that between younger and older deaf North Americans and five times wider than the age gap in the Chinese (see Figure 23.1). Ageism undercut ability, a conclusion also found in many later studies (Levy, 2009).

The Widest Gap The bars show how the six groups score on a memory test, with the overall average right in the middle, at 0. Age slows down memory—no surprise—but culture has a marked effect. Not only do the Chinese score higher (their culture values memorizing), but the gap between old and young is greatest among the hearing Americans. Have decades of listening to ageist remarks within the U.S. culture reduced memory?

Sadly, later studies have found that modernization has made many Asian cultures more ageist than they used to be (Chen & Powell, 2012; Nelson, 2011). And numerous studies continue to find that an older person’s attitude about aging affects, for good or ill, their mental performance, social life, and even physical health.

670

For example, positive concepts of aging correlate with faster recovery from disability (Levy et al., 2012). This study began with 598 healthy people age 70 or older, followed for more than 10 years. Initially they were asked, “When you think of old persons, what are the first five words or phrases that come to mind?” Their answers were rated from most negative (e.g., decrepit, demented) to most positive (e.g., wise, spry). From this a score of attitudes was calculated for each person.

Over the years of the study, many of the participants experienced disabilities, including some so severe that they temporarily could not walk, feed themselves, go to the toilet, or even get out of bed. Some had major surgery or were hospitalized, sometimes requiring intensive care. Then most recovered, at least somewhat. Their resilience depended partly on other illnesses, on income, and on many other factors, as one might predict.

However, when all those factors were taken into account, how quickly and completely people recovered from disability was affected by their earlier attitudes about the old. The most dramatic difference was found in those people who experienced severe disabilities: Compared to those with the most negative stereotypes, those with positive associations about aging became completely independent again 44 percent more often (Levy et al., 2012).

When older people believe that they are independent and in control of their own lives, they are likely to be healthier—mentally, as well as physically—than others their age. Of course some need special care. Elders must find “a delicate balance … knowing when to persist and when to switch gears, … [and] some aspects of aging are out of one’s control” (Lachman et al., 2011, p. 186). If an older person is struggling to carry a heavy bag, help might be appreciated. But do not assume; ask first, don’t grab the bag.

Old Lady Few young people realize that many of the most active people they know are in their 60s and 70s. Tina Turner, born in 1939, had a sold-out international tour when she was officially a senior citizen, and she married her long-time companion when she was 73. Her talent is extraordinary, but her active life is not exceptional.
DPA PHOTOS/NEWSCOM

Sleep and Exercise

Ageism impairs the routines of daily life. It prevents depressed older people from seeking help because they resign themselves to infirmity.

Ageism also leads others to undermine an elder’s vitality and health. For instance, health professionals are less aggressive in treating disease in older patients, researchers testing new prescription drugs enroll few older adults (who are most likely to use those drugs), and caregivers diminish independence by helping the elderly too much (Cruikshank, 2009; Herrera, 2010; Peron & Ruby, 2011–2012).

671

One specific example is sleep. The day–night circadian rhythm diminishes with age: Many older people wake before dawn and are sleepy during the day. Older adults spend more time in bed, take longer to fall asleep, and wake frequently (about 10 times per night) (Ayalon & Ancoli-Israel, 2009). They also are more likely to nap. All this is normal: If they choose their own sleep schedules, elders feel less tired than young adults.

In one study, older adults complaining of sleep problems were mailed six booklets (one each week) (Morgan et al., 2012). The booklets described normal sleep patterns for people their age and gave suggestions to relieve insomnia. Compared to similar older people who did not get the booklets, the informed elders used less sleep medication and reported better quality sleep. Even 6 months after the last booklet, they were more satisfied with their sleep.

The booklets told elders what to do about their sleep. For uninformed elders, ageism can lead to distress over normal sleep; doctors might prescribe narcotics, or people might drink alcohol. These remedies can overwhelm an aging body, causing heavy sleep, confusion, nausea, depression, and unsteadiness.

A similar downward spiral is apparent with regard to exercise. In the United States, only 35 percent of those over age 64 meet the recommended guidelines for aerobic exercise, compared with 56 percent for adults aged 18 to 44 (National Center for Health Statistics, 2013) (see Figure 23.2). Meeting the guidelines for muscle strengthening was worse; only 16 percent of the aged did so.

Heart, Lungs, and Legs As you see, most of the elderly do not meet the minimum exercise standards recommended by the Centers for Disease Control—150 minutes of aerobic exercise a week and muscle-strengthening exercises twice a week. This is especially troubling since those activities have been proven many times to safeguard the health of all the major organs, as well as to correlate with intelligence, memory, and joy.

An ageist culture assumes that the patterns of the young are ideal. Consequently, team sports are organized in ways that emerging adults prefer; traditional dancing assumes a balanced sex ratio; many yoga, aerobic, and other classes are paced and designed for people in their 20s. If an old man tries to join a pick-up basketball game, the reaction of the young players might be rejection or sympathy. If an old woman dons running shorts and jogs around the park, others might snicker. No wonder some elders exercise less.

Added to the problems caused by the ageism of the culture is self-imposed ageism, which reduces movement. That increases stiffness and reduces range of motion while impairing circulation, digestion, and thinking. Balance is decreased, necessitating a slower gait, a cane, or a walker. A better reaction to aging would be to increase activity, and that would add healthy years, reduce depression, prevent heart disease, and more (Mullen et al., 2012; de Fina et al., 2013).

672

Especially for Young Adults Should you always speak louder and slower when talking to a senior citizen? (see response, page 674]

Response for Young Adults: No. Some seniors hear well, and they would resent it.

Many people think they are compassionate when they infantilize the elderly, regarding them as if they were children (“so cute,” “second childhood”) (Albert & Freedman, 2010). Some of the elderly accept the stereotype. It is easier not to protest: At every age and for every preconceived idea, changing someone else’s attitudes and assumptions is difficult.

Among the professionals most likely to harbor stereotypes are nurses, doctors, and other care workers. Their ageism is difficult to erase (Eymard & Douglas, 2012), partly because it is based on their experience in treating older patients who are sick and dependent. Understandably, some professionals generalize based on their experiences and treat all elderly as if they were feeble (Williams et al., 2009).

elderspeak A condescending way of speaking to older adults that resembles baby talk, with simple and short sentences, exaggerated emphasis, repetition, and a slower rate and a higher pitch than used in normal speech.

A specific example is elderspeak—the way people talk to the old (Nelson, 2011). Like baby talk, elderspeak uses simple and short sentences, slower talk, higher pitch, louder volume, and frequent repetition. Elderspeak is especially patronizing when people call an older person “honey” or “dear,” or use a nickname instead of a surname (“Billy,” not “Mr. White”).

Ironically, elderspeak reduces communication. Higher frequencies are harder for the elderly to hear, stretching out words makes comprehension worse, shouting causes anxiety, and simplified vocabulary reduces the precision of language.

Destructive Protection

Some younger adults and the media discourage the elderly from leaving home. For example, whenever an older person is robbed, raped, or assaulted, sensational headlines add to fear and consequently promote ageism. In fact, street crime targets young adults, not old ones (see Figure 23.3).

Victims of Crime As people grow older, they are less likely to be crime victims. These figures come from personal interviews in which respondents were asked whether they had been the victim of a violent crime—assault, sexual assault, rape, or robbery—in the past several months. This approach yields more accurate results than official crime statistics because many crimes are never reported to the police.

The homicide rate (the most reliable indicator of violent crime, since reluctance to report is not an issue) of those over age 65 is less than one-fifth the rate for those aged 25 to 34 (see Figure 17.2) (U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics, 2011). To protect our relatives, perhaps we should insist that our young adults never leave the house alone—a ridiculous suggestion that makes it obvious why telling older adults to stay home is shortsighted.

673

Although advertisements induce younger adults to buy medic-alert devices for older relatives, it might be better to go biking with them. Lest you think that bikes are only for children, a study of five nations (Germany, Italy, Finland, Hungary, and the Netherlands) found that 15 percent of Europeans older than 75 ride their bicycles every day (Tacken & van Lamoen, 2005).

In the United States, few elders ride bikes. Protected bike paths are scarce and many bikes are designed for speed, not stability. Laws requiring bike helmets often apply only to children—another example of ageism.

The Demographic Shift

demographic shift A shift in the proportions of the populations of various ages.

Demography is the science that describes populations, including population by cohort, age, gender, or region. Demographers describe “the greatest demographic upheaval in human history” (Bloom, 2011, p. 562), a demographic shift in the proportions of the population of various ages. In an earlier era, there were 20 times more children than older people, and only 50 years ago the world had 7 times more people under age 15 than over age 64. This is no longer true.

The World’s Aging Population

The United Nations estimates that nearly 8 percent of the world’s population in 2010 was 65 or older, compared with only 2 percent a century earlier. This number is expected to double by the year 2050. Already 13 percent in the United States are that old, as are 14 percent in Canada and Australia, 20 percent in Italy, and 23 percent in Japan (United Nations, 2012).

Speed Demon? Road rage? No. Neither his bike nor his garb is designed for speed, and anger is far more common in the young than in those over age 75, as this man is. He seems faster and happier than the drivers on his right, stuck in traffic in central London.
ASHLEY COOPER/CORBIS

As you saw in Chapter 1, demographers often depict the age structure of a population as a series of stacked bars, one bar for each age group, with the bar for the youngest at the bottom and the bar for the oldest at the top (refer to Visualizing Development, p. 680). Historically, the shape was a demographic pyramid. Like a wedding cake, it was widest at the base, and each higher level was narrower than the one beneath it, for three reasons—none of which is currently true:

  1. More children were born than the replacement rate of one per adult, so each new generation had more people than the previous one. NOW FALSE
  2. Many babies died, which made the bottom bar much wider than later ones. NOW FALSE
  3. Serious illness was almost always fatal, reducing the size of older groups. NOW FALSE

Sometimes unusual events caused a deviation from this wedding-cake pattern. For example, the Great Depression and World War II reduced the number of births. Then postwar prosperity and the soldiers’ return caused a baby boom between 1946 and 1964, just when infant survival increased. The mushrooming birth and survival rates led many demographers to predict a population explosion, with mass starvation by the year 2000 (Ehrlich, 1968).

That fear evaporated as new data emerged. Birth rates fell and a “green revolution” doubled the food supply. Early death has become rare; demographic stacks have become rectangles, not pyramids. Indeed, some people worry about another demographic shift: fewer babies and more elders, which will greatly affect world health and politics (Albert & Freedman, 2010).

This flipped demographic pattern is not yet starkly evident everywhere. Most nations still have more people under age 15 than over age 64. Worldwide, children outnumber elders more than 3 to 1—but not 20 to 1 as they once did. United Nations predictions for 2015 are for 1,904,088,000 people younger than 15 and 603,986,000 older than 64. Not until 2075 is the ratio projected to be 1 to 1 (United Nations, 2013).

674

Same Situation, Far Apart: Keep Smiling Good humor seems to be a cause of longevity, and vice versa. This is true for both sexes, including the British men on Founder’s Day (left) and the two Indian women on an ordinary sunny day in Dwarka (right).
TIM GRAHAM/GETTY IMAGES
MITCHELL KANASHKEVICH/CORBIS

Statistics that Frighten

Unfortunately, demographic data are sometimes reported in ways designed to alarm. For instance, have you heard that people aged 80 and older are the fastest-growing age group? Or that the number of people with Alzheimer disease is increasing rapidly? Both true; both misleading.

In 2010 in the United States there were more than 4 times as many people 80 and older than there were 50 years earlier (11.8 million compared with 2.7 million). The risk of Alzheimer disease increases dramatically with age. Stating the facts that way triggers ageist fears of a nation burdened by hungry hordes of frail and confused elders, requiring billions of dollars of health care in crowded hospitals and nursing homes.

But stop and think. The population has also grown. The percent of U.S. residents age 80 and older has doubled, not quadrupled (increasing between 1960 and 2010 from 1.6 percent to 3.8 percent). That does not overwhelm the other 96.2 percent. What percent of them are in nursing homes or hospitals? (Guess—the answer will be presented soon.) Since Alzheimer rates increase with age, the absolute numbers are rising—but not the proportions at each age. Indeed, the rate may be decreasing, as Chapter 24 explains.

dependency ratio A calculation of the number of self-sufficient productive adults compared with the number of dependents (children and the elderly) in a given population.

Statisticians sometimes report the dependency ratio, estimating the proportion of the population that depends on care from others. This ratio is calculated by comparing the number of dependents (defined as those under age 15 or over 64) to the number of people in the middle, aged 15 to 64.

In 2010, the nation with the highest dependency ratio was Uganda, 1:0.98, with more dependents than adults; the lowest was Bahrain, 1:3.5, with one dependent per three and a half adults. In most nations, including the United States, the dependency ratio is about 1:2, that is, one person under 15 or over 64 for two people in between (United Nations, 2012).

But this way of calculating the dependency ratio assumes that older adults are dependent. This mistake is echoed in those dire predictions of what will happen when baby boomers age: supposedly a shrinking number of working adults will carry a crushing burden of senility and fragility. Social Security, Medicare, and public hospitals will be bankrupt, according to some. That specter is alarmist, ageist, and untrue.

The truth is that most elders are fiercely independent. They are more often caregivers, not care receivers, caring not only for each other but also for the young, those other “dependents.”

675

Only 10 percent of those over age 64 depend on other people for basic care. Those who do need help are more likely to get it from close relatives than tax-paying strangers. In the United States in 2012, only 3 percent of people over 64 were currently in nursing homes or hospitals (that is down from 5 percent in 1990). Less than one-half of 1 percent of the total population are in such medical institutions (Centers for Medicaid and Medicare Services, 2013).

As more and more people reach old age, the proportion (not the number) of elders who are in nursing homes and hospitals is decreasing, not increasing. Most older adults live completely independently, alone or with an aging spouse; a minority are living with adult children, and even fewer are in institutions.

The stereotype is not completely false. It is true that dependency increases after age 80. But it also is true that many very old people remain independent. Compared to most other nations, more people are in long-term hospitals and nursing homes in the United States, yet less than 10 percent of U.S. residents over age 80 are in such facilities at any given time. (Is that what you guessed?)

Some are admitted to a hospital for a short-term stay, but not for long. For those age 85 and older, each year 22 percent are admitted to the hospital, with an average stay of 6 days (National Center for Health Statistics, 2012). Most then go home, some go to a rehab facility for a few weeks, and very few go to a nursing home for years of care.

Young, Old, And Oldest

If you guessed that far more than 10 percent of the very old were in nursing homes, do not feel foolish. Almost everyone overestimates because people tend to notice only the feeble, not recognizing the rest. This is a characteristic of human thought—the memorable case is thought to be typical.

Other developmental examples of the distortion of the memorable case is thinking of teenagers as delinquents, of single mothers as neglectful, of divorced fathers as deadbeats. When you hear such stereotypes, you probably reject them as ignorant. Hopefully you now recognize that the same distorting tendency feeds ageism.

Gerontologists distinguish among the young-old, the old-old, and the oldest-old.

Ages are sometimes used to demarcate these three groups. Many of the young-old are aged 65 to 75, old-old 75 to 85, and oldest-old over 85. However, age itself does not indicate dependency. A young-old person could be 70 or 90; an old-old person can be 65 or 100. Actually, an elder of any age could be any of these. For well-being and independence, which are characteristic of the young-old, attitude is more important than age (O’Rourke et al., 2010a).

The reality that most people over age 64 are quite capable of caring for themselves does not mean that they are unaffected by time. The processes of senescence, described in Chapter 20, continue lifelong. Faces wrinkle, bodies shrink, hearing fades. As you remember, such age changes vary a great deal from person to person; good health habits slow down senescence. But aging is ongoing; no 80-year-old has the physical stamina, appearance, or strength of the average 20-year-old, or of him or herself even 10 years earlier.

676

SUMMING UP

Ageism is stereotyping based on age, a prejudice that leads to less competent and less confident elders. Ageism is evident in people of all walks of life and all ages, but it is particularly harmful when it is present in health professionals and in the elderly themselves. Elderspeak is one example.

Demographic changes have resulted in a higher proportion of elders and a lower proportion of children in every nation. In theory, as nations have more elderly people and fewer children, the dependency ratio should stay the same, but this is inaccurate. Basing the dependency ratio on everyone under age 15 and over age 65 is a distortion. Most elders are quite independent and few need full-time care. A distinction needs to be made between the young-old, who are the self-sufficient majority, the old-old, who show some signs of failing, and the oldest-old, who need help in daily life.