Chapter 1. Attentional Filtering

1.1 Introduction

Cognitive Tool Kit
true
true
true

Attentional Filtering

How good are you at ignoring what going on around you? In this task you are asked to follow information presented in one ear (referred to as the attending ear). Your task is to not pay attention to what is going on in your other ear. Try this task to see how you respond to distraction.

1.2 Experiment Setup

1.3 Instructions

Instructions

You will need to press the space bar to begin the experiment. You will then be presented with 2 three-word phrases simultaneously, one in each ear. You are to attend to what is presented in the ear you chose during set-up to be your attending ear. At the end of the trial, a selection of words will be presented as buttons. Please select, in the order they were presented, the words you heard in the attending ear.

1.4 Experiment

Begin Experiment

1.5 Results

Results

1.6 Debriefing

Debriefing

It is impossible for us to process all the sources of information that come our way. We have to respond to some events while ignoring others. By and large, and with a remarkable record of success, we are able to select the stimuli that need our attention. This ability is called selective attention. We are not perfect at it, but we do well enough that we do not usually have to actively think about how we accomplish this function. Researchers often study the ways we segment the world so that we can gather the information we need and ignore the information we don’t. One of the first and still most common ways to understand how we select information is based on the channel or the source of the information. In visual terms, the computer window you are reading at this moment is a source or channel of information. If you are paying attention to this window, what is going on within it is kept together and selected. For the most part, you can ignore what is going on beyond the window, including any other areas of the computer screen.

In this experiment, we controlled the channel of information by directing one set of words to one ear and another set of words to the other ear. Your job was to pay attention to the information coming from just one ear. Such simple physical cues are important features by which we can choose to pay attention to one stimulus and ignore others Broadbent (1956). Other simple physical differences have been found useful in helping us select the source of information. If two people are speaking, one with a high-pitched voice and the other with a low-pitched voice, the pitch of the voice can help you distinguish the two people (Triesman, 1964).

Grey and Wedderburn (1960), the experiment being replicated here, asked if something other than mere physical differences influence how we select the information we attend to. They examined whether the meaning of the stimulus can play a role in how we pay attention. Let us look at a sample set of stimuli carefully:

Left Ear Right ear
Mice 3
5 eat
Cheese 4

In this example, the participant was asked to listen to the left ear. The participant is to respond: Mice 5 cheese. What Gray and Wedderburn found was that their participants like to group their results by meaning. Several of their participants would report Mice eat cheese. Thus meaning played a role in how we group information for selection and can overrule the need to group by physical location. Such results have played an important role in how we think about how attention selects stimuli to process, suggesting that mere physical aspects of a stimulus, while important, is not the defining feature of attention.

References:

Broadbent, D. E. (1956). Successive responses to simultaneous stimuli. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 8, 145-152.

Gray, J. A. & Wedderburn, A. A. I. (1960). Grouping strategies with simultaneous stimuli. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 180-184.

Triesman, A. (1964). The effect of irrelevant material on the efficiency of selective listening. American Journal of Psychology, 77, 206-214.

1.7 Quiz

Quiz

Question 1.1

T0IM0hR7ZKTrI7DmkmE9RZZA6/E4gkBJW5OoLK4POYIN0bp3bTQkZ7RoeTSPQD1v/Qcee1PN51ypwUYRIOZXYJNzKXppksVEDPrL6YrESZI2F86CSv7EALdPLhAtv6ij0Bv6vsPOb4YX0uKJN/jwkqlCELwQIwQ/pOCIF5Q2mbg+sY40TE59gaSJVLTJOIv5Mhw0sJUGxDotZXtBfvN4IPmiGjQFmyAmwzC6Bv7iUOjm7HwYIG7JhRGKU9n7Fzu+rel14JIRPiS93lRETonSdOALDwpSbRfkVGulA+kR3Kb6YT6Ssg0IQw==
1
Correct.
Incorrect.
The independent variable is the value that is changed by the experimenter. In this case, the major variable manipulated was whether the phrase used meaningful or random words, so the correct answer is meaningful phrases versus random words.

Question 1.2

9vzuJ+1HLKSZfe1dCIT0UaE45iH/nyaZyAgfzPyQCcj0wrD7j9hBzcLHDv+cKnG+lvb/t1bbJxlSbCfkvW49hoPgmJ6Lc4dEexM3wk++eZK6pQDUYf0Tm4CIL7CB22PBhysyCjY6vwhG3krBXn+nqYuW1sflyYePcbFSoi1MEGppSbym08KmwA6FzA5Vlo5v1gMjw25GgJ7xdepB8IxOrZDZxLrcAJZ8ZcMfI1BQz1EXL78bUU/j1priv2qHPLg1vhfGT1qSwpIWNMEKa57ZsYaWMeMsgax3bYP53b1SLjx9ieiCQ0Kujp8+Z86eZ0u/
1
Correct.
Incorrect.
The dependent variable is the value that the experimenter collects to indicate how you performed in the experiment. In this case, the most important variable we measured was percent of the second word reported from the unattended ear.

Question 1.3

bAdvkiPtcohFi69IK5KEv702MeEsqVq4uvrG/dkwHBKTR/IN+1fWQiiqzA+I1zSPPoBLTk3iK22/hUX094OU2CzcYYnPT6LleCRDlfBt9ejlLJH8S0NQueWbdEMoDi95haEoz80bX+/6rcYvVv11BA==
1
Correct.
Incorrect.
The main issue being examined is how we use attention to select and process some information from the environment and not others.

Question 1.4

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
Correct.
Incorrect.
What was interesting about the results of this experiment was the finding that if messages alternate from the attended ear, we still tend to hear the meaningful message.

Question 1.5

pu5GHj3RhZIjv2dyw2zX2g5ERgA9ljUFnx0BdLQUhDrTwjtdHtcC75uZ9hhU6k61Y6AZt/AfB35t0Of2lyGIxWbDrJQlnZExogD3r7i+tkrSWbZ3tUtnYKguK43egf0d0t2nA63eHFgp+hfi5eR5M7noH6H9BfoGIjXA3TRl/mdatc0RFa7ECHRkoxq6f4fsKZfVu68iShyQqVpPbrFRWd6L6giDozKpm+D5CKIlaLVkvwXI3Jb3uZCfSzzlnuzuIYE5z6qW+of2nHh427+3jQ==
Correct.
Incorrect.
The correct answer is attention follows meaning. This result shows us that our cognitive processes use many features of a stimulus—including simple sensory parameters such as meaning and attended ear—to select what information to process.