The Social Psychology of Attitudes

KEY THEME

An attitude is a learned tendency to evaluate objects, people, or issues in a particular way.

KEY QUESTIONS

Should high school graduation requirements include a class on basic sex education, birth control methods, and safe sex? Should there be a compulsory military or community service requirement for all young adults? Should the government be doing more to prepare for climate change?

On these and many other subjects, you’ve probably formed an attitude. Psychologists formally define an attitude as a learned tendency to evaluate some object, person, or issue in a particular way (Banaji & Heiphetz, 2010; Bohner & Dickel, 2011). Attitudes are typically positive or negative, but they can also be ambivalent, as when you have mixed feelings about an issue, person, or group (Costarelli, 2011).

As shown in Figure 11.1 on the next page, attitudes can include three components. First, an attitude may have a cognitive component: your thoughts about a given topic or object. For example, Emil is an art lover. He often tells his friends that, in his opinion, visiting galleries and museums encourages people to be more open to new experiences. Second, an attitude may have an emotional or affective component, as when Emil talks excitedly about how energized he is after seeing an exhibition of Jeff Koons’ art. Finally, an attitude may have a behavioral component, in which attitudes are reflected in action, as when Emil donates to an organization that introduces schoolchildren to the arts.

image
FIGURE 11.1 The Components of Attitudes An attitude is a positive or negative evaluation of an object, person, or idea. An attitude may have cognitive, emotional, and behavioral components.

462

Along with forming attitudes toward objects, ideas, or political campaigns, we also form attitudes about people. The In Focus box “Interpersonal Attraction and Liking” discusses some of the factors that affect the thoughts and feelings that we develop about other people.

The Effect of Attitudes on Behavior

image
Attitudes and Behavior These student protesters in Gauhati, India, are calling for an end to all large dam projects in their country. The students believe that large dams have a negative environmental impact, particularly on parts of the country downriver from the dams. People who hold strong opinions and express them openly, like these protestors, are most likely to behave in ways that are consistent with their attitudes.
AP Photo/Anupam Nath

Intuitively, you probably assume that your attitudes tend to guide your behavior. But social psychologists have consistently found that people don’t always act in accordance with their attitudes. For example, you might disapprove of cheating yet find yourself peeking at a classmate’s exam paper when the opportunity presents itself.

When are your attitudes likely to influence or determine your behavior? Social psychologists have found that you’re most likely to behave in accordance with your attitudes when:

Clearly, your attitudes do influence your behavior in many instances. Now, consider the opposite question: Can your behavior influence your attitudes?

The Effect of Behavior on Attitudes

FRIED GRASSHOPPERS FOR LUNCH?!

Suppose you have volunteered to participate in a psychology experiment. At the lab, a friendly experimenter asks you to indicate your degree of preference for a variety of foods, including fried grasshoppers, which you rank pretty low on the list. During the experiment, the experimenter instructs you to eat some fried grasshoppers. You manage to swallow three of the crispy critters. At the end of the experiment, your attitudes toward grasshoppers as a food source are surveyed again.

463

IN FOCUS

Interpersonal Attraction and Liking

In psychology, attraction refers to feeling drawn to other people—having positive thoughts and feelings about them. Often, attraction motivates us to interact with or develop a relationship with the attractive person.

What makes one person more attractive than another? Personal characteristics such as warmth and trustworthiness, adventurousness, and social status influence judgments of attractiveness (Finkel & Baumeister, 2010; Sprecher & Felmlee, 2008). But physical appearance, especially facial features, is probably the most significant factor in attraction. Studies reveal that wide smiles, high eyebrows, dilated pupils, and full lips are judged as attractive by both men and women, and that these preferences are consistent across many cultures (Chatterjee, 2011; Perrett, 2010). Some evolutionary psychologists argue that we associate attractive facial features with health, a desirable characteristic in a mating partner (Fink & Penton-Voak, 2002). However, the sexes do differ in some respects. For example, in online dating, women tend to prefer men who are taller than average, but men tend to prefer women who are of short or average height (Hitsch & others, 2010).

image
Ideals of Beauty Around the World Large eyes, a wide smile, and full lips are attractive in cultures around the world. The beautiful smiles of a Hmong woman from Thailand, a young Hispanic American man, a woman from Chile, and a man from the Omo Delta in East Africa would be considered attractive by any cultural standard.
John Warburton-Lee/DanitaDelimont.com

Some aspects of attraction are interpersonal. For example, we are more attracted to people whom we perceive as being like us—in physical characteristics, personality traits, attitudes, and even psychological health (Finkel & Baumeister, 2010). Although similarity is a powerful predictor of attraction in most Western cultures, cross-cultural research has shown that it is less important in some Eastern cultures, such as Japan (Heine & others, 2009).

Familiarity is another predictor of attraction and liking. In general, the more we interact with a person, the more we tend to like that person. Why? One explanation is that most interactions with other people are relatively pleasant (Reis & others, 2011). So, unless a person is particularly obnoxious or unpleasant, frequent interactions lead to more feelings of mutual pleasure, understanding, and acceptance.

The situations in which we interact with people also affect attraction. When happy, intoxicated, or physically aroused by exercise or exertion, we are more likely to rate others as attractive (Finkel & Baumeister, 2010). And, if we anticipate that attractive people are likely to be attracted to or like us, we’re more likely to be attracted to them, to like them, and to behave warmly toward them (Stinson & others, 2009).

Finally, feelings of attraction can be influenced by the socioeconomic and cultural environment. For example, cross-cultural research has shown that men in societies in which food and resources are in short supply tend to prefer heavier women (Swami & Tovée, 2006; Tovée & others, 2006). Conversely, a preference for thinner women is more common in societies where resources are abundant (Swami & others, 2010).

But is it culture or hunger that shapes a preference for heavier women? In a clever study, Leif Nelson and Evan Morrison (2005) compared the preferences of college students as they were entering and leaving a campus dining hall at dinnertime. The presumably hungry men entering the dining hall preferred heavier women than the satiated men exiting the dining hall. (Having an empty or full stomach didn’t affect the female college students’ ratings of an ideal body shape.) The moral of the story: Culture affects body shape preference, but may do so through processes that are not culture-specific but rather situational.

Researchers have also observed a preference for specific body proportions that is consistent across cultures (Singh & others, 2010). Whether heavy or thin, a woman with a waist that is a good deal smaller than her hips seems to be universally viewed as attractive. This is true even among men who are blind; without ever having seen images of women considered beautiful, they preferred this proportion when they felt female mannequins (Karremans & others, 2010). Why? This body proportion has been shown to predict both lower risk for a range of diseases, including diabetes and cancer, and increased reproductive success (Singh & Singh, 2011). Evolutionary researchers suggest that this preference makes perfect sense, as a healthy reproductive partner increases the chance of genes being passed on. (The research on mate preferences is discussed further on page 378 in Chapter 9.)

464

Later in the day, you talk to a friend who also participated in the experiment. You mention how friendly and polite you thought the experimenter was. But your friend had a different experience. He thought the experimenter was an arrogant, rude jerk.

image
Fried Grasshoppers: Tasty or Disgusting? Most Americans do not rate fried grasshoppers as one of their favorite foods. Suppose you agreed to eat a handful of grasshoppers after being asked to do so by a rude, unfriendly experimenter. Do you think your attitude toward fried grasshoppers would improve more than that of a person who ate grasshoppers after being asked to do so by a friendly, polite experimenter? Why or why not?
p.studio66/Shutterstock

Here’s the critical question: Whose attitude toward eating fried grasshoppers is more likely to change in a positive direction? Given that you interacted with a friendly experimenter, most people assume that your feelings about fried grasshoppers are more likely to have improved than your friend’s attitude. In fact, it is your friend—who encountered the obnoxious experimenter—who is much more likely to hold a more positive attitude toward eating fried grasshoppers than you.

At first glance, this finding seems to go against the grain of common sense. So how can we explain this outcome? The fried grasshoppers story represents the basic design of a classic experiment by social psychologist Philip Zimbardo and his colleagues (1965). Zimbardo’s experiment underscored the importance of cognitive dissonance, a phenomenon first identified by social psychologists Leon Festinger and J. Merrill Carlsmith (1959). Cognitive dissonance is an unpleasant state of psychological tension (dissonance) that occurs when there’s an inconsistency between two thoughts or perceptions (cognitions). This state of dissonance is so unpleasant that we are strongly motivated to reduce it (Festinger, 1957, 1962; Gawronski, 2012).

Cognitive dissonance commonly occurs in situations in which you become uncomfortably aware that your behavior and your attitudes are in conflict (Cooper, 2012). In these situations, you are simultaneously holding two conflicting cognitions: your original attitude versus the realization that your behavior contradicts that attitude. If you can easily rationalize your behavior to make it consistent with your attitude, then any dissonance you might experience can be quickly and easily resolved. But when your behavior cannot be easily justified, how can you resolve the contradiction and eliminate the unpleasant state of dissonance? Since you can’t go back and change the behavior, you change your attitude to make it consistent with your behavior.

image
Social Psychologist Philip Zimbardo (b. 1933) Zimbardo grew up in an immigrant family in a poor neighborhood in the South Bronx, an experience that sensitized him to the power of situational influences and the destructive nature of stereotypes and prejudice (Zimbardo, 2005, 2007).

Zimbardo’s research has ranged from attitude change to shyness, prison reform, and the psychology of evil. As Zimbardo (2000b) observes, “The joy of being a psychologist is that almost everything in life is psychology, or should be, or could be.” Later in the chapter, we’ll encounter Zimbardo’s most famous—and controversial—research: a study known as the “Stanford Prison Experiment.”

Karen Ballard/Redux Picturee

Let’s take another look at the results of the grasshopper study, this time from the perspective of cognitive dissonance theory. Your attitude toward eating grasshoppers did not change. Why? Because you could easily rationalize the conflict between your attitude (“Eating grasshoppers is disgusting”) and your behavior (eating three grass-hoppers). You probably justified your behavior by saying something like, “I ate the grasshoppers because I wanted to help out the nice experimenter.” However, your friend, who encountered the rude experimenter, can’t use that rationalization. Thus, he experiences an uncomfortable state of cognitive dissonance. Since he can’t go back and change his behavior, he is left with the only part of the equation that can be changed—his attitude (see Figure 11.2). “You know, eating those grasshoppers wasn’t that bad,” your friend comments. “In fact, they were kind of crunchy.” Notice how his change in attitude reduces the dissonance between his previous attitude and his behavior. And he might not have even realized that his attitude had changed.

image
FIGURE 11.2 How Cognitive Dissonance Leads to Attitude Change When your behavior conflicts with your attitudes, an uncomfortable state of tension is produced. However, if you can rationalize or explain your behavior, the conflict (and the tension) is eliminated or avoided. If you can’t explain your behavior, you may change your attitude so that it is in harmony with your behavior.

Research from social neuroscience suggests that cognitive dissonance can lead to attitude change quickly, perhaps without us even realizing that the process is occurring. Difficult decisions are often followed by an attitude change that favors the object or outcome chosen. Brain scans show changes in the parts of the brain associated with distress, arousal, emotion, and conflict within seconds after a person makes a difficult decision, which may indicate the discomfort produced by the change in attitude (Jarcho & others, 2010; van Veen & others, 2009).

This response to cognitive dissonance does not seem to be unique to adults or even humans. Louisa Egan and her colleagues (2007) have seen dissonance among four-year-old children who are forced to choose between two stickers—a dolphin sticker and a dragonfly sticker, for example—that they previously liked equally. They also observed the same effect among capuchin monkeys who were forced to choose between two colors of M&Ms that they previously liked equally. After choosing one, the monkeys preferred the chosen M&M color from then on. In both cases, it was not just that they were familiar with the chosen sticker or chosen M&M. Even when the researchers made the choice, the children and chimps preferred the chosen stickers and M&M color over the other options.

465

Attitude change due to cognitive dissonance is quite common in everyday life. For example, consider the person who impulsively buys a new leather coat that she really can’t afford. “It was too good a bargain to pass up,” she rationalizes. Similarly, researchers have found that people who quit smoking offered fewer rationalizations for smoking. But if they started smoking again, they started rationalizing again. For example, some said, “You’ve got to die of something, so why not enjoy yourself and smoke” (Fotuhi & others, 2013).

Cognitive dissonance also influences how we frame decisions we have made when we have chosen between two alternatives, such as which college to attend. After you make a choice, you emphasize the negative features of the choice you’ve rejected, which is commonly called a “sour grapes” rationalization. You also emphasize the positive features of the choice to which you have committed yourself—a “sweet lemons” rationalization.