7.4 Minority Influence

Social psychologists have long considered how the numerical majority could influence people. But in the mid- to late 1960s, amid the discontent and protests surrounding the Vietnam War, and with the civil rights and other movements escalating both in the United States and abroad, open and celebrated dissent began to take root. Perhaps not surprisingly, this period also saw the dawn of research into how the voice of one could influence the views of the many. Leading this charge was Serge Moscovici (Moscovici et al., 1969; Moscovici, 1980). He pioneered work on minority influence—the process by which dissenters (or numerical minorities) produce attitude change within a group, despite the extraordinary risk of social rejection and disturbance of the status quo. It’s vital to understand these processes, for without this knowledge we would have little understanding of how social change occurs.

Minority influence

The process by which dissenters (or numerical minorities) produce attitude change within a group, despite the extraordinary risk of social rejection and disturbance of the status quo.

Ralph Waldo Emerson famously wrote, “All history is a record of the power of minorities, and of minorities of one.” Indeed, single individuals and small movements in philosophy, science, the arts, religion, and politics often have profoundly altered the course of history: Homer, Plato, Confucius, Moses, Jesus, Muhammad, Nicolaus Copernicus, Galileo, Karl Marx, Charles Darwin, Sojourner Truth, Thomas Edison, Susan B. Anthony, Mahatma Gandhi, Albert Einstein, Adolf Hitler, Rosa Parks, Mother Teresa, Martin Luther King Jr., César Chávez—the list could go on and on. Thus, the study of minority influence is a window into the agents of cultural and social changes through scientific or artistic achievements, technological advances, political or social movements, horrific wars, advances in racial or gender equality, or other historical trends. Toward the end of this chapter, we’ll consider how individuals can influence others by holding a position of authority or rising to a leadership position. But for now, we’re going to focus on how individual dissenters can influence the majority even without the advantage of being in a position of authority.

How Minorities Exert Their Influence

Picketing Against Everything with Nothing Video on LaunchPad

In Asch’s classic studies on conformity, people conformed to the normative pressure of the majority, even when the majority’s judgments did not appear to be correct. In their initial study of minority influence, Moscovici and colleagues (1969) developed their own perceptual judgment paradigm but with some interesting twists. Instead of confronting participants with majority pressure, Moscovici presented them with a minority view and examined how that view influenced their perceptions. In what they were told was a study of color perception, groups of six participants were asked to view slides, all varying shades of blue, and name the color in each. In the control conditions of the study, all participants indicated that they saw the slides were blue. As in the Asch studies, however, confederates were involved in the experimental conditions. In the inconsistent condition, each group had four actual participants and two confederates, who described two thirds of the slides as green and the other third as blue. In the consistent condition, the two confederates described all the slides as green. Did this minority view change what the participants reported? It did, but primarily when the minority was consistent. When the confederates were consistent in saying a slide was green, 32% of the participants indicated at least once that they too thought the slide was green, compared with only 8% in the inconsistent condition and less than 1% in the control condition.

This research indicated that a minority can indeed have an influence over the majority, a shocking finding in a field that had previously considered only how the majority could pressure the individual. Armed with these findings, Moscovici also studied the lives of historical figures such as Galileo and Freud and how they succeeded in thwarting the consensus of their times. Moscovici (1980) proposed his conversion theory to explain how and why being influenced by a minority differs from being influenced by the majority. According to Moscovici, because people generally want to fit in with the majority group, they often go along with the majority position without deeply considering the message the majority is delivering. They tend just to accept it. The minority position, however, is by definition more distinctive. Although Moscovici reasoned that people generally don’t want to identify with the minority, the distinctiveness of the minority’s position better captures their attention. As a result, they tend to consider it more thoroughly; this deeper consideration can lead to a change in attitude.

Conversion theory

The explanation that people are influenced by a minority because the minority’s distinctive position better captures their attention.

247

Think ABOUT

[Buyenlarge/Getty Images]

Can you think of an example where after initially reacting to a minority opinion negatively, you were eventually influenced by it?

You may have noticed that the dual processes specified by Moscovici’s conversion theory sound quite similar to the dual automatic and controlled processes we described in our presentation of social cognition (see chapter 3). If you did, you’re right. Research indicates that the majority often influences us in a relatively automatic fashion. Especially when we’re not invested in an issue, we tend to give the majority the benefit of the doubt and accept its position without thinking too deeply about it. But because we don’t give those in the minority the benefit of the doubt, minorities often exert their influence only by provoking carefully elaborated thought (e.g., Crano & Chen, 1998; De Dreu & De Vries, 1993; Maass & Clark, 1983).

Martin and colleagues (2007) built on prior research showing that thoughtful processing tends to lead to private attitude change that better guides behavior, whereas the change in attitudes from more automatic processing tends to be more superficial, shorter lasting, and less influential in guiding behavior. Thus, Martin and colleagues reasoned that if being convinced by a minority involves more elaborate processing of the message, it should lead to a stronger and more enduring attitude that will be a more potent guide to behavior. In contrast, if one is convinced by a majority and this involves more superficial processing, the resulting attitude should be weaker and have less influence on one’s behavior.

FIGURE 7.5

Minority Opinion
The Palazzo dei Priori (left) and the Arco Etrusco (right) are two historical landmarks in Perugia, Italy. Mucchi-Faina et al. (1991) showed that study participants’ ideas about how to market these sites to increase tourism were more creative when participants thought that using these sites was advocated by the minority rather than the majority.
[(Left) © Stuart Robertson/Alamy; (right) Getty Images/DeAgostini]

To test this idea, they presented students with arguments, attributed to either a minority or a majority, recommending that students be required to pay membership fees to join the student union. The researchers also measured how relevant students felt this issue was to them personally, because earlier research had suggested that people process information more thoroughly when it is highly relevant (Petty & Cacioppo, 1984). Consistent with this prior finding, the study showed that when personal relevance was high, all participants, regardless of minority or majority influence, processed the message thoroughly. It was when the issue was low in personal relevance that influence from a minority, rather than a majority, source made a difference. Under these conditions, participants persuaded by a minority to pay membership fees developed a stronger attitude than those who were persuaded by a majority. In fact, those who received the message from a minority source were actually more likely to sign a petition supporting membership fees than were those who received the message from a majority. Martin and colleagues explain that when the argument was attributed to the minority, it was processed more deeply; as a consequence, it led to stronger and more enduring attitudes that had a greater influence on behavior.

Part of the reason that minorities exert their influence by provoking more systematic processing of their position is that people want to understand why the minority sees a given issue so differently from the majority. Certainly, in so doing the minority group or individual courts negative reactions from others. Research consistently finds that those who adopt minority positions are generally disliked (Nemeth, 1979), and those who adopt the minority position may in fact be aware that they are flirting with such disdain. This would help to explain the minority slowness effect. When people are asked about their attitudes on various topics such as sports, politics, celebrities, and social issues, those who adopt the minority position take longer to express their opinions (Bassili, 2003).

Minority slowness effect

Occurs when people who hold the minority position take longer to express their opinions.

248

The minority position presents a distinctive and interesting puzzle that people want to figure out. They can derive considerable benefits from trying to do so. As people think extensively about the minority’s argument and why it advocates that argument, they often think about different perspectives themselves, which allows for the consideration of novel and creative possibilities (Kenworthy et al., 2008; Nemeth, 1986). In a finding consistent with this idea, Charlan Nemeth and colleagues documented that although being influenced by the majority tends to elicit conventional problem-solving solutions, being influenced by a minority increases original thinking and diversified strategies in figuring out tasks such as word completions and word compositions (e.g., Nemeth & Kwan, 1987). In one example, Mucchi-Faina and colleagues (1991) had students from Perugia, Italy, try to think of ways to enhance the international reputation of their city. The students were shown pictures of two historic landmarks in Perugia, the Palazzo dei Priori (FIGURE 7.5, left) and the Arco Etrusco (FIGURE 7.5, right) and were told that marketing these sites to enhance the city’s reputation was advocated by either a majority or a minority of citizens surveyed. When students were presented with what they thought was the minority opinion on how to enhance the city’s reputation (versus what they thought was the majority opinion), they offered more creative and unique ideas about how to do so.

APPLICATION: How Minorities Can Be More Influential

APPLICATION:
How Minorities Can Be More Influential

Earlier we noted that in addition to conforming for normative reasons, people also conform because of informational influence. We tend to trust others as sources of information. A few years ago, your current author and family were heading to a political speech by then presidential candidate Barack Obama. We did not know exactly where he was speaking but simply followed the droves of people making their way to a particular location: We assumed that these other people knew where to go and followed their lead. We used others as a source of information. Of course, we certainly would have been less likely to follow only a few people heading in a particular direction. This example illustrates the types of issues for which majorities can have greater influence. As a result of peoples’ tendency to rely on others for information, minorities are not especially influential concerning issues for which there is an objective answer. We tend to trust the majority on questions of fact. In contrast, minorities tend to have their greatest potential for influence on matters of opinion. In one study, for example, minorities had less influence when Italian students were asked, “From which country does Italy import most of its raw oil?” than when the students were asked, “From which country should Italy import most of its raw oil?” (Maass et al., 1996).

249

Research also has identified a number of other qualities that enhance the likelihood that a minority can successfully sway the majority. These are important tips to keep in mind the next time you find yourself in a minority position and want to convince others.

It is important for the minority to project self-confidence and be consistent in its advocacy (e.g., Moscovici et al.,1969; Wood et al., 1994). When Dr. Martin Luther King Jr. endeavored to convince a nation of the need for racial equality, he did so with unwavering consistency, even though it meant that he was hated by many.

Although consistency and self-confidence are important, they can backfire if the minority person or group is perceived as rigid and inflexible. In these cases, the attributions will shift, and the minority position will be dismissed. (“He’s just a quack with nothing important for me to consider.”) Rather, studies show that a flexible and open-minded behavioral style, indicating a willingness to compromise, is an effective complement to consistency in promoting persuasion by either a minority or a majority (Moscovici et al., 1985).

Getting members of the majority to defect, or cross over, and adopt the minority view is one of the most potent ingredients of minority influence. In fact, in studies of jury decision making, a minority is more influential when it can achieve a defection from the majority than if it starts with someone already on its side (Nemeth & Wachtler, 1974). Part of the reason that this can lead to a snowball effect and influence the rest of the majority is that it undermines an “us versus them” mentality.

Finally, as with all forms of persuasion, the more people identify with the person attempting to persuade them, the more likely they are to be persuaded. In other words, we are more likely to be convinced if the minority (or majority) is part of our in-group, because we are more likely to be influenced by those who are like us (Maass & Clark, 1984).

SECTION review: Minority Influence

Minority Influence
Despite disturbance of the status quo, minorities can produce attitude change and throughout history have been agents of cultural and social change.
How minorities exert influence
  • Although people don’t want to identify with a minority, the distinctiveness of the minority position better captures their attention, and they give it deeper consideration. This can lead to lasting attitude change.
  • Minorities are generally disliked, so those holding a minority position may take longer to express their opinions.
  • The majority finds the minority position puzzling, which may lead to original thinking and diversified strategies in figuring out solutions.
  • Application: When minorities are influential

  • To influence opinion successfully, minority advocates need to be consistent, confident, and yet flexible.
  • A minority is more influential when there is a defection from the majority and when the minority is seen as part of the in-group.
  • 250