8-3 Using Emerging Behaviors to Infer Neural Maturation

As brain areas mature, a person’s behaviors correspond to the functions of the maturing areas. Stated differently, behaviors cannot emerge until the requisite neural machinery has developed. When that machinery is in place, however, related behaviors develop quickly through stages and are shaped significantly by epigenetic factors.

Researchers have studied these interacting changes in the brain and behavior, especially in regard to the emergence of motor skills, language, and problem solving in children. We now explore development in these three areas.

Motor Behaviors

Developing locomotion skills are easy to observe in human infants. At first, babies cannot move about independently, but eventually, they roll over, then crawl, then walk.

Other motor skills develop in less obvious but no less systematic ways. Shortly after birth, infants are capable of flexing their arms in such a way that they can scoop something toward their body, and they can direct a hand, as toward a breast when suckling. Between 1 and 3 months of age, babies also begin to make spontaneous hand and digit movements consisting of almost all the skilled finger movements they will make as an adult, a kind of motor babbling.

These movements at first are directed toward handling parts of their body and their clothes (Wallace & Whishaw, 2003). Only then are reaching movements directed toward objects in space. Tom Twitchell (1965) studied and described how the ability to reach for objects and grasp them progresses in stages, illustrated in Figure 8-20.

image
Figure 8-20: FIGURE 8-20 Development of the Grasping Response of Infants
Information from T. E. Twitchell (1965). The automatic grasping response of infants. Neuropsychologia, 3, p. 251.

Between 8 and 11 months, infants’ grasping becomes more sophisticated as the pincer grasp, employing the index finger and the thumb, develops. The pincer grasp is significant developmentally: it allows babies to make the very precise finger movements needed to manipulate small objects. What we see then, is a sequence in the development of grasping: first scooping, then grasping with all of the fingers, then grasping with independent finger movements.

261

If increasingly well-coordinated grasping depends on the emergence of certain neural machinery, anatomical changes in the brain should accompany the emergence of these motor behaviors. Such changes do take place, especially in the development of dendritic arborizations and in fiber connections between neocortex and spinal cord. And a correlation has been found between myelin formation and the ability to grasp (Yakovlev & Lecours, 1967).

A classic symptom of motor cortex damage, detailed in Section 11-1, is permanent loss of the pincer grasp.

In particular, a group of axons from motor cortex neurons myelinate at about the same time that whole-hand reaching and grasping develop. Another group of motor cortex neurons known to control finger movements myelinates at about the time that the pincer grasp develops. MRI studies of changes in cortical thickness show that increased motor dexterity is associated with decreased cortical thickness in the hand region of the left motor cortex of right-handers (Figure 8-21A).

image
Figure 8-21: FIGURE 8-21 Correlations Between Gray Matter Thickness and Behavior (A) Red shading corresponds to regions showing significant cortical thinning correlated with improved motor skills. (B) White shading corresponds to regions showing significant cortical thickening correlated with improved language skills. (C) Red shading shows regions of decreased cortical thickness correlated with improved vocabulary scores.
(A) and (B) Research from L. H. Lu, C. M. Leonard, P. M. Thompson, E. Kan, J. Jolley, et al. (2007). Normal developmental changes in inferior frontal gray matter are associated with improvement in phonological processing: A longitudinal MRI analysis. Cerebral Cortex, 17, pp. 1092–1099. (C) Research from Elizabeth R. Sowell, Paul M. Thompson, Christiana M. Leonard, Suzanne E. Welcome, Eric Kan, and Arthur W. Toga. Longitudinal MAPping of Cortical Thickness and Brain Growth in Normal Children. J. Neurosci. 2004 24: 8223–8231. Figure 9.

We can now make a simple prediction. If specific motor cortex neurons are essential for adultlike grasping movements to emerge, removing those neurons should make an adult’s grasping ability similar to a young infant’s, which is in fact what happens.

Language Development

The gradual series of developments that accompanies speech acquisition has usually progressed significantly by age 3 or 4. According to Eric Lenneberg (1967), children reach certain important speech milestones in a fixed sequence and at constant chronological ages. Children start to form a vocabulary by 12 months. This 5-to-10-word repertoire typically doubles over the next 6 months. By 2 years, vocabulary will range from 200 to 300 words that include mostly everyday objects. In another year, vocabulary approaches 1000 words and begins to include simple sentences. At 6 years, children boast a vocabulary of about 2500 words and can understand more than 20,000 words en route to an adult vocabulary of more than 50,000 words.

Although language skills and motor skills generally develop in parallel, the capacity for language depends on more than the ability to make controlled movements of the mouth, lips, and tongue. Precise movements of the muscles controlling these body parts develop well before children can speak. Furthermore, even when children have sufficient motor skill to articulate most words, their vocabulary does not rocket ahead but rather progresses gradually.

A small proportion of children (about 1 percent) have typical intelligence and motor skill development, yet their speech acquisition is markedly delayed. Such children may not begin to speak in phrases until after age 4, despite an apparently healthy environment and the absence of any obvious neurological signs of brain damage. Because the timing of speech onset appears universal in the remaining 99 percent of children across all cultures, something different has likely taken place in the brain maturation of a child with late language acquisition. Specifying what that difference is—that is hard.

262

Because the age of language onset is usually between 1 and 2 and language acquisition is largely complete by age 12, the best strategy considers how the cortex is different before and after these two milestones. By age 2, cell division and migration are complete in the language zones of the cerebral cortex. The major changes that take place from age 2 to 12 are in neuronal connectivity and myelination of the speech zones.

Changes in dendritic complexity in these areas are among the most impressive in the brain. Recall from Figure 8-12 that the axons and dendrites of the speech zone called Broca’s area are simple at birth but grow dramatically denser at age 15 to 24 months. This neuronal development correlates with an equally dramatic change in language ability, given that a baby’s vocabulary starts to expand rapidly at about age 2.

Focus 7-1 describes research on newborns’ reactions to language.

We can therefore infer that language development may be constrained, at least in part, by the maturing language areas in the cortex. Individual differences in the speed of language acquisition may be accounted for by differences in this neural development. Children with early language ability may have an early-maturating speech zone, whereas it may develop later in children with delayed language onset.

Results of MRI studies of the language cortex show that, in contrast with the thinning of motor cortex associated with enhanced dexterity shown in Figure 8-21A, a thickening of the left inferior frontal cortex areas associated with enhanced phonological processing (understanding speech sounds), as shown in Figure 8-21B. The unique association between cortical thickening and phonological processing is not due to a general relation between all language functions and cortical thickening, however. Figure 8-21C shows significant thinning of diffuse cortical regions associated with better vocabulary—regions outside the language areas—and vocabulary is one of the best predictors of general intelligence.

Development of Problem-Solving Ability

The first researcher to try to identify discrete stages of cognitive development was Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget (1952). He realized that he could infer children’s understanding of the world by observing their behavior. For example, a baby who lifts a cloth to retrieve a hidden toy shows an understanding that objects continue to exist even when out of sight. This understanding of object permanence is revealed by the behavior of the infant in the upper row of photographs in Figure 8-22.

image
Figure 8-22: FIGURE 8-22 Two Stages of Cognitive Development The infant shows that she understands object permanence—that things continue to exist when they are out of sight (top). This girl does not yet understand the principle of conservation of liquid volume. Beakers with identical volumes but different shapes seem to her to hold different amounts of liquid (bottom).
Doug Goodman/Science Source
Copyright 2016 Macmillan; Photo by Ellie Miller

263

An absence of understanding also can be seen in children’s behavior, as shown by the actions of the 5-year-old girl in the lower row of photographs in Figure 8-22. She was shown two identical beakers with identical volumes of liquid, then watched as one beaker’s liquid was poured into a shorter, wider beaker. When asked which beaker contained more liquid, she pointed to the taller beaker, not understanding that the amount of liquid remains constant despite the difference in appearance. Children display an understanding of this principle, the conservation of liquid volume, at about age 7.

By studying children engaged in such tasks, Piaget concluded that cognitive development is a continuous process. Children’s strategies for exploring the world and their understanding of it are constantly changing. These changes are not simply the result of acquiring specific pieces of new knowledge. Rather, at certain points in development, fundamental changes take place in the organization of a child’s strategies for learning about the world and for solving problems. With these developing strategies comes new understanding.

Piaget identified four major stages of cognitive development, summarized in Table 8-2:

image

If we take Piaget’s stages as rough approximations of qualitative changes that take place in children’s thinking as they grow older, we can ask what neural changes might underlie them. One place to look for brain changes is in the relative rate of brain growth.

After birth, brain and body do not grow uniformly but rather during irregularly occurring periods commonly called growth spurts. In his analysis of ratios of brain weight to body weight, Herman Epstein (1979) found consistent spurts in brain growth between 3 and 10 months (accounting for an increase of 30 percent in brain weight by age 18 months) as well as ages 2 to 4, 6 to 8, 10 to 12, and 14 to 16+ years. The increments in brain weight were about 5 percent to 10 percent in each of these 2-year periods.

264

EXPERIMENT 8-1

Question: In what sequence do the forebrain structures required for learning and memory mature?

image
Research from W. H. Overman, J. Bachevalier, M. Turner, and A. Peuster (1992). Object recognition versus object discrimination: Comparison between human infants and infant monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, p. 18.

Conclusion: Neural structures underlying the concurrent-discrimination task mature sooner than those underlying the nonmatching-to-sample task

Brain growth takes place without a concurrent increase in the number of neurons, and so it is most likely due to the growth of glial cells, blood vessels, myelin, and synapses. Although synapses themselves would be unlikely to add much weight to the brain, their growth is accompanied by increased metabolic demands that cause neurons to become larger, new blood vessels to form, and new astrocytes to be produced for neuronal support and nourishment.

We would expect such an increase in cortical complexity to generate more complex behaviors, so we might predict significant, perhaps qualitative, changes in cognitive function during each growth spurt. The first four brain growth spurts Epstein identified coincide nicely with the four main stages of cognitive development Piaget described. Such correspondence suggests significant alterations in neural functioning with the onset of each cognitive stage.

At the same time, differences in the rate of brain development, or perhaps in the rate at which specific groups of neurons mature, may account for individual differences in the age at which the various cognitive advances identified by Piaget emerge. Although Piaget did not identify a fifth stage of cognitive development in later adolescence, a growth spurt that occurs then implies one.

Growth spurts are superficial measures of changes taking place in the brain. To link them to cognitive development, we need to know at a deeper level what neural events are contributing to brain growth and just where they are taking place. A way to find out is to observe healthy children’s attempts to solve specific problems that are diagnostic of damage to discrete brain regions in adults. If children perform a particular task poorly, then whatever brain region regulates that task must not yet be mature. Similarly, if children can perform one task but not another, the tasks apparently require different brain structures, and these structures must mature at different rates.

William Overman and Jocelyne Bachevalier (Overman et al., 1992) used this logic to study the development of forebrain structures required for learning and memory in young children and in monkeys. The Procedure section of Experiment 8-1 shows the three intelligence test items presented to their participants. The first task was simply to learn to displace an object to obtain a food reward. When participants had learned the displacement task, they were trained in two more tasks believed to measure temporal lobe and basal ganglia functioning, respectively.

265

In the nonmatching-to-sample task, participants were shown an object they could displace to receive a food reward. After a brief (15-second) delay, two objects were presented: the first object and a novel object. The participants then had to displace the novel object to obtain the food reward. Nonmatching to sample is thought to measure object recognition, which is a temporal lobe function. The participant can find the food only by recognizing the original object and not choosing it.

In the third task, concurrent discrimination, participants were presented with a pair of objects and had to learn that one object in that pair was always associated with a food reward, whereas the other object was never rewarded. The task was made more difficult by sequentially giving participants 20 different object pairs. Each day, they were presented with one trial per pair. Concurrent discrimination is thought to measure trial-and-error learning of specific object information, a function of the basal ganglia.

Adults easily solve both the nonmatching and the concurrent tasks but report that the concurrent task is more difficult because it requires remembering far more information. The key question developmentally is whether there is a difference in the ages at which children (or monkeys) can solve these two tasks.

It turns out that children can solve the concurrent task by about 12 months of age, but not until about 18 months can they solve what most adults believe to be the easier nonmatching task. These results imply that the basal ganglia, the critical area for the concurrent discrimination task, mature more quickly than the temporal lobe, the critical region for the nonmatching-to-sample task.

A Caution about Linking Correlation to Causation

Throughout this section we have described research results implying that changes in the brain cause changes in behavior. Neuroscientists assert that by looking at behavioral development and brain development in parallel, they can make some inferences regarding the causes of behavior. Bear in mind, however, that the fact that two things correlate (take place together) does not prove that one of them causes the other.

The correlation–causation problem raises red flags in brain and behavior studies, because research in behavioral neuroscience, by its very nature, is often based on correlations. Nevertheless, correlational studies, especially of development, have proved a powerful source of insight into the principles of brain and behavior.

8-3 REVIEW

Using Emerging Behaviors to Infer Neural Maturation

Before you continue, check your understanding.

Question 1

The last stage in motor development in infants is the ability to make __________.

Question 2

Language development is correlated with cortical thinning related to __________ and cortical thickening related to __________.

Question 3

Brain growth spurts correlate with __________.

Question 4

The nonmatching-to-sample task is believed to measure the function of the __________; the concurrent-discrimination learning task is believed to measure the function of the __________.

Question 5

Describe a major challenge in inferring changes in brain development from the emergence of behaviors.

Answers appear in the Self Test section of the book.