Group living has benefits and costs

1137

Apart from their direct influences on reproductive success, social systems can contribute to survival in many ways, but they can also involve costs. Thus the cost–benefit approach of behavioral ecology is relevant to understanding the evolution of social behavior.

An obvious example of a benefit of group living is improved foraging efficiency. By hunting in packs, African wild dogs (see Figure 58.17) employ cooperative strategies that enable them to bring down larger prey than could a single dog. The larger the pack, the greater the hunting success rate. Once the prey is killed, the presence of conspecifics also reduces the risk that the wild dogs will lose their prey to larger scavengers, such as hyenas.

Living in a group can also reduce the risk of individuals becoming prey themselves. Many small birds forage in flocks. To test the hypothesis that flocking provides protection against predators, R. E. Kenward released a trained goshawk (Accipiter gentilis) near wild common wood-pigeons (Columba palumbus) in England. The hawk was most successful when it attacked solitary pigeons. Its success in capturing a pigeon in a flock decreased as the number of pigeons in the flock increased (Figure 52.21A). The larger the flock, the sooner some individual in the flock spotted the hawk and flew away. This escape behavior stimulated other individuals in the flock to take flight as well.

image
Figure 52.21 Group Living Provides Protection from Predators Animals that live in groups can spread the cost of looking out for predators. (A) The larger the number of common wood-pigeons in a flock, the greater the chances that one of the pigeons will spot a predatory goshawk before it attacks, and the lower the chances that the hawk will capture one of the pigeons. (B) A male Belding’s ground squirrel gives an alarm call upon spotting a predator. Although this behavior increases his individual risk of becoming prey, he increases the survival chances of many of his close relatives.

Alarm calling is another means of reducing predation risk, but the caller incurs a risk cost by calling attention to itself. Belding’s ground squirrels (Urocitellus beldingi) live in large colonies in open meadows. When one squirrel announces the presence of a predator with loud, sharp barks, all the nearby squirrels dive into their burrows (Figure 52.21B). Paul Sherman of Cornell University showed that callers double their risk of being preyed on—so why do they do it? Research by Sherman and by others has shown that this altruistic behavior is a product of kin selection. In this polygynous species, males establish large territories in the spring that include the territories of several females, whom they inseminate. The females then drive off the males. Female offspring settle near their mothers, so neighboring females in a colony tend to be sisters, and they defend each other’s young. Sherman showed that males are less likely to give alarm calls than females, and that females are more likely to give alarm calls when related individuals are nearby.

Social behavior has many costs as well as benefits. Foraging in a group may reduce the amount of food available to each individual, and the foraging individuals may interfere with one another’s foraging activities. Individuals living in groups may face more competition for mates, as well as for food, than solitary individuals would. A large group may actually attract the attention of predators. And living at high population densities can increase the risk of disease transmission. The study of disease transmission in wild animal populations is a relatively new field, but such studies have made it apparent that species living in social groups are more prone to outbreaks of disease than are solitary species.