6.1 Basic Concepts of Sensation and Perception

sensation the process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus energies from our environment.

6-1 What are sensation and perception? What do we mean by bottom-up processing and top-down processing?

perception the process of organizing and interpreting sensory information, enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events.

Heather Sellers’ curious mix of “perfect vision” and face blindness illustrates the distinction between sensation and perception. When she looks at a friend, her sensation is normal: Her sensory receptors detect the same information yours would, and her nervous system transmits that information to her brain. Her perception—the processes by which her brain organizes and interprets sensory input—is almost normal. Thus, she may recognize people from their hair, gait, voice, or particular physique, just not their face. Her experience is much like the struggle you or I would have trying to recognize a specific penguin.

In our everyday experiences, sensation and perception blend into one continuous process.

As our brain absorbs the information in FIGURE 6.1, bottom-up processing enables our sensory systems to detect the lines, angles, and colors that form the flower and leaves. Using top-down processing we interpret what our senses detect.

Figure 6.1
What’s going on here? Our sensory and perceptual processes work together to help us sort out the complex images, including the hidden couple in Sandro Del-Prete’s drawing, The Flowering of Love.

But how do we do it? How do we create meaning from the blizzard of sensory stimuli that bombards our bodies 24 hours a day? Meanwhile, in a silent, cushioned, inner world, our brain floats in utter darkness. By itself, it sees nothing. It hears nothing. It feels nothing. So, how does the world out there get in? To phrase the question scientifically: How do we construct our representations of the external world? How do a campfire’s flicker, crackle, and smoky scent activate neural connections? And how, from this living neurochemistry, do we create our conscious experience of the fire’s motion and temperature, its aroma and beauty? In search of answers, let’s look at some processes that cut across all our sensory systems.

Transduction

6-2 What three steps are basic to all our sensory systems?

Every second of every day, our sensory systems perform an amazing feat: They convert one form of energy into another. Vision processes light energy. Hearing processes sound waves. All our senses

transduction conversion of one form of energy into another. In sensation, the transforming of stimulus energies, such as sights, sounds, and smells, into neural impulses our brain can interpret.

psychophysics the study of relationships between the physical characteristics of stimuli, such as their intensity, and our psychological experience of them.

The process of converting one form of energy into another that our brain can use is called transduction. Later in this chapter, we’ll focus on individual sensory systems. How do we see? Hear? Taste? Smell? Feel pain? Keep our balance? In each case, one of our sensory systems receives, transforms, and delivers the information to our brain. The field of psychophysics studies the relationships between the physical energy we can detect and its effects on our psychological experiences.

Let’s explore some strengths and weaknesses in our ability to detect and interpret stimuli in the vast sea of energy around us.

231

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • What is the rough distinction between sensation and perception?

Sensation is the bottom-up process by which our sensory receptors and our nervous system receive and represent stimuli. Perception is the top-down process in which our brain creates meaning by organizing and interpreting what our senses detect.

Thresholds

6-3 How do absolute thresholds and difference thresholds differ, and what effect, if any, do stimuli below the absolute threshold have on us?

At this moment, we are being struck by X-rays and radio waves, ultraviolet and infrared light, and sound waves of very high and very low frequencies. To all of these we are blind and deaf. Other animals with differing needs detect a world that lies beyond our experience. Migrating birds stay on course aided by an internal magnetic compass. Bats and dolphins locate prey using sonar, bouncing echoing sound off objects. Bees navigate on cloudy days by detecting invisible (to us) polarized light.

The shades on our own senses are open just a crack, allowing us a restricted awareness of this vast sea of energy. But for our needs, this is enough.

Absolute Thresholds

absolute threshold the minimum stimulus energy needed to detect a particular stimulus 50 percent of the time.

To some kinds of stimuli we are exquisitely sensitive. Standing atop a mountain on an utterly dark, clear night, most of us could see a candle flame atop another mountain 30 miles away. We could feel the wing of a bee falling on our cheek. We could smell a single drop of perfume in a three-room apartment (Galanter, 1962).

German scientist and philosopher Gustav Fechner (1801–1887) studied our awareness of these faint stimuli and called them our absolute thresholds—the minimum stimulation necessary to detect a particular light, sound, pressure, taste, or odor 50 percent of the time. To test your absolute threshold for sounds, a hearing specialist would expose each of your ears to varying sound levels (FIGURE 6.2 below). For each tone, the test would define where half the time you could detect the sound and half the time you could not. That 50-50 point would define your absolute threshold.

Figure 6.2
Absolute threshold Can I detect this sound? An absolute threshold is the intensity at which a person can detect a stimulus half the time. Hearing tests locate these thresholds for various frequencies.

signal detection theory a theory predicting how and when we detect the presence of a faint stimulus (signal) amid background stimulation (noise). Assumes there is no single absolute threshold and that detection depends partly on a person’s experience, expectations, motivation, and alertness.

Detecting a weak stimulus, or signal (such as a hearing-test tone), depends not only on its strength but also on our psychological state—our experience, expectations, motivation, and alertness. Signal detection theory predicts when we will detect weak signals (measured as our ratio of “hits” to “false alarms”). Lonely, anxious people at speed-dating events tend to respond with a low threshold, and thus can be unselective in reaching out to potential dates (McClure et al., 2010). Signal detection theorists seek to understand why people respond differently to the same stimuli, and why the same person’s reactions vary as circumstances change.

Try out this old riddle on a couple of friends. “You’re driving a bus with 12 passengers. At your first stop, 6 passengers get off. At the second stop, 3 get off. At the third stop, 2 more get off but 3 new people get on. What color are the bus driver’s eyes?” Do your friends detect the signal—who is the bus driver?—amid the accompanying noise?

subliminal below one’s absolute threshold for conscious awareness.

priming the activation, often unconsciously, of certain associations, thus predisposing one’s perception, memory, or response.

Stimuli you cannot detect 50 percent of the time are subliminal—below your absolute threshold (see FIGURE 6.2). Under certain conditions, you can be affected by stimuli so weak that you don’t consciously notice them. An unnoticed image or word can reach your visual cortex and briefly prime your response to a later question. In a typical experiment, the image or word is quickly flashed, then replaced by a masking stimulus that interrupts the brain’s processing before conscious perception (Herring et al., 2013; Van den Bussche et al., 2009). In one such experiment, researchers monitored brain activity as they primed people with either unperceived action words (such as go and start) or inaction words (such as still or stop). Without any conscious awareness, the inaction words automatically evoked brain activity associated with inhibiting behavior (Hepler & Albarracin, 2013).

“The heart has its reasons which reason does not know.”

Pascal, Pensées, 1670

Another priming experiment illustrated the deep reality of sexual orientation. As people gazed at the center of a screen, a photo of a nude person was flashed on one side and a scrambled version of the photo on the other side (Jiang et al., 2006). Because the nude images were immediately masked by a colored checkerboard, viewers saw nothing but flashes of color and were unable to state on which side the nude had appeared. To test whether this unseen image unconsciously attracted their attention, the experimenters then flashed a geometric figure to one side or the other. This, too, was quickly followed by a masking stimulus. When asked to give the figure’s angle, straight men guessed more accurately when it appeared where a nude woman had been a moment earlier (FIGURE 6.3). Gay men (and straight women) guessed more accurately when the geometric figure replaced a nude man. As other experiments confirm, we can evaluate a stimulus even when we are not aware of it—and even when we are unaware of our evaluation (Ferguson & Zayas, 2009).

Figure 6.3
The hidden mind After an image of a nude man or woman was flashed to one side or another, then masked before being perceived, people’s attention was unconsciously drawn to images in a way that reflected their sexual orientation (Jiang et al., 2006).

232

How can we feel or respond to what we do not know and cannot describe? An imperceptibly brief stimulus often triggers a weak response that can be detected by brain scanning (Blankenburg et al., 2003; Haynes & Rees, 2005, 2006). The stimulus may reach consciousness only when it triggers synchronized activity in multiple brain areas (Dehaene, 2009, 2014). Such experiments reveal the dual-track mind at work: Much of our information processing occurs automatically, out of sight, off the radar screen of our conscious mind. Our conscious minds are upstairs executives who delegate routine tasks to downstairs mental butlers.

So can we be controlled by subliminal messages? For more on that question, see Thinking Critically About: Subliminal Persuasion.

Difference Thresholds

difference threshold the minimum difference between two stimuli required for detection 50 percent of the time. We experience the difference threshold as a just noticeable difference (or jnd).

To function effectively, we need absolute thresholds low enough to allow us to detect important sights, sounds, textures, tastes, and smells. We also need to detect small differences among stimuli. A musician must detect minute discrepancies when tuning an instrument. Parents must detect the sound of their own child’s voice amid other children’s voices. While living two years in Scotland, I [DM] noticed that sheep baa’s all sound alike to my ears. But not to those of ewes, who, after shearing, will streak directly to the baa of their lamb amid the chorus of other distressed lambs.

The difference threshold (or the just noticeable difference [jnd]) is the minimum difference a person can detect between any two stimuli half the time. That difference threshold increases with the size of the stimulus. If we listen to our music at 40 decibels, we might detect an added 5 decibels. But if we increase the volume to 110 decibels, we probably won’t detect a 5 decibel change.

233

THINKING CRITICALLY ABOUT

Subliminal Persuasion

6-4 Does subliminal sensation enable subliminal persuasion?

Hoping to penetrate our unconscious, entrepreneurs offer audio and video programs to help us lose weight, stop smoking, or improve our memories. Soothing ocean sounds may mask messages we cannot consciously hear: “I am thin”; “Smoke tastes bad”; or “I do well on tests—I have total recall of information.” Such claims make two assumptions: (1) We can unconsciously sense subliminal (literally, “below threshold”) stimuli. (2) Without our awareness, these stimuli have extraordinary suggestive powers. Can we? Do they?

As we have seen, subliminal sensation is a fact. Remember that an “absolute” threshold is merely the point at which we can detect a stimulus half the time. At or slightly below this threshold, we will still detect the stimulus some of the time.

But does this mean that claims of subliminal persuasion are also facts? The near-consensus among researchers is No. The laboratory research reveals a subtle, fleeting effect. Priming thirsty people with the subliminal word thirst might therefore, for a moment, make a thirst-quenching beverage ad more persuasive (Strahan et al., 2002). Likewise, priming thirsty people with Lipton Iced Tea may increase their choosing the primed brand (Karremans et al., 2006; Veltkamp et al., 2011; Verwijmeren et al., 2011a,b). But the subliminal-message hucksters claim something different: a powerful, enduring effect on behavior.

To test whether subliminal recordings have this enduring effect, Anthony Greenwald and his colleagues (1991, 1992) randomly assigned university students to listen daily for five weeks to commercial subliminal messages claiming to improve either self-esteem or memory. But the researchers played a practical joke and switched half the labels. Some students who thought they were receiving affirmations of self-esteem were actually hearing the memory-enhancement message. Others got the self-esteem message but thought their memory was being recharged.

Subliminal persuasion? Although subliminally presented stimuli can subtly influence people, experiments discount attempts at subliminal advertising and self-improvement. (The playful message here is not actually subliminal—because you can easily perceive it.)

Were the recordings effective? Students’ test scores for self-esteem and memory, taken before and after the five weeks, revealed no changes. Yet the students perceived themselves receiving the benefits they expected. Those who thought they had heard a memory recording believed their memories had improved. Those who thought they had heard a self-esteem recording believed their self-esteem had grown. (Reading this research, one hears echoes of the testimonies that ooze from ads for such products. Some customers, having bought what is not supposed to be heard (and having indeed not heard it!) offer testimonials like, “I really know that your recordings were invaluable in reprogramming my mind.”)

Over a decade, Greenwald conducted 16 double-blind experiments evaluating subliminal self-help recordings. His results were uniform: No recording helped more than a placebo, which works only because of our belief in it (Greenwald, 1992).

Weber’s law the principle that, to be perceived as different, two stimuli must differ by a constant minimum percentage (rather than a constant amount).

In the late 1800s, Ernst Weber noted something so simple and so widely applicable that we still refer to it as Weber’s law. This law states that for an average person to perceive a difference, two stimuli must differ by a constant minimum percentage (not a constant amount). The exact proportion varies, depending on the stimulus. Two lights, for example, must differ in intensity by 8 percent. Two objects must differ in weight by 2 percent. And two tones must differ in frequency by only 0.3 percent (Teghtsoonian, 1971).

The difference threshold In this computer-generated copy of the Twenty-third Psalm, each line of the typeface increases slightly. How many lines are required for you to experience a just noticeable difference?

234

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • Using sound as your example, explain how these concepts differ: absolute threshold, subliminal stimulation, and difference threshold.

Absolute threshold is the minimum stimulation needed to detect a particular sound (such as an approaching bike on the sidewalk behind us) 50 percent of the time. Subliminal stimulation happens when, without our awareness, our sensory system processes the sound of the approaching bike (when it is below our absolute threshold). A difference threshold is the minimum difference needed to distinguish between two sounds (such as the familiar hum of a friend’s bike and the unfamiliar sound of another bike).

Sensory Adaptation

sensory adaptation diminished sensitivity as a consequence of constant stimulation.

6-5 What is the function of sensory adaptation?

“We need above all to know about changes; no one wants or needs to be reminded 16 hours a day that his shoes are on.”

Neuroscientist David Hubel (1979)

Entering your neighbors’ living room, you smell a musty odor. You wonder how they endure it, but within minutes you no longer notice it. Sensory adaptation has come to your rescue. When we are constantly exposed to an unchanging stimulus, we become less aware of it because our nerve cells fire less frequently. (To experience sensory adaptation, move your watch up your wrist an inch: You will feel it—but only for a few moments.)

Why, then, if we stare at an object without flinching, does it not vanish from sight? Because, unnoticed by us, our eyes are always moving. This continual flitting from one spot to another ensures that stimulation on the eyes’ receptors continually changes (FIGURE 6.4).

Figure 6.4
The jumpy eye Our gaze jumps from one spot to another every third of a second or so, as eye-tracking equipment illustrated as a person looked at this photograph of Edinburgh’s Princes Street Gardens (Henderson, 2007). The circles represent visual fixations, and the numbers indicate the time of fixation in milliseconds (300 milliseconds = 3/10ths of a second).

What if we actually could stop our eyes from moving? Would sights seem to vanish, as odors do? To find out, psychologists have devised ingenious instruments that maintain a constant image on the eye’s inner surface. Imagine that we have fitted a volunteer, Mary, with one of these instruments—a miniature projector mounted on a contact lens (FIGURE 6.5a). When Mary’s eye moves, the image from the projector moves as well. So everywhere that Mary looks, the scene is sure to go.

Figure 6.5
Sensory adaptation: now you see it, now you don’t! (a) A projector mounted on a contact lens makes the projected image move with the eye. (b) Initially, the person sees the stabilized image, but soon she sees fragments fading and reappearing. (From “Stabilized images on the retina,” by R. M. Pritchard. Copyright © 1961 Scientific American, Inc. All rights reserved.)

If we project images through this instrument, what will Mary see? At first, she will see the complete image. But within a few seconds, as her sensory system begins to fatigue, things get weird. Bit by bit, the image vanishes, only to reappear and then disappear—often in fragments (FIGURE 6.5b).

Although sensory adaptation reduces our sensitivity, it offers an important benefit: freedom to focus on informative changes in our environment without being distracted by background chatter. Stinky or heavily perfumed people don’t notice their odor because, like you and me, they adapt to what’s constant and detect only change. Our sensory receptors are alert to novelty; bore them with repetition and they free our attention for more important things. The point to remember: We perceive the world not exactly as it is, but as it is useful for us to perceive it.

Our sensitivity to changing stimulation helps explain television’s attention-grabbing power. Cuts, edits, zooms, pans, sudden noises—all demand attention. The phenomenon is irresistible even to TV researchers. One noted that during conversations, “I cannot for the life of me stop from periodically glancing over to the screen” (Tannenbaum, 2002).

Sensory adaptation even influences how we perceive emotions. By creating a 50-50 morphed blend of an angry face and a scared face, researchers showed that our visual system adapts to a static facial expression by becoming less responsive to it (Butler et al., 2008; FIGURE 6.6). The effect is created by our brain, not our retinas. We know this because the illusion also works when we view either side image with one eye, and the center image with the other eye.

Figure 6.6
Emotion adaptation Gaze at the angry face on the left for 20 to 30 seconds, then look at the center face (looks scared, yes?). Then gaze at the scared face on the right for 20 to 30 seconds, before returning to the center face (now looks angry, yes?). (From Butler et al., 2008.)

235

Sensory adaptation and sensory thresholds are important ingredients in our perceptions of the world around us. Much of what we perceive comes not just from what’s “out there” but also from what’s behind our eyes and between our ears.

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • Why is it that after wearing shoes for a while, you cease to notice them (until questions like this draw your attention back to them)?

The shoes provide constant stimulation. Sensory adaptation allows us to focus on changing stimuli.

Perceptual Set

perceptual set a mental predisposition to perceive one thing and not another.

6-6 How do our expectations, contexts, motivation, and emotions influence our perceptions?

To see is to believe. As we less fully appreciate, to believe is to see. Through experience, we come to expect certain results. Those expectations may give us a perceptual set, a set of mental tendencies and assumptions that affects (top-down) what we hear, taste, feel, and see.

Consider: Is the center image in FIGURE 6.7 below an old or young woman? What we see in such a drawing can be influenced by first looking at either of the two unambiguous versions (Boring, 1930).

Figure 6.7
Perceptual set Show a friend either the left or right image. Then show the center image and ask, “What do you see?” Whether your friend reports seeing an old woman’s face or young woman’s profile may depend on which of the other two drawings was viewed first. In each of those images, the meaning is clear, and it will establish perceptual expectations.

Everyday examples of perceptual set—of “mind over mind”—abound. In 1972, a British newspaper published unretouched photographs of a “monster” in Scotland’s Loch Ness—“the most amazing pictures ever taken,” stated the paper. If this information creates in you the same expectations it did in most of the paper’s readers, you, too, will see the monster in a similar photo in FIGURE 6.8. But when a skeptical researcher approached the original photos with different expectations, he saw a curved tree limb—as had others the day that photo was shot (Campbell, 1986). With this different perceptual set, you may now notice that the object is floating motionless, with ripples outward in all directions—hardly what we would expect of a swimming monster.

Figure 6.8
Believing is seeing What do you perceive? Is this Nessie, the Loch Ness monster, or a log?

236

There
Are Two
Errors in The
The Title Of
This Book

Book by Robert M. Martin, 2011

Did you perceive what you expected in this title—and miss the errors? If you are still puzzled, see explanation below.

The title’s first error is its repeated “the.” Its ironic second error is its misstatement that there are two errors, when there is only one.

Perceptual set can also affect what we hear. Consider the kindly airline pilot who, on a takeoff run, looked over at his sad co-pilot and said, “Cheer up.” Expecting to hear the usual “Gear up,” the co-pilot promptly raised the wheels—before they left the ground (Reason & Mycielska, 1982).

Perceptual set similarly affects taste. One experiment invited bar patrons to sample free beer (Lee et al., 2006). When researchers added a few drops of vinegar to a brand-name beer, the tasters preferred it—unless they had been told they were drinking vinegar-laced beer. Then they expected, and usually experienced, a worse taste. In another experiment, preschool children, by a 6-to-1 margin, thought french fries tasted better when served in a McDonald’s bag rather than a plain white bag (Robinson et al., 2007).

What determines our perceptual set? Through experience we form concepts, or schemas, that organize and interpret unfamiliar information. Our preexisting schemas for monsters and tree trunks influence how we apply top-down processing to interpret ambiguous sensations.

“We hear and apprehend only what we already half know.”

Henry David Thoreau, Journal, 1860

In everyday life, stereotypes about gender (another instance of perceptual set) can color perception. Without the obvious cues of pink or blue, people will struggle over whether to call the new baby “he” or “she.” But told an infant is “David,” people (especially children) have perceived “him” as bigger and stronger than if the same infant was called “Diana” (Stern & Karraker, 1989). Some differences, it seems, exist merely in the eyes of their beholders.

237

Context Effects

A given stimulus may trigger radically different perceptions, partly because of our differing perceptual set (FIGURE 6.9), but also because of the immediate context. Some examples:

Figure 6.9
Culture and context effects What is above the woman’s head? In one study, nearly all the East Africans who were questioned said the woman was balancing a metal box or can on her head and that the family was sitting under a tree. Westerners, for whom corners and box-like architecture were more common, were more likely to perceive the family as being indoors, with the woman sitting under a window. (Adapted from Gregory & Gombrich, 1973.)
Figure 6.10
What emotion is this? (See Figure 6.11 below.)
Figure 6.11
Context makes clearer Serena Williams is celebrating! (Example from Barrett et al., 2011.)

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • Does perceptual set involve bottom-up or top-down processing? Why?

It involves top-down processing. Our perceptual set influences our interpretation of stimuli based on our experiences, assumptions, and expectations.

Motivation and Emotion

Perceptions are influenced, top-down, not only by our expectations and by the context, but also by our emotions and motivation.

Hearing sad rather than happy music can predispose people to perceive a sad meaning in spoken homophonic words—mourning rather than morning, die rather than dye, pain rather than pane (Halberstadt et al., 1995). When angry, people more often perceive neutral objects as guns (Baumann & Steno, 2010). After listening to irritating (and anger-cuing) music, they also perceive a harmful action such as robbery as more serious (Seidel & Prinz, 2013).

Dennis Proffitt (2006a,b; Schnall et al., 2008) and others have demonstrated the power of emotions with other clever experiments showing that

238

Motives also matter. Desired objects, such as a water bottle when thirsty, seem closer (Balcetis & Dunning, 2010). This perceptual bias energizes our going for it. Our motives also direct our perception of ambiguous images.

“When you’re hitting the ball, it comes at you looking like a grapefruit. When you’re not, it looks like a blackeyed pea.”

Former major league baseball player George Scott

Emotions and motives color our social perceptions, too. People more often perceive solitary confinement, sleep deprivation, and cold temperatures as “torture” when experiencing a small dose of such themselves (Nordgren et al., 2011). Spouses who feel loved and appreciated perceive less threat in stressful marital events—“He’s just having a bad day” (Murray et al., 2003). Professional referees, if told a soccer team has a history of aggressive behavior, will assign more penalty cards after watching videotaped fouls (Jones et al., 2002). The moral of these stories: To believe is, indeed, to see.

REVIEW: Basic Concepts of Sensation and Perception

REVIEW Basic Concepts of Sensation and Perception

LEARNING OBJECTIVES

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE Take a moment to answer each of these Learning Objective Questions (repeated here from within this section). Then click the 'show answer' button to check your answers. Research suggests that trying to answer these questions on your own will improve your long-term retention (McDaniel et al., 2009).

6-1 What are sensation and perception? What do we mean by bottom-up processing and top-down processing?

Sensation is the process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus energies from our environment. Perception is the process of organizing and interpreting this information, enabling recognition of meaningful events. Sensation and perception are actually parts of one continuous process. Bottom-up processing is sensory analysis that begins at the entry level, with information flowing from the sensory receptors to the brain. Top-down processing is information processing guided by high-level mental processes, as when we construct perceptions by filtering information through our experience and expectations.

6-2 What three steps are basic to all our sensory systems?

Our senses (1) receive sensory stimulation (often using specialized receptor cells); (2) transform that stimulation into neural impulses; and (3) deliver the neural information to the brain. Transduction is the process of converting one form of energy into another. Researchers in psychophysics study the relationships between stimuli’s physical characteristics and our psychological experience of them.

6-3 How do absolute thresholds and difference thresholds differ, and what effect, if any, do stimuli below the absolute threshold have on us?

Our absolute threshold for any stimulus is the minimum stimulation necessary for us to be consciously aware of it 50 percent of the time. Signal detection theory predicts how and when we will detect a faint stimulus amid background noise. Individual absolute thresholds vary, depending on the strength of the signal and also on our experience, expectations, motivation, and alertness. Our difference threshold (also called just noticeable difference, or jnd) is the difference we can discern between two stimuli 50 percent of the time. Weber’s law states that two stimuli must differ by a constant minimum percentage (not a constant amount) to be perceived as different.
     Priming (the often unconscious activation of certain associations that may predispose one’s perception, memory, or response)shows that we process some information from stimuli below our absolute threshold for conscious awareness.

6-4 Does subliminal sensation enable subliminal persuasion?

Subliminal stimuli are those that are too weak to detect 50 percent of the time. While subliminal sensation is a fact, such sensations are too fleeting to enable exploitation with subliminal messages: There is no powerful, enduring effect.

6-5 What is the function of sensory adaptation?

Sensory adaptation (our diminished sensitivity to constant or routine odors, sounds, and touches) focuses our attention on informative changes in our environment.

6-6 How do our expectations, contexts, motivation, and emotions influence our perceptions?

Perceptual set is a mental predisposition that functions as a lens through which we perceive the world. Our learned concepts (schemas) prime us to organize and interpret ambiguous stimuli in certain ways. Our physical and emotional context, as well as our motivation, can create expectations and color our interpretation of events and behaviors.

TERMS AND CONCEPTS TO REMEMBER

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE Match each of the terms on the left with its definition on the right. Click on the term first and then click on the matching definition. As you match them correctly they will move to the bottom of the activity.

Question

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

Use to create your personalized study plan, which will direct you to the resources that will help you most in .

239