9.2 Language and Thought

Imagine an alien species that could pass thoughts from one head to another merely by pulsating air molecules in the space between them. Perhaps these weird creatures could inhabit a future science fiction movie?

language our spoken, written, or signed words and the ways we combine them to communicate meaning.

Actually, we are those creatures. When we speak, our brain and voice apparatus conjure up air pressure waves that we send banging against another’s eardrum—enabling us to transfer thoughts from our brain into theirs. As cognitive scientist Steven Pinker (1998) has noted, we sometimes sit for hours “listening to other people make noise as they exhale, because those hisses and squeaks contain information.” And thanks to all those funny sounds created in our heads from the air pressure waves, we get people’s attention. We get them to do things. We maintain relationships (Guerin, 2003). Depending on how you vibrate the air after opening your mouth, you may get a scowl or a kiss.

But language is more than vibrating air. As I [DM] create this paragraph, my fingers on a keyboard generate electronic binary numbers that are translated into the squiggles in front of you. When transmitted by reflected light rays into your retina, those squiggles trigger formless nerve impulses that project to several areas of your brain, which integrate the information, compare it to stored information, and decode meaning. Thanks to language, information is moving from my mind to yours. Monkeys mostly know what they see. Thanks to language (spoken, written, or signed), we comprehend much that we’ve never seen and that our distant ancestors never knew. Today, notes Daniel Gilbert (2006), the average taxi driver in Pittsburgh “knows more about the universe than did Galileo, Aristotle, Leonardo, or any of those other guys who were so smart they only needed one name.”

To Pinker (1990), language is “the jewel in the crown of cognition.” If you were able to retain only one cognitive ability, make it language, suggests researcher Lera Boroditsky (2009). Without sight or hearing, you could still have friends, family, and a job. But without language, could you have these things? “Language is so fundamental to our experience, so deeply a part of being human, that it’s hard to imagine life without it.”

Language transmits knowledge Whether spoken, written, or signed, language—the original wireless communication—enables mind-to-mind information transfer, and with it the transmission of civilization’s accumulated knowledge across generations.

Language Structure

9-8 What are the structural components of a language?

Consider how we might go about inventing a language. For a spoken language, we would need three building blocks:

phoneme in a language, the smallest distinctive sound unit.

morpheme in a language, the smallest unit that carries meaning; may be a word or a part of a word (such as a prefix).

grammar in a language, a system of rules that enables us to communicate with and understand others. In a given language, semantics is the set of rules for deriving meaning from sounds, and syntax is the set of rules for combining words into grammatically sensible sentences.

371

Like life constructed from the genetic code’s simple alphabet, language is complexity built of simplicity. In English, for example, 40 or so phonemes can be combined to form more than 100,000 morphemes, which alone or in combination produce the 616,500 word forms in the Oxford English Dictionary. Using those words, we can then create an infinite number of sentences, most of which (like this one) are original. I know that you can know why I worry that you think this sentence is starting to get too complex, but that complexity—and our capacity to communicate and comprehend it—is what distinguishes our human language capacity (Hauser et al., 2002; Premack, 2007).

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • How many morphemes are in the word cats? How many phonemes?

Two morphemes—cat and s, and four phonemes—c, a, t, and s

Language Development

9-9 What are the milestones in language development, and how do we acquire language?

Make a quick guess: How many words of your native language did you learn between your first birthday and your high school graduation? Although you use only 150 words for about half of what you say, you probably learned about 60,000 words (Bloom, 2000; McMurray, 2007). That averages (after age 2) to nearly 3500 words each year, or nearly 10 each day! How you did it—how those 3500 words could so far outnumber the roughly 200 words your schoolteachers consciously taught you each year—is one of the great human wonders.

Could you even state your language’s rules of syntax (the correct way to string words together to form sentences)? Most of us cannot. Yet before you were able to add 2 + 2, you were creating your own original and grammatically appropriate sentences. As a preschooler, you comprehended and spoke with a facility that puts to shame college students struggling to learn a foreign language.

We humans have an astonishing facility for language. With remarkable efficiency, we sample tens of thousands of words in our memory, effortlessly assemble them with near-perfect syntax, and spew them out, three words a second (Vigliocco & Hartsuiker, 2002). Seldom do we form sentences in our minds before speaking them. Rather we organize them on the fly as we speak. And while doing all this, we also adapt our utterances to our social and cultural context, following rules for speaking (How far apart should we stand?) and listening (Is it OK to interrupt?). Given how many ways there are to mess up, it’s amazing that we master this social dance. When and how does it happen?

When Do We Learn Language?

Receptive Language Children’s language development moves from simplicity to complexity. Infants start without language (in fantis means “not speaking”). Yet by 4 months of age, babies can recognize differences in speech sounds (Stager & Werker, 1997). They can also read lips: They prefer to look at a face that matches a sound, so we know they can recognize that ah comes from wide open lips and ee from a mouth with corners pulled back (Kuhl & Meltzoff, 1982). This marks the beginning of the development of babies’ receptive language, their ability to understand what is said to and about them. Infants’ language comprehension greatly outpaces their language production. Even at six months, long before speaking, many infants recognize object names (Ber-gelson & Swingley, 2012, 2013). At 7 months and beyond, babies grow in their power to do what you and I find difficult when listening to an unfamiliar language: to segment spoken sounds into individual words.

372

babbling stage beginning at about 4 months, the stage of speech development in which the infant spontaneously utters various sounds at first unrelated to the household language.

Productive Language Long after the beginnings of receptive language, babies’ productive language, their ability to produce words, matures. They recognize noun–verb differences—as shown by their responses to a misplaced noun or verb—earlier than they utter sentences with nouns and verbs (Bernal et al., 2010).

Before nurture molds babies’ speech, nature enables a wide range of possible sounds in the babbling stage, beginning at around 4 months. Many of these spontaneously uttered sounds are consonant-vowel pairs formed by simply bunching the tongue in the front of the mouth (da-da, na-na, ta-ta) or by opening and closing the lips (ma-ma), both of which babies do naturally for feeding (MacNeilage & Davis, 2000). Babbling does not imitate the adult speech babies hear—it includes sounds from various languages. From this early babbling, a listener could not identify an infant as being, say, French, Korean, or Ethiopian. Deaf infants who observe their deaf parents signing begin to babble more with their hands. (Petitto & Marentette, 1991)

one-word stage the stage in speech development, from about age 1 to 2, during which a child speaks mostly in single words.

By about 10 months old, infants’ babbling has changed so that a trained ear can identify the household language (de Boysson-Bardies et al., 1989). Without exposure to other languages, babies lose their ability to hear and produce sounds and tones found outside their native language (Meltzoff et al., 2009; Pallier et al., 2001). Thus, by adulthood, those who speak only English cannot discriminate certain sounds in Japanese speech. Nor can Japanese adults with no training in English hear the difference between the English r and l. For a Japanese-speaking adult, la-la-ra-ra may sound like the same syllable repeated. A Japanese-speaking person told that the train station is “just after the next light” may wonder, “The next what? After the street veering right, or farther down, after the light?”

two-word stage beginning about age 2, the stage in speech development during which a child speaks mostly in two-word statements.

Around their first birthday, most children enter the one-word stage. They have already learned that sounds carry meanings, and if repeatedly trained to associate, say, fish with a picture of a fish, 1-year-olds will look at a fish when a researcher says, “Fish, fish! Look at the fish!” (Schafer, 2005). They now begin to use sounds—usually only one barely recognizable syllable, such as ma or da—to communicate meaning. But family members learn to understand, and gradually the infant’s language conforms more to the family’s language. Across the world, baby’s first words are often nouns that label objects or people (Tardif et al., 2008). At this one-word stage, a single inflected word (“Doggy!”) may equal a sentence (“Look at the dog out there!”).

At about 18 months, children’s word learning explodes from about a word per week to a word per day. By their second birthday, most have entered the two-word stage (TABLE 9.1). They start uttering two-word sentences in telegraphic speech. Like yesterday’s telegrams that charged by the word (TERMS ACCEPTED. SEND MONEY), a 2-year-old’s speech contains mostly nouns and verbs (“Want juice”). Also like telegrams, it follows rules of syntax, arranging words in a sensible order. English-speaking children typically place adjectives before nouns—white house rather than house white. Spanish reverses this order, as in casa blanca.

TABLE 9.1
Summary of Language Development

telegraphic speech early speech stage in which a child speaks like a telegram—“go car”—using mostly nouns and verbs.

Moving out of the two-word stage, children quickly begin uttering longer phrases (Fromkin & Rodman, 1983). If they get a late start on learning a particular language, such as after receiving a cochlear implant or being adopted by a family in another country, their language development still proceeds through the same sequence, although usually at a faster pace (Ertmer et al., 2007; Snedeker et al., 2007). By early elementary school, children understand complex sentences and begin to enjoy the humor conveyed by double meanings: “You never starve in the desert because of all the sand-which-is there.”

373

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • What is the difference between receptive and productive language, and when do children normally hit these milestones in language development?

Infants normally start developing receptive language skills (ability to understand what is said to and about them) around 4 months of age. Then, starting with babbling at 4 months and beyond, infants normally start building productive language skills (ability to produce sounds and eventually words).

Creating a language Brought together as if on a desert island (actually a school), Nicaragua’s young deaf children over time drew upon sign gestures from home to create their own Nicaraguan Sign Language, complete with words and intricate grammar. Our biological predisposition for language does not create language in a vacuum. But activated by a social context, nature and nurture work creatively together (Osborne, 1999; Sandler et al., 2005; Senghas & Coppola, 2001).

Explaining Language Development

The world’s 6000+ or so languages are structurally very diverse (Evans & Levinson, 2009). Linguist Noam Chomsky has argued that all languages nonetheless share some basic elements, which he calls universal grammar. All human languages, for example, have nouns, verbs, and adjectives as grammatical building blocks. Moreover, said Chomsky, we humans are born with a built-in predisposition to learn grammar rules, which helps explain why preschoolers pick up language so readily and use grammar so well. It happens so naturally—as naturally as birds learn to fly—that training hardly helps.

We are not born with a built-in specific language, and languages are much more diverse than the universal grammar idea implies (Bergen, 2014; Ibbotson, 2012). Europeans and Native Australia–New Zealand populations, though geographically separated for 50,000 years, can readily learn each others’ languages (Chater et al., 2009). And whatever language we experience as children, whether spoken or signed, we all readily learn its specific grammar and vocabulary (Bavelier et al., 2003). Yet no matter what language we learn, we start speaking it mostly in nouns (kitty, da-da) rather than in verbs and adjectives (Bornstein et al., 2004). Biology and experience work together.

A natural talent Human infants come with a remarkable capacity to soak up language. But the particular language they learn will reflect their unique interactions with others.

Statistical Learning When adults listen to an unfamiliar language, the syllables all run together. A young Sudanese couple new to North America and unfamiliar with English might, for example, hear United Nations as “Uneye Tednay Shuns.” Their 7-month-old daughter would not have this problem. Human infants display a remarkable ability to learn statistical aspects of human speech (Aslin & Newport, 2012; Werker et al., 2012). Their brains not only discern word breaks, they statistically analyze which syllables, as in “hap-py-ba-by,” most often go together. After just two minutes of exposure to a computer voice speaking an unbroken, monotone string of nonsense syllables (bidakupadotigolabubidaku …), 8-month-old infants were able to recognize (as indicated by their attention) three-syllable sequences that appeared repeatedly (Saffran et al., 1996, 2009).

In further testimony to infants’ surprising knack for soaking up language, research shows that 7-month-olds can learn simple sentence structures. After repeatedly hearing syllable sequences that follow one rule (an ABA pattern, such as ga-ti-ga and li-na-li), infants listened longer to syllables in a different sequence (an ABB pattern, such as wo-fe-fe, rather than wo-fe-wo). Their detecting the difference between the two patterns supports the idea that babies come with a built-in readiness to learn grammatical rules (Marcus et al., 1999).

374

Critical Periods Could we train adults to perform this same feat of statistical analysis later in the human life span? Many researchers believe not. Childhood seems to represent a critical (or “sensitive”) period for mastering certain aspects of language before the language-learning window closes (Hernandez & Li, 2007). People who learn a second language as adults usually speak it with the accent of their native language, and they also have difficulty mastering the new grammar. In one experiment, Korean and Chinese immigrants considered 276 English sentences (“Yesterday the hunter shoots a deer”) and decided whether they were grammatically correct or incorrect (Johnson & Newport, 1991). All had been in the United States for approximately 10 years: Some had arrived in early childhood, others as adults. As FIGURE 9.9 reveals, those who learned their second language early learned it best. The older one is when moving to a new country, the harder it will be to learn its language and to absorb its culture (Cheung et al., 2011; Hakuta et al., 2003).

Figure 9.9
Our ability to learn a new language diminishes with age Ten years after coming to the United States, Asian immigrants took an English grammar test. Although there is no sharply defined critical period for second language learning, those who arrived before age 8 understood American English grammar as well as native speakers did. Those who arrived later did not. (Data from Johnson & Newport, 1991.)

The window on language learning closes gradually in early childhood. Later-than-usual exposure to language (at age 2 or 3) unleashes the idle language capacity of a child’s brain, producing a rush of language. But by about age 7, those who have not been exposed to either a spoken or a signed language gradually lose their ability to master any language.

“Childhood is the time for language, no doubt about it. Young children, the younger the better, are good at it; it is child’s play. It is a onetime gift to the species.”

Lewis Thomas, The Fragile Species, 1992

Deafness and Language Development

The impact of early experiences is evident in language learning in prelingually (before learning language) deaf5 children born to hearing-nonsigning parents. These children typically do not experience language during their early years. Natively deaf children who learn sign language after age 9 never learn it as well as those who lose their hearing at age 9 after learning a spoken language such as English. They also never learn English as well as other natively deaf children who learned sign in infancy (Mayberry et al., 2002). Those who learn to sign as teens or adults are like immigrants who learn English after childhood: They can master basic words and learn to order them, but they never become as fluent as native signers in producing and comprehending subtle grammatical differences (Newport, 1990). Moreover, the late learners show less right hemisphere brain activity in regions that are active as native signers read sign language (Newman et al., 2002). As a flower’s growth will be stunted without nourishment, so, too, children will typically become linguistically stunted if isolated from language during the critical period for its acquisition.

“Children can learn multiple languages without an accent and with good grammar, if they are exposed to the language before puberty. But after puberty, it’s very difficult to learn a second language so well. Similarly, when I first went to Japan, I was told not even to bother trying to bow, that there were something like a dozen different bows and I was always going to ‘bow with an accent’.”

Psychologist Stephen M. Kosslyn, “The World in the Brain,” 2008

More than 90 percent of all deaf children are born to hearing parents. Most of these parents want their children to experience their world of sound and talk. Cochlear implants enable this by converting sounds into electrical signals and stimulating the auditory nerve by means of electrodes threaded into the child’s cochlea. But if an implant is to help children become proficient in oral communication, parents cannot delay the surgery until their child reaches the age of consent. Giving cochlear implants to children is hotly debated. Deaf culture advocates object to giving implants to children who were deaf prelingually. The National Association of the Deaf, for example, argues that deafness is not a disability because native signers are not linguistically disabled. More than five decades ago, Gallaudet University linguist William Stokoe (1960) showed that sign is a complete language with its own grammar, syntax, and meanings.

375

Don’t means Don’t—no matter how you say it! Deaf children of deaf-signing parents and hearing children of hearing parents have much in common. They develop language skills at about the same rate, and they are equally effective at opposing parental wishes and demanding their way.

Deaf culture advocates sometimes further contend that deafness could as well be considered “vision enhancement” as “hearing impairment.” Close your eyes and immediately you, too, will notice your attention being drawn to your other senses. In one experiment, people who had spent 90 minutes sitting quietly blindfolded became more accurate in their location of sounds (Lewald, 2007). When kissing, lovers minimize distraction and increase sensitivity by closing their eyes.

People who lose one channel of sensation compensate with a slight enhancement of their other sensory abilities (Backman & Dixon, 1992; Levy & Langer, 1992). Blind musicians are more likely than sighted ones to develop perfect pitch (Hamilton, 2000). Blind people are also more accurate than sighted people at locating a sound source with one ear plugged (Gougoux et al., 2005; Lessard et al., 1998). And when reading Braille—requiring sensitive touch perception—the unused processing power of the visual cortex gets used (Amedi et al., 2003).

In deaf cats, brain areas normally used for hearing donate themselves to the visual system (Lomber et al., 2010). So, too, in people who have been deaf from birth: They exhibit enhanced attention to their peripheral vision (Bavelier et al., 2006). Their auditory cortex, starved for sensory input, remains largely intact but becomes responsive to touch and to visual input (Karns et al., 2012). Once repurposed, the auditory cortex becomes less available for hearing—which helps explain why cochlear implants are most effective when given before age 2 (Geers & Nicholas, 2013; Niparko et al., 2010).

Living in a Silent World Worldwide, 360 million people live with disabling hearing loss (WHO, 2013). Some are profoundly deaf; others (more men than women) have hearing loss (Agrawal et al., 2008). Some were deaf prelingually; others have known the hearing world. Some sign and identify with the language-based Deaf culture. Others, especially those who lost their hearing after speaking a language, are “oral” and converse with the hearing world by reading lips or reading written notes. Still others move between the two cultures.

The challenges of life without hearing may be greatest for children. Unable to communicate in customary ways, signing playmates may struggle to coordinate their play with speaking playmates. School achievement may also suffer; academic subjects are rooted in spoken languages. Adolescents may feel socially excluded, with a resulting low self-confidence. Children who grow up around other deaf people more often identify with Deaf culture and feel positive self-esteem. If raised in a signing household, whether by deaf or hearing parents, they also express higher self-esteem and feel more accepted (Bat-Chava, 1993, 1994).

Hearing improved

Adults whose hearing becomes impaired later in life also face challenges. When older people with hearing loss must expend effort to hear words, they have less remaining cognitive capacity available to remember and comprehend them (Wingfield et al., 2005). In several studies, people with hearing loss, especially those not wearing hearing aids, have reported feeling sadder, being less socially engaged, and more often experiencing others’ irritation (Chisolm et al., 2007; Fellinger et al., 2007; Kashubeck-West & Meyer, 2008; National Council on Aging, 1999). They also may experience a sort of shyness: “It’s almost universal among the deaf to want to cause hearing people as little fuss as possible,” observed Henry Kisor (1990, p. 244), a Chicago newspaper editor and columnist who lost his hearing at age 3. “We can be self-effacing and diffident to the point of invisibility. Sometimes this tendency can be crippling. I must fight it all the time.” Helen Keller, both blind and deaf, noted that “Blindness cuts people off from things. Deafness cuts people off from people.”

376

I [DM] understand. My mother, with whom we communicated by writing notes on an erasable “magic pad,” spent her last dozen years in an utterly silent world, largely withdrawn from the stress and strain of trying to interact with people outside a small circle of family and old friends. With my own hearing declining on a trajectory toward hers, I find myself sitting front and center at plays and meetings, seeking quiet corners in restaurants, and asking my wife to make necessary calls to friends whose accents differ from ours. I do benefit from cool technology (see www.hearingloop.org) that, at the press of a button, can transform my hearing aids into in-the-ear loudspeakers for the broadcast of phone, TV, and public address system sound. Yet I still experience frustration when, with or without hearing aids, I can’t hear the joke everyone else is guffawing over; when, after repeated tries, I just can’t catch that exasperated person’s question and can’t fake my way around it; when family members give up and say, “Oh, never mind” after trying three times to tell me something unimportant.

As she aged, my mother came to feel that seeking social interaction was simply not worth the effort. I share newspaper columnist Kisor’s belief that communication is worth the effort (p. 246): “So, … I will grit my teeth and plunge ahead.” To reach out, to connect, to communicate with others, even across a chasm of silence, is to affirm our humanity as social creatures.

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • What was the premise of researcher Noam Chomsky’s work in language development?

Chomsky maintained that all languages share a universal grammar, and humans are biologically predisposed to learn the grammar rules of language.

  • Why is it so difficult to learn a new language in adulthood?

Our brain’s critical period for language learning is in childhood, when we can absorb language structure almost effortlessly. As we move past that stage in our brain’s development, our ability to learn a new language diminishes dramatically.

The Brain and Language

aphasia impairment of language, usually caused by left hemisphere damage either to Broca’s area (impairing speaking) or to Wernicke’s area (impairing understanding).

9-10 What brain areas are involved in language processing and speech?

Broca’s area controls language expression—an area of the frontal lobe, usually in the left hemisphere, that directs the muscle movements involved in speech.

We think of speaking and reading, or writing and reading, or singing and speaking as merely different examples of the same general ability—language. But consider this curious finding: Aphasia, an impairment of language, can result from damage to any of several cortical areas. Even more curious, some people with aphasia can speak fluently but cannot read (despite good vision), while others can comprehend what they read but cannot speak. Still others can write but not read, read but not write, read numbers but not letters, or sing but not speak. These cases suggest that language is complex, and that different brain areas must serve different language functions.

To review research on left and right hemisphere language processing—and to test your own speed in processing words presented to your left and right hemispheres—visit LaunchPad’s PsychSim 6: Dueling Hemispheres.

Wernicke’s area controls language reception—a brain area involved in language comprehension and expression; usually in the left temporal lobe.

Indeed, in 1865, French physician Paul Broca reported that after damage to an area of the left frontal lobe (later called Broca’s area) a person would struggle to speak words while still being able to sing familiar songs and comprehend speech.

In 1874, German investigator Carl Wernicke discovered that after damage to an area of the left temporal lobe (Wernicke’s area) people could speak only meaningless words. Asked to describe a picture that showed two boys stealing cookies behind a woman’s back, one patient responded: “Mother is away her working her work to get her better, but when she’s looking the two boys looking the other part. She’s working another time” (Geschwind, 1979). Damage to Wernicke’s area also disrupts understanding.

“It is the way systems interact and have a dynamic interdependence that is—unless one has lost all sense of wonder—quite awe-inspiring.”

Simon Conway Morris, “The Boyle Lecture,” 2005

Today’s neuroscience has confirmed brain activity in Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas during language processing (FIGURE 9.10). But language functions are distributed across other brain areas as well. Functional MRI scans show that different neural networks are activated by nouns and verbs (or objects and actions); by different vowels; and by reading stories of visual versus motor experiences (Shapiro et al., 2006; Speer et al., 2009). Different neural networks also enable one’s native language and a second language (Perani & Abutalebi, 2005).

Figure 9.10
Brain activity when speaking and hearing words

377

The big point to remember: In processing language, as in other forms of information processing, the brain operates by dividing its mental functions—speaking, perceiving, thinking, remembering—into subfunctions. Your conscious experience of reading this page seems indivisible, but you are engaging many different neural networks in your brain to compute each word’s form, sound, and meaning (Posner & Carr, 1992). Different brain areas also process information about who spoke and what was said (Perrachione et al., 2011). We saw this distributed processing in Chapter 6’s discussion of vision, for which the brain engages specialized subtasks, such as discerning color, depth, movement, and form. And in vision as in language, a localized trauma that destroys one of these neural work teams may cause people to lose just one aspect of processing. In visual processing, a stroke may destroy the ability to perceive movement but not color. In language processing, a stroke may impair the ability to speak distinctly without harming the ability to read. What we experience as a continuous, unified stream of experience is actually but the visible tip of a subdivided information-processing iceberg. E pluribus unum: Out of many, one.

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • ______________ ______________ is the part of the brain that, if damaged, might impair your ability to speak words. Damage to ______________ ______________ might impair your ability to understand language.

Broca’s area; Wernicke’s area

Do Other Species Have Language?

9-11 What do we know about other animals’ capacity for language?

Humans have long and proudly proclaimed that language sets us above all other animals. “When we study human language,” asserted linguist Noam Chomsky (1972), “we are approaching what some might call the ‘human essence,’ the qualities of mind that are, so far as we know, unique [to humans].” Let’s see if research on animal language supports claims that humans, alone, have language.

Animals display impressive comprehension and communication. Vervet monkeys sound different alarm cries for different predators: a barking call for a leopard, a cough for an eagle, and a chuttering for a snake. Hearing the leopard alarm, other vervets climb the nearest tree. Hearing the eagle alarm, they rush into the bushes. Hearing the snake chutter, they stand up and scan the ground (Byrne, 1991). To indicate such things as a type of threat—an eagle, leopard, falling tree, or neighboring group—monkeys will combine 6 different calls into a 25-call sequence (Balter, 2010). But is this language? This question launched many studies with chimpanzees.

Talking hands Human language appears to have evolved from gestured communications (Corballis, 2002, 2003; Pollick & de Waal, 2007). Even today, gestures are naturally associated with spontaneous speech, especially speech that has spatial content. Both gesture and speech communicate, and when they convey the same rather than different information (as they do in baseball’s sign language), we humans understand faster and more accurately (Hostetter, 2011; Kelly et al., 2010). Outfielder William Hoy, the first deaf player to join the major leagues (1892), reportedly helped invent hand signals for “Strike!” “Safe!” (shown here) and “Yerr out!” (Pollard, 1992). Referees in all sports now use invented signs, and fans are fluent in sports sign language.

In the late 1960s, psychologists Allen Gardner and Beatrix Gardner (1969) built on chimpanzees’ natural tendencies for gestured communication by teaching sign language to a young chimpanzee named Washoe. After four years, Washoe could use 132 signs; by her life’s end in 2007, she was using more than 245 signs (Metzler, 2011; Sanz et al., 1998). Washoe, for example, signed “You me go out, please.” Some word combinations seemed creative—saying water bird for “swan” or apple-which-is-orange for “orange” (Patterson, 1978; Rumbaugh, 1977). But some psychologists grew skeptical. Were the chimps language champs or were the researchers chumps? Consider, said the skeptics:

378

Comprehending canine Border collie Rico had a vocabulary of 200 human words. If asked to retrieve a toy with a name he had never heard, Rico would pick out a new toy from a group of familiar items (Kaminski et al., 2004). Hearing that name for the second time four weeks later, Rico more often than not would retrieve the same toy. Another border collie, Chaser, has set an animal record by learning 1022 object names (Pilley & Reid, 2011). Like a 3-year-old child, she can also categorize them by function and shape. She can “fetch a ball” or “fetch a doll.”

Controversy can stimulate progress, and in this case, it triggered more evidence of chimpanzees’ abilities to think and communicate. One surprising finding was that Washoe trained her adopted son Loulis to use the signs she had learned. After her second infant died, Washoe became withdrawn when told, “Baby dead, baby gone, baby finished.” Two weeks later, researcher-caretaker Roger Fouts (1992, 1997) signed better news: “I have baby for you.” Washoe reacted with instant excitement. Hair on end, she swaggered and panted while signing over and again, “Baby, my baby.” It took several hours for the foster mom and infant to warm to each other, but then Washoe broke the ice by signing, “Come baby” and cuddling Loulis. Without human assistance, Loulis eventually picked up 68 signs, simply by observing Washoe and three other language-trained chimps signing together.

Even more stunning was a report that Kanzi, a bonobo with a reported 384-word vocabulary, could understand syntax (rules of word order) in spoken English (Savage-Rumbaugh et al., 1993, 2009). Kanzi, who appears to have the receptive language ability of a human 2-year-old, has responded appropriately when asked, “Can you show me the light?” and “Can you bring me the [flash]light?” and “Can you turn the light on?” Given stuffed animals and asked—for the first time—to “make the dog bite the snake,” he put the snake to the dog’s mouth.

379

But is this language? Chimpanzees’ ability to express themselves in American Sign Language (ASL) raises questions about the very nature of language. Here, the trainer is asking, “What is this?” The sign in response is “Baby.” Does the response constitute language?

So, how should we interpret these studies? Are humans the only language-using species? If by language we mean verbal or signed expression of complex grammar, most psychologists would now agree that humans alone possess language. If we mean, more simply, an ability to communicate through a meaningful sequence of symbols, then apes are indeed capable of language.

One thing is certain: Studies of animal language and thinking have moved psychologists toward a greater appreciation of other species, not only for our common traits but also for their own remarkable abilities. In the past, many psychologists doubted that other species could plan, form concepts, count, use tools, show compassion, or use language (Thorpe, 1974). Today, thanks to animal researchers, we know better. It’s true that humans alone are capable of complex sentences. Moreover, 2½-year-old children display some cognitive abilities, such as following an actor’s gaze to a target, that are unmatched even by chimpanzees (Herrmann et al., 2010). Humans, alone, also have a version of a gene (FOXP2) that helps enable the lip, tongue, and vocal cord movements of human speech (Lieberman, 2013). Humans with a mutated form of this gene have difficulty speaking words.

Nevertheless, other species do exhibit insight, show family loyalty, communicate with one another, care for one another, and transmit cultural patterns across generations. Working out what this means for the moral rights of other animals is an unfinished task.

For examples of intelligent communication and problem solving among orangutans, elephants, and killer whales, watch LaunchPad’s 6-minute Video: How Intelligent Are Animals?

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • If your dog barks at a stranger at the front door, does this qualify as language? What if the dog yips in a telltale way to let you know she needs to go out?

These are definitely communications. But if language consists of words and the grammatical rules we use to combine them to communicate meaning, few scientists would label a dog’s barking and yipping as language.

Thinking and Language

9-12 What is the relationship between thinking and language, and what is the value of thinking in images?

Thinking and language intricately intertwine. Asking which comes first is one of psychology’s chicken-and-egg questions. Do our ideas come first and then the words to name them? Or are our thoughts conceived in words and unthinkable without them?

Language Influences Thinking

linguistic determinism Whorf’s hypothesis that language determines the way we think.

Linguist Benjamin Lee Whorf (1956) contended that “language itself shapes a [person’s] basic ideas.” The Hopi, who have no past tense for their verbs, could not readily think about the past, said Whorf.

Whorf’s linguistic determinism hypothesis is too extreme. We all think about things for which we have no words. (Can you think of a shade of blue you cannot name?) And we routinely have unsymbolized (wordless, imageless) thoughts, as when someone, while watching two men carry a load of bricks, wondered whether the men would drop them (Heavey & Hurlburt, 2008; Hurlburt et al., 2013).

Nevertheless, to those who speak two dissimilar languages, such as English and Japanese, it seems obvious that a person may think differently in different languages (Brown, 1986). Unlike English, which has a rich vocabulary for self-focused emotions such as anger, Japanese has more words for interpersonal emotions such as sympathy (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Many bilingual individuals report that they have different senses of self, depending on which language they are using (Matsumoto, 1994). In one series of studies with bilingual Israeli Arabs (who spoke both Arabic and Hebrew), participants thought differently about their social world, with differing automatic associations with Arabs and Jews, depending on which language the testing session used (Danziger & Ward, 2010).

380

Culture and color In Papua New Guinea, Berinmo children have words for different shades of “yellow,” which might enable them to spot and recall yellow variations more quickly. Here and everywhere, “the languages we speak profoundly shape the way we think, the way we see the world, the way we live our lives,” notes psychologist Lera Boroditsky (2009).

Depending on which emotion they want to express, bilingual parents will often switch languages. “When my mom gets angry at me, she’ll speak in Mandarin,” explained one Chinese-American student. “If she’s really mad, she’ll switch to Cantonese” (Chen et al., 2012). Bilingual individuals may even reveal different personality profiles when taking the same test in two languages, with their differing cultural associations (Chen & Bond, 2010; Dinges & Hull, 1992). This happened when China-born, bilingual University of Waterloo students were asked to describe themselves in English or Chinese (Ross et al., 2002). The English-language self-descriptions fit typical Canadian profiles, expressing mostly positive self-statements and moods. Responding in Chinese, the same students gave typically Chinese self-descriptions, reporting more agreement with Chinese values and roughly equal positive and negative self-statements and moods. Similar personality changes have been shown when bicultural, bilingual Americans and Mexicans shifted between the cultural frames associated with English and Spanish (Ramírez-Esparza et al., 2006). “Learn a new language and get a new soul,” says a Czech proverb.

So our words may not determine what we think, but they do influence our thinking (Boroditsky, 2011). We use our language in forming categories. In Brazil, the isolated Piraha people have words for the numbers 1 and 2, but numbers above that are simply “many.” Thus, if shown 7 nuts in a row, they find it difficult to lay out the same number from their own pile (Gordon, 2004).

Words also influence our thinking about colors. Whether we live in New Mexico, New South Wales, or New Guinea, we see colors much the same, but we use our native language to classify and remember them (Davidoff, 2004; Roberson et al., 2004, 2005). Imagine viewing three colors and calling two of them “yellow” and one of them “blue.” Later you would likely see and recall the yellows as being more similar. But if you speak the language of Papua New Guinea’s Berinmo tribe, which has words for two different shades of yellow, you would more speedily perceive and better recall the variations between the two yellows. And if your language is Russian, which has distinct names for various shades of blue, such as goluboy and siniy, you might recall the yellows as more similar and remember the blues better. Words matter.

Perceived differences grow as we assign different names. On the color spectrum, blue blends into green—until we draw a dividing line between the portions we call “blue” and “green.” Although equally different on the color spectrum, two different items that share the same color name (as the two “blues” do in FIGURE 9.11, contrast B) are harder to distinguish than two items with different names (“blue” and “green,” as in Figure 9.11, contrast A) (Özgen, 2004). Likewise, two places seem closer and more vulnerable to the same natural disaster if labeled as in the same state rather than at an equal distance in adjacent states (Burris & Branscombe, 2005; Mishra & Mishra, 2010). Tornadoes don’t know about state lines, but people do.

Figure 9.11
Language and perception When people view blocks of equally different colors, they perceive those with different names as more different. Thus the “green” and “blue” in contrast A may appear to differ more than the two equally different blues in contrast B (Özgen, 2004).

Given words’ subtle influence on thinking, we do well to choose our words carefully. Is “A child learns language as he interacts with his caregivers” any different from “Children learn language as they interact with their caregivers”? Many studies have found that it is. When hearing the generic he (as in “the artist and his work”) people are more likely to picture a male (Henley, 1989; Ng, 1990). If he and his were truly gender free, we shouldn’t skip a beat when hearing that “man, like other mammals, nurses his young.”

“All words are pegs to hang ideas on.”

Henry Ward Beecher, Proverbs from Plymouth Pulpit, 1887

To expand language is to expand the ability to think. Children’s thinking develops hand in hand with their language (Gopnik & Meltzoff, 1986). Indeed, it is very difficult to think about or conceptualize certain abstract ideas (commitment, freedom, or rhyming) without language! And what is true for preschoolers is true for everyone: It pays to increase your word power. That’s why most textbooks, including this one, introduce new words—to teach new ideas and new ways of thinking. And that’s also why psychologist Steven Pinker (2007) titled his book on language The Stuff of Thought.

381

Increased word power helps explain what McGill University researcher Wallace Lambert (1992; Lambert et al., 1993) has called the bilingual advantage. Bilingual people are skilled at inhibiting one language while using the other. And thanks to their well-practiced “executive control” over language, they also are better at inhibiting their attention to irrelevant information (Kroll & Bialystok, 2013). This superior attentional control is evident from 7 months of age into adulthood and even helps protect against cognitive decline in later life (Bak et al., 2014; Bialystok et al., 2012; Kroll et al., 2014).


To consider how researchers have learned about the benefits of learning more than one language, visit LaunchPad’s How Would You Know If There is a Bilingual Advantage?

Lambert helped devise a Canadian program that immerses English-speaking children in French. (The number of non-Quebec children enrolled rose from 65,000 in 1981 to over 328,000 in 2010 [Statistics Canada, 2013].) Not surprisingly, the children attain a natural French fluency unrivaled by other methods of language teaching. Moreover, compared with similarly capable children in control groups, they do so without detriment to their English fluency, and with increased aptitude scores, creativity, and appreciation for French-Canadian culture (Genesee & Gándara, 1999; Lazaruk, 2007).

Whether we are in the linguistic minority or majority, language links us to one another. Language also connects us to the past and the future. “To destroy a people, destroy their language,” observed poet Joy Harjo.

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • Benjamin Lee Whorf’s controversial hypothesis, called ______________ ______________, suggested that we cannot think about things unless we have words for those concepts or ideas.

linguistic determinism

Thinking in Images

When you are alone, do you talk to yourself? Is “thinking” simply conversing with yourself? Without a doubt, words convey ideas. But sometimes ideas precede words. To turn on the cold water in your bathroom, in which direction do you turn the handle? To answer, you probably thought not in words but with implicit (nondeclarative, procedural) memory—a mental picture of how you do it (see Chapter 8).

“When we see a person walking down the street talking to himself, we generally assume that he is mentally ill. But we all talk to ourselves continuously—we just have the good sense of keeping our mouths shut…. It’s as though we are having a conversation with an imaginary friend possessed of infinite patience. Who are we talking to?”

Sam Harris, “We Are Lost in Thought,” 2011

Indeed, we often think in images. Artists think in images. So do composers, poets, mathematicians, athletes, and scientists. Albert Einstein reported that he achieved some of his greatest insights through visual images and later put them into words. Pianist Liu Chi Kung harnessed the power of thinking in images. One year after placing second in the 1958 Tschaikovsky piano competition, Liu was imprisoned during China’s cultural revolution. Soon after his release, after seven years without touching a piano, he was back on tour. Critics judged Liu’s musicianship as better than ever. How did he continue to develop without practice? “I did practice,” said Liu, “every day. I rehearsed every piece I had ever played, note by note, in my mind” (Garfield, 1986).

For someone who has learned a skill, such as ballet dancing, even watching the activity will activate the brain’s internal simulation of it, reported one British research team after collecting fMRIs as people watched videos (Calvo-Merino et al., 2004). So, too, will imagining a physical experience, which activates some of the same neural networks that are active during the actual experience (Grèzes & Decety, 2001). Small wonder, then, that mental practice has become a standard part of training for Olympic athletes (Suinn, 1997; Ungerleider, 2005).

One experiment on mental practice and basketball free-throw shooting tracked the University of Tennessee women’s team over 35 games (Savoy & Beitel, 1996). During that time, the team’s free-throw accuracy increased from approximately 52 percent in games following standard physical practice to some 65 percent after mental practice. Players had repeatedly imagined making free throws under various conditions, including being “trash-talked” by their opposition. In a dramatic conclusion, Tennessee won the national championship game in overtime, thanks in part to their free-throw shooting.

382

Mental rehearsal can also help you achieve an academic goal, as researchers demonstrated with two groups of introductory psychology students facing a midterm exam one week later (Taylor et al., 1998). (Scores of other students, not engaging in any mental simulation, formed a control group.) The first group spent five minutes each day visualizing themselves scanning the posted grade list, seeing their A, beaming with joy, and feeling proud. This outcome simulation had little effect, adding only 2 points to their exam-score average. Another group spent five minutes each day visualizing themselves effectively studying—reading the textbook, going over notes, eliminating distractions, declining an offer to go out. This process simulation paid off: This second group began studying sooner, spent more time at it, and beat the others’ average by 8 points. The point to remember: It’s better to spend your fantasy time planning how to get somewhere than to dwell on the imagined destination.

To experience your own thinking as (a) manipulating words and (b) manipulating images, visit LaunchPad’s PsychSim 6: My Head Is Spinning!

***

What, then, should we say about the relationship between thinking and language? As we have seen, language influences our thinking. But if thinking did not also affect language, there would never be any new words. And new words and new combinations of old words express new ideas. The basketball term slam dunk was coined after the act itself had become fairly common. Blogs became part of our language after web logs appeared. So, let us say that thinking affects our language, which then affects our thought (FIGURE 9.12).

Figure 9.12
The interplay of thought and language The traffic runs both ways between thinking and language. Thinking affects our language, which affects our thought.

What time is it now? When we asked you (in the section on overconfidence) to estimate how quickly you would finish this chapter, did you underestimate or overestimate?

Psychological research on thinking and language mirrors the mixed impressions of our species by those in fields such as literature and religion. The human mind is simultaneously capable of striking intellectual failures and of striking intellectual power. Misjudgments are common and can have disastrous consequences. So we do well to appreciate our capacity for error. Yet our efficient heuristics often serve us well. Moreover, our ingenuity at problem solving and our extraordinary power of language mark humankind as almost “infinite in faculties.”

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE

  • What is mental practice, and how can it help you to prepare for an upcoming event?

Mental practice uses visual imagery to mentally rehearse future behaviors, activating some of the same brain areas used during the actual behaviors. Visualizing the details of the process is more effective than visualizing only your end goal.

383

REVIEW: Language and Thought

REVIEW Language and Thought

LEARNING OBJECTIVES

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE Take a moment to answer each of these Learning Objective Questions (repeated here from within this section). Then click the 'show answer' button to check your answers. Research suggests that trying to answer these questions on your own will improve your long-term retention (McDaniel et al., 2009).

9-8 What are the structural components of a language?

Phonemes are a language’s basic units of sound. Morphemes are the elementary units of meaning. Grammar—the system of rules that enables us to communicate—includes semantics (rules for deriving meaning) and syntax (rules for ordering words into sentences).

9-9 What are the milestones in language development, and how do we acqiure language?

Language development’s timing varies, but all children follow the same sequence. Receptive language (the ability to understand what is said to or about you) develops before productive language (the ability to produce words). At about 4 months of age, infants babble, making sounds found in languages from all over the world. By about 10 months, their babbling contains only the sounds found in their household language. Around 12 months of age, children begin to speak in single words. This one-word stage evolves into two-word (telegraphic) utterances before their second birthday, after which they begin speaking in full sentences.
     Linguist Noam Chomsky has proposed that all human languages share a universal grammar—the basic building blocks of language—and that humans are born with a predisposition to learn language. We acquire a specific language through learning as our biology and experience interact. Childhood is a critical period for learning to speak and/or sign fluently. This is an important consideration for parents of deaf children, who might master oral communication if given a cochlear implant during this critical period. Deaf culture advocates oppose such implants on the grounds that deafness is a difference, not a disability.

9-10 What brain areas are involved in language processing and speech?

Aphasia is an impairment of language, usually caused by left-hemisphere damage. Two important language- and speech-processing areas are Broca’s area, a region of the frontal lobe that controls language expression, and Wernicke’s area, a region in the left temporal lobe that controls language reception. Language processing is spread across other brain areas as well, where different neural networks handle specific linguistic subtasks.

9-11 What do we know about other animals’ capacity for language?

A number of chimpanzees and bonobos have (1) learned to communicate with humans by signing or by pushing buttons wired to a computer, (2) developed vocabularies of nearly 400 words, (3) communicated by stringing these words together, (4) taught their skills to younger animals, and (5) demonstrated some understanding of syntax. But only humans communicate in complex sentences. Nevertheless, other animals’ impressive abilities to think and communicate challenge humans to consider what this means about the moral rights of other species.

9-12 What is the relationship between thinking and language, and what is the value of thinking in images?

Although Benjamin Lee Whorf’s linguistic determinism hypothesis suggested that language determines thought, it is in fact more accurate to say that language influences thought. Different languages embody different ways of thinking, and immersion in bilingual education can enhance thinking. We often think in images when we use implicit (nondeclarative, procedural) memory—our automatic memory system for motor and cognitive skills and classically conditioned associations. Thinking in images can increase our skills when we mentally practice upcoming events.

TERMS AND CONCEPTS TO REMEMBER

RETRIEVAL PRACTICE Match each of the terms on the left with its definition on the right. Click on the term first and then click on the matching definition. As you match them correctly they will move to the bottom of the activity.

Question

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

Use to create your personalized study plan, which will direct you to the resources that will help you most in .

TEST
YOUR-
SELF THINKING AND LANGUAGE

Test yourself repeatedly throughout your studies. This will not only help you figure out what you know and don’t know; the testing itself will help you learn and remember the information more effectively thanks to the testing effect.

Thinking

Question

1. A mental grouping of similar things is called a lXW26UHd+L/b3OE8 .

Correct!
Wrong. Please see correct answer displayed.

Question

2. The most systematic procedure for solving a problem is a(n) VEmO1ZNTFkbeEDDNa6WJdw== .

Correct!
Wrong. Please see correct answer displayed.

Question

k4hv1HaGzT/3Emsv/H6uMKa0d7kPsIXwOwwEwHMNmEWOO6dYzgRJT+pwnn+0oCzr6hSq7DU8JHXk7qoBmtN4JFyKemLrhZur7z4W+xBRBQkfD6Md1mYVTPuT4Yh0fPdscHv/YdXIYIKMt/m3LAc5sQTU/IFKRMSBEuxp57rEw60dyywjllvVL3YZYm+eplZgApLbqPadhemKIY/Khi6/jaESrpPjE6Bqc22bhUuSRQdy4FtyVL01kuZnVtkxz6bG/58M8zNzvKIauGBMQxQT3q/OqfUK2QoTJ0GZjnpDtxbISZLIQ2yhh20Qai4WaPnFKRaMgQ==
Answer:
Oscar will need to guard against confirmation bias (searching for support for his own views and ignoring contradictory evidence) as he seeks out opposing viewpoints. Even if Oscar encounters new information that disproves his beliefs, belief perseverance may lead him to cling to these views anyway. It will take more compelling evidence to change his beliefs than it took to create them.

Question

7cNc/LsH9xb+CwSocM3XuFuE/e0Y/C3lzkYavg/FoGIhNEAcHTocArEKd3RbmJZkfYuj92Ibb8+bWE8Ym522Ar41LIcTx/MeaEh81vK20jY1vBuMmhUgg7mfKkIQWNppV+huHc0zlkN72FOaMle+qeuJN8IfghVpvlBcCaNmATtB8eD38NkRxvZKQSmdiDskPePjmwDz1GmodWObTlp9WX6Pq+QStHwAL3xmHAIMp97JF0W/wEdZ6YLnMIQiCt1FqOdK8WczE7pt7Te3xWBelxKlUE0MEom4KaoNy+DZuLVQsvkHrjGmGtFzWnnSEfcB1j6RYoiDhUKb1SEtXQzw7KXwQSXRb0nZSlCnIPefDk4ddPqNs6tD5OwSK3U/gh1SQMDM6G+8eEEkbBr4ujl6TNHhNaNoVNO2DtnEyUooAq0k/a138y9IsytJT+0+1RoEs9XrDer+wvAd+aFre9GDEQ==
3
Correct!
Sorry, wrong answer.
Maximum tries = 3

Question

5. Widely reported terrorist attacks, such as on 9/11 in the United States, led some observers to initially assume in 2014 that the missing Malaysian Airlines Flight 370 was probably also the work of terrorists. This assumption illustrates the s5k4DLMto8HSe1bdwjvXArOBDJo= heuristic.

Correct!
Wrong. Please see correct answer displayed.

Question

6. When consumers respond more positively to ground beef described as “75 percent lean” than to the same product labeled “25 percent fat,” they have been influenced by 2LunrYbNVoUXnlgu .

Correct!
Wrong. Please see correct answer displayed.

Question

qgkGEQkyBRCl0znhjSY0k5XpDAzK52C4pR3VKiHe5Sbt0Co8x7zCrjkjsPnJUeE+UkwZ2sn8rFxTZHSAw79tsVE0frKrrURqeEsbyzLnYRoPxjiG8OeplSEChv7VsgF/0SHHWVlOeMt1ySEEOrBsC9z9LO8qy0OOoBtGi6yrPEHdV4Ej5I2jMEUTIcB8dCmcxY2R0DTDmk9cAhj6aGsbymkGptDJV95coOnV0HSFPsCds1Zql+GpkP9dwNRBWKkhFE17Cfi3jqOfs3yCOxfBRsuo9uA=
3
Correct!
Sorry, wrong answer.
Maximum tries = 3

Language and Thought

Question

37HDauVGfFHva5jWiHW/RkjYpbsPBykDHyRdu01iyxIvK5J2McUpBkMvY8NsflKB/WgPwnmpGEZJ662cxuvVfkdDl9qdSExFwHhxDj3ALoE2NSXJCtKqppCvQwKu0AX8UfJ6ltTkJevs2lhPXXL6SCv+h+I+QxdjMxuFRmHxS4fQROcSHI9oNa0JlzjqYX5WS6BKzrKI2+GBo5PyTYRYOQ==
3
Correct!
Sorry, wrong answer.
Maximum tries = 3

Question

9. The three basic building blocks of language are yjsMyijh2P5iwyOLWHIuqQ==zfPJ/HczkMRwdXhS4HNZqQ==qPw1HbA8DkNvw7id .

Correct!
Wrong. Please see correct answer displayed.

Question

10. When young children speak in short phrases using mostly verbs and nouns, this is referred to as +0SmTxmKASUAj+Cosp+8+Q==  wH7Q7t4jasqp59Mv .

Correct!
Wrong. Please see correct answer displayed.

Question

11. According to Chomsky, all languages share a(n) NARVcbkaiHSDHSkbkN9iiw==  qPw1HbA8DkNvw7id .

Correct!
Wrong. Please see correct answer displayed.

Question

WO+zoXZhAF9lZecv/Oafnbwb0LH/Xh8buF4Dh0D/9OZaBd83hfVO5zheAKcBxZDFxjrcF64jJlcD8v2NExxwRxgmp/cBnnaZItN4z5aXMRm/tSxbk1/+ONu7MU10KJOS3eXpMkuYArnRtQn+uM5lz5QbAc4+xL0tumnudgzersTmMRDhAHf0Nf+I/X2QG9ydLDMJyrJZdb4DCPKlC9qpufm3xAT2QYRiEQfqtbn7k5vPsldvlxip9/Oo90Rx0o17P3lSAuqyz7q7pNGtq0Ra2HHxQUvTmi4fgIrzrewTIBUkXUBhngFym9ibtVQHQN9t
3
Correct!
Sorry, wrong answer.
Maximum tries = 3