12.2 Social Influence: Controlling People

Malala Yousafzai, a 15-year-old Pakistani girl who stood up for women’s rights despite being hunted by the Taliban, was named one of the world’s 100 most influential people by Time magazine in both 2013 and 2014.
Wang Lei/Xinhua/Landov

Those of us who grew up watching superhero cartoons have usually thought a bit about which of the standard superpowers we’d most like to have. Super strength and super speed have obvious benefits, invisibility could be interesting as well as lucrative, and there’s always a lot to be said for flying. But when it comes right down to it, the ability to control other people would probably be the most useful. After all, who needs to change the course of mighty rivers or bend steel in his bare hands if someone else can be convinced to do it for them? The things we want from life—gourmet food, interesting jobs, big houses, fancy cars—can often be given to us by others, and the things we want most—loving families, loyal friends, admiring children, appreciative employers—cannot be had any other way. Getting others to do what we want them to do would be the ultimate super power.

Luckily, you’ve got it! Social influence is the control of one person’s behavior by another (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004) and it happens all the time. Every one of us influences other people, and every one of us is influenced by them. The techniques we use are numerous and varied, but in the end they all rely on the fact that human beings have three basic motivations: to experience pleasure and to avoid experiencing pain (the hedonic motive), to be accepted and to avoid being rejected (the approval motive), and to believe what is right and to avoid believing what is wrong (the accuracy motive; Bargh, Gollwitzer, & Oettingen, 2010; Fiske, 2010). As you will see, most attempts at social influence appeal to one or more of these three motivations.

social influence

The control of one person’s behavior by another.

The Hedonic Motive: Pleasure Is Better Than Pain

Pleasure seeking is the most basic of all motives, and social influence often involves creating situations in which others can achieve more pleasure by doing what we want them to do than by doing something else. Parents, teachers, governments, and businesses influence our behavior by offering rewards and threatening punishments (see FIGURE 12.7). There’s nothing mysterious about how these influence attempts work, and they are often quite effective. When the Republic of Singapore warned its citizens that anyone caught chewing gum in public would face a year in prison and a $5,500 fine, the rest of the world was outraged; but when the outrage subsided, it was hard to ignore the fact that gum chewing in Singapore had fallen to an all-time low. A good caning will get your attention every time.

Figure 12.7: FIGURE 12.7 The Cost of Speeding The penalty for speeding in Massachusetts used to be a modest fine. In 2006, the law changed so that drivers under 18 who are caught speeding now lose their licenses for 90 days—and to get them back, they have to pay $500, attend 8 hours of training classes, and retake the state’s driving exam. Guess what? Deaths among drivers under 18 fell by 38% in just 3 years. In other words, more than 8,000 young lives were saved by appealing to the hedonic motive. (Data from Moskowitz, 2010.)
© Journal-Courier/Clayton Stalter/The Image Works

394

Question 12.5

Why do rewards and punishments sometimes backfire?

You’ll recall from the Learning chapter that even a sea slug will repeat behaviors that are followed by rewards and avoid behaviors that are followed by punishments. Although the same is generally true of human beings, there are some instances in which rewards and punishments can backfire. For example, children in one study were allowed to draw with colored markers. Some were given a “Good Player Award” (the “rewarded” children) and some were not (the “unrewarded” children). The next day, all of the children were again given markers, but this time no awards were offered. The results showed that the previously rewarded children drew with the markers less than the previously unrewarded children did (Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973). Why? Because previously rewarded children had come to think of drawing as something one does to get a reward—and since they weren’t going to receive a reward the second day, then why the heck should they draw a picture? (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999)? Rewards and punishments can also backfire because people resent being manipulated. Researchers placed signs in two restrooms on a college campus: “Please don’t write on these walls” and “Do not write on these walls under any circumstances.” Two weeks later, the walls in the second restroom had more graffiti on the walls, presumably because students didn’t appreciate the threatening tone of the second sign and wrote on the walls just to prove that they could (Pennebaker & Sanders, 1976).

Why would the ability to control others be the ultimate superpower? In a 2010 survey that asked Americans to identify the things that annoyed them most, 19 of the top 20 annoyances were caused by other people. The remaining annoyance was caused by other people’s dogs. (Data from Consumer Reports Magazine, 2010.)

The Approval Motive: Acceptance Is Better Than Rejection

Other people stand between us and starvation, predation, loneliness, and all the other things that make getting shipwrecked such an unpopular pastime. We depend on others for safety, sustenance, and solidarity, and so we are powerfully motivated to have others like us, accept us, and approve of us (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Leary, 2010). Like the hedonic motive, the approval motive can be a lever for social influence.

395

Culture & Community: Free parking.

Free parking. People don’t like to be manipulated, and they get upset when someone threatens their freedom. Is this a uniquely Western reaction? To find out, psychologists asked college students for one of two favors and then measured how irritated the students felt (Jonas et al., 2009). In one case, the psychologists asked students if they would give up their right to park on campus for a week (“Would you mind if I used your parking card so I can participate in a research project in this building?”). In the other case, the psychologists asked students if they would give up everyone’s right to park on campus for a week (“Would you mind if we closed the entire parking lot for a tennis tournament?”). How did students react to these requests?

Data from Jonas et al., 2009.
Fuse/JupiterImages

It depended on culture. As the figure shows, European American students were more irritated by a request that limited their own freedom than by a request that limited everyone’s freedom (“If nobody can park, that’s inconvenient. But if everybody except me can park, that’s unfair!”). But Latino and Asian American students had precisely the opposite reactions (“The needs of the requestor outweigh the needs of one student, but they don’t outweigh the needs of all students”). It appears that all people value freedom—just not necessarily their own.

Normative Influence

When you get on an elevator, you are supposed to face forward and not talk to the person next to you even if you were talking to that person before you got on the elevator unless you are the only two people on the elevator in which case it’s okay to talk and face sideways but still not backward. Although no one ever taught you this particularly long rule of elevator etiquette, you probably picked it up somewhere along the way. The unwritten rules that govern social behavior are called norms, which are customary standards for behavior that are widely shared by members of a culture (Cialdini, 2013; Miller & Prentice, 1996).

norms

Customary standards for behavior that are widely shared by members of a culture.

How are we influenced by other people’s behavior?

We learn norms with exceptional ease and we obey them with exceptional fidelity because we know that if we don’t, others won’t approve of us. For example, every human culture has a norm of reciprocity, which is the unwritten rule that people should benefit those who have benefited them (Gouldner, 1960). When a friend buys you lunch, you must eventually return the favor; and if you don’t, your friend will eventually get a new one. Indeed, the norm of reciprocity is so strong that when researchers randomly pulled the names of strangers from a telephone directory and sent them all Christmas cards, they received Christmas cards back from most (Kunz & Woolcott, 1976).

norm of reciprocity

The unwritten rule that people should benefit those who have benefited them.

Norms can be a powerful tool for social influence. Normative influence is a phenomenon that occurs when another person’s behavior provides information about what is appropriate (see FIGURE 12.8). For example, waiters and waitresses know all about the norm of reciprocity, and so they often give customers a piece of candy along with the bill, hoping that customers who receive a free candy will feel obligated to do “a little extra” for the server who did “a little extra” for them. Research shows that this little trick works quite nicely (Strohmetz et al., 2002).

normative influence

A phenomenon that occurs when another person’s behavior provides information about what is appropriate.

Figure 12.8: FIGURE 12.8 The Perils of Connection Other people’s behavior defines what is normal, which is one of the reasons why obesity spreads through social networks (Christakis & Fowler, 2007).
© Francis Dean/Dean pictures/The Image Works

396

Conformity

People can influence us by invoking familiar norms, such as the norm of reciprocity. But if you’ve ever found yourself in a fancy restaurant, sneaking a peek at the person next to you in the hopes of discovering whether the little fork is meant to be used for shrimp or salad, then you know that other people can also influence us by defining new norms in ambiguous, confusing, or novel situations. Conformity is the tendency to do what others do simply because others are doing it, and it results in part from normative influence.

conformity

The tendency to do what others do simply because others are doing it.

Why do we do what we see other people doing?

Have you ever wondered which big spender left the bill as a tip? In fact, the bills are often put there by the very people you are tipping because they know that the presence of paper money will suggest to you that others are leaving big tips and that it would be socially appropriate for you to do the same. By the way, the customary gratuity for someone who writes a textbook for you is 15%. But most students send us more.
Don Paulson Photography/Purestock/Superstock
Figure 12.9: FIGURE 12.9 Asch’s Conformity Study If you were asked which of the lines on the right (A, B, or C) matches the standard line on the left, what would you say? Research on conformity suggests that your answer would depend, in part, on how other people in the room answered the same question.

In a classic study, male participants sat in a room with seven other men who appeared to be ordinary participants but who were actually trained actors (Asch, 1951, 1956). An experimenter explained that the participants would be shown cards with three printed lines and that his job was simply to say which of the three lines matched a “standard line” that was printed on another card (see FIGURE 12.9). The experimenter held up a card and then asked each man to answer in turn. The real participant was among the last to be called on. Everything went well on the first two trials, but then on the third trial something really strange happened: The actors all began giving the same wrong answer! What did the real participants do? Seventy-five percent of them conformed and announced the wrong answer on at least one trial. Subsequent research has shown that these participants didn’t actually misperceive the length of the lines, but were instead succumbing to normative influence (Asch, 1955; Nemeth & Chiles, 1988). Giving the wrong answer was apparently “the right thing to do,” and so participants did it. The behavior of others can tell us what is proper, appropriate, expected, and accepted—in other words, it can define a norm—and once a norm is defined, we feel obliged to honor it. This fact can be put to good use. When the Sacramento Municipal Utility District randomly selected 35,000 customers and sent them electric bills showing how their energy consumption compared to that of their neighbors, consumption fell by 2% (Kaufman, 2009).

A perplexed participant (center) is flanked by actors who have just given the wrong answer in one of Solomon Asch’s class conformity experiments.
© William Vandivert

397

Other Voices: 91% of All Students Read This Box and Love It

91% of All Students Read This Box and Love It

Tina Rosenberg is an editorial writer for the New York Times. Her 1995 book The Haunted Land: Facing Europe’s Ghosts After Communism won both the Pulitzer Prize and the National Book Award.
Noah Greenberg photography

Binge drinking is a problem on college America (Wechsler & Nelson, 2001). About half of all students report doing it, and those who do are much more likely to miss classes, get behind in their school work, drive drunk, and have unprotected sex. So what to do?

Colleges have tried a number of remedies—from education to abstinence—and none of them has worked particularly well. But lately, some schools have taken a new approach called “social norming.” Although this approach is surprisingly effective, it is also controversial. Tina Rosenberg’s recent book is titled Join the Club: How Peer Pressure Can Transform the World. In the following essay, she describes both the technique and the controversy.

… Like most universities, Northern Illinois University in DeKalb has a problem with heavy drinking. In the 1980s, the school was trying to cut down on student use of alcohol with the usual strategies. One campaign warned teenagers of the consequences of heavy drinking. “It was the ‘don’t run with a sharp stick you’ll poke your eye out’ theory of behavior change,” said Michael Haines, who was the coordinator of the school’s Health Enhancement Services. When that didn’t work, Haines tried combining the scare approach with information on how to be well: “It’s O.K. to drink if you don’t drink too much—but if you do, bad things will happen to you.”

That one failed, too. In 1989, 45 percent of students surveyed said they drank more than five drinks at parties. This percentage was slightly higher than when the campaigns began. And students thought heavy drinking was even more common; they believed that 69 percent of their peers drank that much at parties.

But by then Haines had something new to try. In 1987 he had attended a conference on alcohol in higher education sponsored by the United States Department of Education. There Wes Perkins, a professor of sociology at Hobart and William Smith Colleges, and Alan Berkowitz, a psychologist in the school’s counseling center, presented a paper that they had just published on how student drinking is affected by peers. “There are decades of research on peer influence—that’s nothing new,” Perkins said at the meeting. What was new was their survey showing that when students were asked how much their peers drank, they grossly overestimated the amount. If the students were responding to peer pressure, the researchers said, it was coming from imaginary peers.

The “aha!” conclusion Perkins and Berkowitz drew was this: maybe students’ drinking behavior could be changed by just telling them the truth.

Haines surveyed students at Northern Illinois University and found that they also had a distorted view of how much their peers drink. He decided to try a new campaign, with the theme “most students drink moderately.” The centerpiece of the campaign was a series of ads in the Northern Star, the campus newspaper, with pictures of students and the caption “two thirds of Northern Illinois University students (72%) drink 5 or fewer drinks when they ‘party’.”…

Haines’s staff also made posters with campus drinking facts and told students that if they had those posters on the wall when an inspector came around, they would earn $5. (35 percent of the students did have them posted when inspected.) Later they [the staff members] made buttons for students in the fraternity and sorority system—these students drank more heavily—that said “Most of Us,” and offered another $5 for being caught wearing one. The buttons were deliberately cryptic, to start a conversation.

After the first year of the social norming campaign, the perception of heavy drinking had fallen from 69 to 61 percent. Actual heavy drinking fell from 45 to 38 percent. The campaign went on for a decade, and at the end of it NIU students believed that 33 percent of their fellow students were episodic heavy drinkers, and only 25 percent really were—a decline in heavy drinking of 44 percent….

Why isn’t this idea more widely used? One reason is that it can be controversial. Telling college students “most of you drink moderately” is very different [from] saying “don’t drink.” (It’s so different, in fact, that the National Social Norms Institute, with headquarters at the University of Virginia, gets its money from Anheuser Busch—a decision that has undercut support for the idea of social norming.) The approach angers people who lobby for a strong, unmuddied message of disapproval—even though, of course, disapproval doesn’t reduce bad behavior, and social norming does….

Social norming is a powerful but controversial tool for changing behavior. When we tell students about drinking on campus, should we tell them what’s true (even if the truth is a bit ugly) or should we tell them what’s best (even if they are unlikely to do it)?

From the New York Times, March 27, 2013. © 2013 The New York Times. All rights reserved. Used by permission and protected by the Copyright Laws of the United States. The printing, copying, redistribution, or retransmission of this Content without express written permission is prohibited. http://opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2013/03/27/the-destructive-influence-of-imaginary-peers/

Obedience

In most situations, there are a few people whom we all recognize as having special authority both to define the norms and to enforce them. The guy who works at the movie theater may be some high school fanboy with a bad haircut and a 10:00 p.m. curfew, but in the theater he has authority. So when he asks you to stop texting in the middle of the movie, you do as you are told. Obedience is the tendency to do what authorities tell us to do.

obedience

The tendency to do what powerful authorities tell us to do.

398

Why do we do what others tell us to do?

Why do we obey authorities? Well, yes, sometimes they have guns. But while authorities are often capable of rewarding and punishing us, research shows that much of their influence is normative (Tyler, 1990). Psychologist Stanley Milgram (1963) demonstrated this in one of psychology’s most infamous experiments. The participants in this experiment met a middle-aged man who was introduced as another participant but who was actually a trained actor. An experimenter in a lab coat explained that the participant would play the role of teacher and the actor would play the role of learner. The teacher and learner would sit in different rooms, the teacher would read words to the learner over a microphone, and the learner would then repeat the words back to the teacher. If the learner made a mistake, the teacher would press a button that delivered an electric shock to the learner (see FIGURE 12.10). The shock-generating machine (which, by the way, was totally fake) offered 30 levels of shock, ranging from 15 volts (labeled slight shock) to 450 volts (labeled Danger: severe shock).

Figure 12.10: FIGURE 12.10 Milgram’s Obedience Studies The learner (left) is being hooked up to the shock generator (right) that was used in Stanley Milgram’s obedience studies.
© 1965 by Stanley Milgram
© 1965 by Stanley Milgram
In 1971, psychologist Philip Zimbardo built a mock prison in the basement of the Stanford Psychology Department and invited students to play the roles of either a prisoner or a guard. After 6 days, he was forced to halt his study because many of the guards had become so abusive toward the prisoners that he feared for the “prisoners’” safety (Haney, Banks, & Zimbardo, 1973).
Philip G. Zimbardo, Inc.

After the learner was strapped into his chair, the experiment began. When the learner made his first mistake, the participant dutifully delivered a 15-volt shock. As the learner made more mistakes, he received more shocks. When the participant delivered the 75-volt shock, the learner cried out in pain. At 150 volts, the learner screamed, “I refuse to go on. Let me out!” With every shock, the learner’s screams became more agonized. Then, after receiving the 330-volt shock, the learner stopped responding altogether. Participants were naturally upset by all this and typically asked the experimenter to stop, but the experimenter simply replied, “You have no choice, you must go on.” The experimenter never threatened the participant with punishment of any kind. Rather, he just stood there with his clipboard in hand and calmly instructed the participant to continue. So what did the participants do? Eighty percent of the participants continued to shock the learner even after he screamed, complained, pleaded, and then fell silent. And 62% went all the way, delivering the highest possible voltage. Although Milgram’s study was conducted nearly half a century ago, a recent replication revealed about the same rate of obedience (Burger, 2009).

Would normal people electrocute a stranger just because some guy in a lab coat told them to? The answer, it seems, is yes—as long as normal means being sensitive to norms. The participants in this experiment knew that hurting others is not always wrong: Doctors give painful injections, and teachers give painful exams. There are many situations in which it is permissible to cause someone to suffer. The experimenter’s calm demeanor and persistent instruction suggested that he—and not the participant—knew what was appropriate in this particular situation, and so the participant did what the authority ordered.

399

The Accuracy Motive: Right Is Better Than Wrong

If you know just two things—first, that apples taste good, and second, that there are apples in the refrigerator—then you know exactly what to do when you are hungry. Open the fridge! Actions rely on both an attitude, which is an enduring positive or negative evaluation of an object or event, and a belief, which is an enduring piece of knowledge about an object or event. Attitudes tell us what we should do (eat an apple because they are good), and beliefs tell us how to do it (start by opening the fridge because that’s where the apples are). So if our attitudes or beliefs are inaccurate, then our actions are likely to be fruitless. Because we rely so much on our attitudes and beliefs, it isn’t surprising that we are motivated to have the right ones. As you will see, social influence often appeals to this basic motivation.

attitude

An enduring positive or negative evaluation of an object or event.

belief

An enduring piece of knowledge about an object or event.

Informational Influence

Is McDonald’s trying to keep track of sales from the parking lot? Probably not. Rather, it wants you to know that a lot of other people are buying its hamburgers, which suggests that you should too.
Richard Cummins/Corbis

In what ways are we constant targets of informational influence?

If everyone in the mall suddenly ran screaming for the exit, you’d probably join the stampede—not because you were afraid that the panicked people would disapprove of you if you didn’t, but because their behavior would suggest to you that there was something worth running from. Informational influence is a phenomenon that occurs when another person’s behavior provides information about what is true. You can observe the power of informational influence yourself just by standing in the middle of the sidewalk, tilting back your head, and staring at the top of a tall building. Research suggests that within just a few minutes, other people will stop and stare too (Milgram, Bickman, & Berkowitz, 1969). Why? They will assume that if you are staring, then there must be something worth staring at.

informational influence

A phenomenon that occurs when another person’s behavior provides information about what is true.

You are the constant target of informational influence. When a salesperson tells you that “most people buy the iPad with extra memory,” she is artfully suggesting that you should take other people’s behavior as information about the product. Advertisements that refer to soft drinks as “popular” or books as “best sellers” are reminding you that other people are buying these particular drinks and books, which suggests that they know something you don’t and that you’d be wise to follow their example. Situation comedies provide laugh tracks because the producers know that when you hear other people laughing, you will mindlessly assume that something must be funny (Fein, Goethals, & Kugler, 2007; Nosanchuk & Lightstone, 1974). Bars and nightclubs make people stand in line even when there is plenty of room inside because the owners of these establishments know that passersby will see the line and assume that the club is worth waiting for. In short, the world is full of objects and events about which we know little, and we can often cure our ignorance by paying attention to the way in which others are acting toward them.

Persuasion

Why do advertisers hire celebrities to endorse shoes but not cars? Cars are relatively expensive, so people are motivated to process information about them and are therefore persuaded by facts about quality and price. Shoes are relatively inexpensive, so people are not motivated to process information about them and are therefore persuaded by celebrity endorsements.
© Dwayne Newton/photoedit—All Rights Reserved
The Advertising Archives

Persuasion is a phenomenon that occurs when a person’s attitudes or beliefs are influenced by a communication from another person (Albarracín & Vargas, 2010; Petty & Wegener, 1998). How does it work? When the next presidential election rolls around, the candidates will promise to persuade you to vote for them by demonstrating that their positions on the issues are the most practical, intelligent, fair, and beneficial. Having made that promise, they will then devote most of their financial resources to persuading you by other means—for example, by dressing nicely and smiling a lot, by surrounding themselves with famous athletes and movie stars, by repeatedly pairing their opponent’s picture with Osama bin Laden’s, and so on. The candidates will promise to engage in systematic persuasion, which refers to the process by which attitudes or beliefs are changed by appeals to reason, but they will spend most of their time and money engaged in heuristic persuasion, which refers to the process by which attitudes or beliefs are changed by appeals to habit or emotion (Chaiken, 1980; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). (You’ll recall from the Language and Thought chapter that heuristics are simple shortcuts or “rules of thumb.”)

persuasion

A phenomenon that occurs when a person’s attitudes or beliefs are influenced by a communication from another person.

systematic persuasion

The process by which attitudes or beliefs are changed by appeals to reason.

heuristic persuasion

The process by which attitudes or beliefs are changed by appeals to habit or emotion.

400

When is it more effective to appeal to reason or to emotion?

Which form of persuasion will be more effective? That depends on whether you are willing and able to weigh evidence and analyze arguments. In one study, students heard a speech that contained either strong or weak arguments in favor of instituting comprehensive exams at their school (Petty, Cacioppo, & Goldman, 1981). Some students were told that the speaker was a Princeton University professor, and others were told that the speaker was a high school student—a bit of information that could be used as a shortcut to decide whether to believe the speech. Some students were told that their university was considering implementing these exams right away, thereby giving these students a strong motivation to analyze the evidence, and others were told that their university was considering implementing these exams long after they graduated, thereby giving these students little motivation to analyze the evidence. As FIGURE 12.11 shows, when students were highly motivated to analyze the evidence, they were systematically persuaded—that is, their attitudes and beliefs were influenced by the strength of the arguments and not by the status of the speaker. But when students were not highly motivated to analyze the evidence, they were heuristically persuaded—that is, their attitudes and beliefs were influenced by the status of the speaker and not by the strength of the arguments.

Figure 12.11: FIGURE 12.11 Systematic and Heuristic Persuasion (a) Systematic persuasion. When students were motivated to analyze arguments, their attitudes were influenced by the strength of the arguments (strong arguments were more persuasive than weak arguments), but not by the status of the communicator (the Princeton professor was not more persuasive than the high school student). (b) Heuristic persuasion. When students were not motivated to analyze arguments, their attitudes were influenced by the status of the communicator (the Princeton professor was more persuasive than the high school student), but not by the strength of the arguments (strong arguments were no more persuasive than weak arguments; data from Petty, Cacioppo, & Goldman, 1981).

Consistency

Why do we care about being consistent?

If a friend told you that rabbits had just staged a coup in Antarctica and were halting all carrot exports, you probably wouldn’t turn on CNN. You’d know right away that your friend must be joking (or on drugs) because the statement is logically inconsistent with other things that you know are true—for instance, that rabbits do not foment revolution and Antarctica does not export carrots. People evaluate the accuracy of new beliefs by assessing their consistency with old beliefs, and although this is not a foolproof method for determining whether something is true, it provides a pretty good approximation. Because people are strongly motivated to have beliefs that are true, and because consistency is an indicator of truth, they are strongly motivated to have beliefs that are consistent as well.

401

The motivation to have consistent beliefs leaves people vulnerable to social influence. Consider the foot-in-the-door technique, which is a social influence technique that involves making a small request before making a large request (Burger, 1999). In one study (Freedman & Fraser, 1966), experimenters went to a neighborhood and knocked on doors to see if they could convince homeowners to let them install a big, ugly “Drive Carefully” sign in their front yards. One group of homeowners was simply asked if they would allow the ugly sign to be installed, and only 17% said yes. A second group of homeowners was first asked to sign a petition urging the state legislature to promote safe driving (which almost all agreed to do) and was then asked to allow the installation of the ugly sign. And 55% said yes! Why would homeowners be more likely to grant two requests than one?

foot-in-the-door technique

A social influence technique that involves making a small request before making a large request.

Just imagine how the homeowners in the second group felt. They had just signed a petition stating that they thought safe driving was important, but they really didn’t want an ugly sign in their front yards. And yet, saying yes to the petition and no to the sign would be obviously inconsistent. As they wrestled with this dilemma, they probably began to experience a feeling called cognitive dissonance, which is an unpleasant state that arises when a person recognizes the inconsistency of his or her actions, attitudes, or beliefs (Festinger, 1957). When people experience cognitive dissonance, they naturally try to alleviate it, and one way to alleviate cognitive dissonance is to restore consistency among one’s actions, attitudes, and beliefs (Aronson, 1969; Cooper & Fazio, 1984). Allowing the ugly sign to be installed in their yards accomplished that goal. Recent research shows that this phenomenon can be used to good effect: Hotel guests who were asked at check-in to commit to being a “Friend of the Earth” were 25% more likely to reuse their towels during their stay (Baca-Motes et al., 2013).

cognitive dissonance

An unpleasant state that arises when a person recognizes the inconsistency of his or her actions, attitudes, or beliefs.

What happens when we are inconsistent?

We are motivated to be consistent, but there are inevitably times when we just can’t be—for example, when we kindly tell a friend that her new hairstyle is “cutting edge” when we actually think it resembles a wet skunk after an unfortunate encounter with a snowblower. Why don’t we experience cognitive dissonance under such circumstances? Because complementing a friend’s hairstyle may be inconsistent with the belief that it is actually hideous, but it is consistent with the belief that one should be nice to one’s friends. When small inconsistencies are justified by large consistencies, cognitive dissonance does not occur.

For example, participants in one study were asked to perform a dull task that involved turning knobs one way, then the other way, and then back again. After the participants were sufficiently bored, the experimenter explained that he desperately needed a few more people to volunteer for the study, and he asked the participants to go into the hallway, find another person, and tell that person that the knob-turning task was great fun. The experimenter offered some participants $1 to tell this lie, and offered other participants $20. All participants agreed to tell the lie, and after they did so, they were asked to report their true enjoyment of the knob-turning task. The results showed that participants liked the task more when they were paid $1 than when they were paid $20 to lie about it (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959). Why? Because although the belief that I said the task was fun was inconsistent with the belief that the task was really dull, it was perfectly consistent with the belief that $20 is a whole lot of money. Because the large payment justified the lie, only those participants who received the small payment experienced cognitive dissonance, and then reduced it by changing their beliefs about the enjoyableness of the task.

402

SUMMARY QUIZ [12.2]

Question 12.6

1. The ___________ motive describes how people are motivated to experience pleasure and to avoid experiencing pain.
  1. emotional
  2. accuracy
  3. approval
  4. hedonic

d.

Question 12.7

2. The tendency to do what authorities tell us to do is known as
  1. persuasion.
  2. obedience.
  3. conformity.
  4. the self-fulfilling prophecy.

b.

Question 12.8

3. Andrea and Jeff had to wait in line for over an hour to get into an exclusive restaurant. Despite being served a mediocre meal, they glowingly praised the restaurant to their friends. This behavior was probably a result of
  1. conformity
  2. the norm of reciprocity
  3. the foot in the door technique
  4. cognitive dissonance

d.