4.5 The Body Senses: More Than Skin Deep

Vision and audition provide information about the world at a distance. By responding to light and sound energy in the environment, these “distance” senses allow us to identify and locate the objects and people around us. In comparison, the body senses, also called somatosenses (soma from the Greek for “body”), are up close and personal. Haptic perception is the active exploration of the environment by touching and grasping objects with our hands. We use sensory receptors in our muscles, tendons, and joints as well as a variety of receptors in our skin to get a feel for the world around us (see FIGURE 4.24).

This geodesic dome sits on the floor of the Exploratorium, a science museum in San Francisco. It was created in 1971 by August Coppola (brother of director Francis Ford Coppola and father of actor Nicolas Cage) and Carl Day, who wanted to create an environment in which only haptic perception could be used. The inside of the dome is pitch black; visitors must crawl, wiggle, slide, and otherwise navigate the unfamiliar terrain using only their sense of touch. How would you feel being in that environment for an hour or so?
© Exploratorium

haptic perception

The active exploration of the environment by touching and grasping objects with our hands.

Touch

Touch begins with the transduction of skin sensations into neural signals. Four types of receptors located under the skin’s surface enable us to sense pressure, texture, pattern, or vibration against the skin (see FIGURE 4.24). The receptive fields of these specialized cells work together to provide a rich tactile (from Latin, “to touch”) experience when you explore an object by feeling it or attempting to grasp it. In addition, thermoreceptors, nerve fibers that sense cold and warmth, respond when your skin temperature changes. All these sensations blend seamlessly together in perception, of course, but detailed physiological studies have successfully isolated the parts of the touch system (Hollins, 2010; Johnson, 2002).

Figure 4.24: FIGURE 4.24 Touch Receptors Specialized sensory neurons form groups of haptic receptors that detect pressure, temperature, and vibrations against the skin; their long axons enter the brain via the spinal or cranial nerves. Pain receptors populate all body tissues that feel pain; they are distributed around bones and within muscles and internal organs as well as under the skin surface. Different types of pain receptors transmit immediate, sharp pain sensations or signal slow, dull pain that lasts and lasts.
Be warned for your next shopping trip: Touching the merchandise can lead you to value it more highly than just looking at it.
Ckdj/Corbis

There are three important principles regarding the neural representation of the body’s surface. First, the left half of the body is represented in the right half of the brain and vice versa. Second, just as more of the visual brain is devoted to foveal vision where acuity is greatest, more of the tactile brain is devoted to parts of the skin surface that have greater spatial resolution. Regions such as the fingertips and lips are very good at discriminating fine spatial detail, whereas areas such as the lower back are quite poor at that task. Think back to the homunculus you read about in the Neuroscience and Behavior chapter; you’ll recall that different locations on the body project sensory signals to different locations in the somatosensory cortex in the parietal lobe. Third, there is mounting evidence for a distinction between “what” and “where” pathways in touch analogous to similar distinctions we’ve already considered for vision and audition. The “what” system for touch provides information about the properties of surfaces and objects; the “where” system provides information about a location in external space that is being touched or a location on the body that is being stimulated (Lederman & Klatzky, 2009). The evidence from fMRI studies suggests that the “what” and “where” touch pathways involve areas in the lower and upper parts of the parietal lobe, respectively (Reed, Klatzky, & Halgren, 2005).

124

Why might discriminating spatial detail be important for fingertips and lips?

Touch information can have a powerful effect on our decisions and judgments. For example, recent research has shown that merely touching an object that we don’t already own can increase our feeling of ownership and lead us to value the object more highly than when we view it but don’t touch it (Peck & Shu, 2009); the longer we touch an object, the more highly we value it (Wolf, Arkes, & Muhanna, 2008). You might keep this “mere touch” effect in mind the next time you are in a shop and considering buying an expensive item. Retailers are probably aware of this effect: During a recent holiday shopping season, the office of the Illinois state attorney general warned shoppers to be cautious in stores that encouraged them to touch the merchandise (Peck & Shu, 2009).

Pain

Although pain is arguably the least pleasant of sensations, it is among the most important for survival: Pain indicates damage or potential damage to the body. Without the ability to feel pain, we might ignore infections, broken bones, or serious burns. Children with congenital insensitivity to pain, a rare inherited disorder that specifically impairs pain perception, often mutilate themselves (e.g., by biting into their tongues or by gouging their skin while scratching) and are at increased risk of dying during childhood (Nagasako, Oaklander, & Dworkin, 2003).

125

Tissue damage is transduced by pain receptors, the free nerve endings shown in FIGURE 4.24. Fast-acting A-delta fibers transmit the initial sharp pain one might feel right away from a sudden injury, and slower C fibers transmit the longer-lasting, duller pain that persists after the initial injury. If you were running barefoot outside and stubbed your toe against a rock, you would first feel a sudden stinging pain transmitted by A-delta fibers that would die down quickly, only to be replaced by the throbbing but longer-lasting pain carried by C fibers. Both the A-delta and C fibers are impaired in cases of congenital insensitivity to pain, which is one reason why the disorder can be life threatening.

Neural signals for pain travel to two distinct areas in the brain and evoke two distinct psychological experiences (Treede et al., 1999). One pain pathway sends signals to the somatosensory cortex, identifying where the pain is occurring and what sort of pain it is (sharp, burning, dull). The second pain pathway sends signals to the motivational and emotional centers of the brain, such as the hypothalamus and amygdala, and to the frontal lobe. This is the aspect of pain that is unpleasant and motivates us to escape from or relieve the pain.

Pain typically feels as if it comes from the site of the tissue damage that caused it. If you burn your finger, you will perceive the pain as originating there. But we have pain receptors in many areas besides the skin: around bones and within muscles and internal organs as well. When pain originates internally—in a body organ, for example—we actually feel it on the surface of the body. This kind of referred pain occurs when sensory information from internal and external areas converges on the same nerve cells in the spinal cord. One common example is a heart attack: Victims often feel pain radiating from the left arm rather than from inside the chest.

referred pain

Feeling of pain when sensory information from internal and external areas converges on the same nerve cells in the spinal cord.

One influential account of pain perception is known as the gate-control theory of pain, which holds that signals arriving from pain receptors in the body can be stopped, or gated, by interneurons in the spinal cord via feedback from two directions (Melzack & Wall, 1965). Pain can be gated by the skin receptors, for example, when you rub the affected area. Rubbing your stubbed toe activates neurons that “close the gate” to stop pain signals from traveling to the brain. Pain can also be gated from the brain by modulating the activity of pain-transmission neurons. This neural feedback comes from a region in the midbrain called the periaqueductal gray (PAG). Under extreme conditions, such as high stress, naturally occurring endorphins can activate the PAG to send inhibitory signals to neurons in the spinal cord that then suppress pain signals to the brain, thereby modulating the experience of pain. The PAG is also activated through the action of opiate drugs, such as morphine.

gate-control theory of pain

A theory of pain perception based on the idea that signals arriving from pain receptors in the body can be stopped, or gated, by interneurons in the spinal cord via feedback from two directions.

Why does rubbing an injured area sometimes help alleviate pain?

Although some details of the gate-control theory of pain have been challenged, a key concept underlying the theory—that perception is a two-way street—has broad implications. The senses feed information such as pain sensations to the brain, a pattern termed bottom-up control. The brain processes this sensory data into perceptual information at successive levels to support movement, object recognition, and eventually more complex cognitive tasks, such as memory and planning. But there is ample evidence that the brain exerts plenty of control over what we sense as well. Visual illusions and the gestalt principles of filling in, shaping up, and rounding out what isn’t really there provide some examples. This kind of top-down control also explains how the brain influences the experience of touch and pain.

Body Position, Movement, and Balance

Hitting a ball with a bat or racket provides feedback as to where your arms and body are in space, as well as how the resistance of these objects affects your movement and balance. Successful athletes, such as Serena Williams, have particularly well-developed body senses.
AP Photo/Rick Rycroft

It may sound odd, but one aspect of sensation and perception is knowing where parts of your body are at any given moment. Your body needs some way to sense its position in physical space other than relying on the movement of your eyes constantly to visually check the location of your limbs. Sensations related to position, movement, and balance depend on stimulation produced within our bodies. Receptors in the muscles, tendons, and joints signal the position of the body in space, whereas information about balance and head movement originates in the inner ear.

126

Sensory receptors provide the information we need to perceive the position and movement of our limbs, head, and body. These receptors also provide feedback about whether we are performing a desired movement correctly and how resistance from held objects may be influencing the movement. For example, when you swing a baseball bat, the weight of the bat affects how your muscles move your arm as well as the change in sensation when the bat hits the ball.

Maintaining balance depends primarily on the vestibular system, the three fluid-filled semicircular canals and adjacent organs located next to the cochlea in each inner ear (see FIGURE 4.21). The semicircular canals are arranged in three perpendicular orientations and studded with hair cells that detect movement of the fluid when the head moves or accelerates. The bending of the hair cells generates activity in the vestibular nerve, which is then conveyed to the brain. This detected motion enables us to maintain our balance, or the position of our bodies relative to gravity (Lackner & DiZio, 2005).

vestibular system

The three fluid-filled semicircular canals and adjacent organs located next to the cochlea in each inner ear.

Why is it so hard to stand on one foot with your eyes closed?

Vision also helps us keep our balance. If you see that you are swaying relative to a vertical orientation, such as the contours of a room, you move your legs and feet to keep from falling over. Psychologists have experimented with this visual aspect of balance by placing people in rooms that can be tilted forward and backward (Bertenthal, Rose, & Bai, 1997; Lee & Aronson, 1974). If the room tilts enough—-particularly when small children are tested—people will topple over as they try to compensate for what their visual system is telling them. When a mismatch between the information provided by visual cues and vestibular feedback occurs, motion sickness can result. Remember this discrepancy the next time you try reading in the back seat of a moving car!

SUMMARY QUIZ [4.5]

Question 4.13

1. Which part of the body occupies the greatest area in the somatosensory cortex?
  1. calves.
  2. lips.
  3. lower back.
  4. hips.

b.

Question 4.14

2. The location and type of pain we experience is indicated by signals sent to
  1. the amygdala.
  2. the spinal cord.
  3. pain receptors.
  4. the somatosensory cortex.

d.

127