12.1 Social Thinking and Social Influence

442

12-1 What do social psychologists study? How do we tend to explain others’ behavior and our own?

Unlike sociology, which studies societies and social groupings, social psychologists focus more on how individuals view and affect one another.

Social Thinking

social psychology the scientific study of how we think about, influence, and relate to one another.

Personality psychologists focus on the person. They study the personal traits and dynamics that explain why different people may act differently in a given situation, such as the one Willems faced. (Would you have helped the jailer out of the icy water?) Social psychologists focus on the situation. They study the social influences that explain why the same person will act differently in different situations. Might the jailer have acted differently—opting not to march Willems back to jail—under differing circumstances?

image
An etching of Dirk Willems by Dutch artist Jan Luyken (from Martyrs Mirror, 1685)
Mennonite Library and Archives/Bethel College

The Fundamental Attribution Error

attribution theory the theory that we explain someone’s behavior by crediting either the situation or the person’s disposition.

Our social behavior arises from our social thinking. Especially when the unexpected occurs, we want to understand and explain why people act as they do. After studying how people explain others’ behavior, Fritz Heider (1958) proposed an attribution theory: We can attribute the behavior to the person’s stable, enduring traits (a dispositional attribution), or we can attribute it to the situation (a situational attribution).

fundamental attribution error the tendency for observers, when analyzing others’ behavior, to underestimate the impact of the situation and to overestimate the impact of personal disposition.

For example, in class, we notice that Juliette seldom talks. Over coffee, Jack talks nonstop. That must be the sort of people they are, we decide. Juliette must be shy and Jack outgoing. Such attributions—to their dispositions—can be valid, because people do have enduring personality traits. But sometimes we fall prey to the fundamental attribution error (Ross, 1977): We overestimate the influence of personality and underestimate the influence of situations. In class, Jack may be as quiet as Juliette. Catch Juliette at a party and you may hardly recognize your quiet classmate.

David Napolitan and George Goethals (1979) demonstrated the fundamental attribution error in an experiment with Williams College students. They had students talk, one at a time, with a young woman who acted either cold and critical or warm and friendly. Before the conversations, the researchers told half the students that the woman’s behavior would be spontaneous. They told the other half the truth—that they had instructed her to act friendly (or unfriendly).

Did hearing the truth affect students’ impressions of the woman? Not at all! If the woman acted friendly, both groups decided she really was a warm person. If she acted unfriendly, both decided she really was a cold person. They attributed her behavior to her personal disposition even when told that her behavior was situational—that she was merely acting that way for the purposes of the experiment.

WHAT FACTORS AFFECT OUR ATTRIBUTIONS? The fundamental attribution error appears more often in some cultures than in others. Individualist Westerners more often attribute behavior to people’s personal traits. People in East Asian cultures are somewhat more sensitive to the power of the situation (Heine & Ruby, 2010; Kitayama et al., 2009). This difference has appeared in experiments that asked people to view scenes, such as a big fish swimming. Americans focused more on the individual fish, and Japanese people more on the whole scene (Chua et al., 2005; Nisbett, 2003).

image For a quick interactive tutorial, visit LaunchPad’s Concept Practice: Making Attributions.

443

We all commit the fundamental attribution error. Most of us have had the experience of meeting someone we know from a particular situation—a teacher, a boss, a family doctor—in a different context. Often, we may find ourselves surprised by the difference: A teacher who is authoritative and assertive in class seems lower key. The doctor, a revered authority in the office, seems like an ordinary person. Outside their assigned roles, professors seem less professorial, presidents less presidential, managers less managerial.

When we explain our own behavior, we are sensitive to how behavior changes with the situation (Idson & Mischel, 2001). (An important exception: We more often attribute our deliberate and admirable actions not to situations but to our own good reasons [Malle, 2006; Malle et al., 2007].) We also are sensitive to the power of the situation when we explain the behavior of people we know well and have seen in different contexts. We more often commit the fundamental attribution error when a stranger acts badly. Having only seen that red-faced fan screaming at the referee in the heat of competition, we may assume he is a bad person. But outside the stadium, he may be a good neighbor and a great parent.

Some 7 in 10 college women report having experienced a man misattributing her friendliness as a sexual come-on (Jacques-Tiura et al., 2007).

As we act, our eyes look outward; we see others’ faces, not our own. If we could take the observer’s point of view, would we become more aware of our own personal style? To test this idea, researchers have filmed two people interacting with a camera behind each person. Then they showed each person a replay of their interaction—filmed from the other person’s perspective. Seeing their behavior from the other person’s perspective, participants better appreciated the power of the situation (Lassiter & Irvine, 1986; Storms, 1973).

WHAT ARE THE CONSEQUENCES OF OUR ATTRIBUTIONS? The way we explain others’ actions, attributing them to the person or the situation, can have important real-life effects (Fincham & Bradbury, 1993; Fletcher et al., 1990). A person must decide whether to attribute another’s friendliness to romantic interest or social obligation. A partner must decide whether a loved one’s tart-tongued remark reflects a mean disposition or a bad day. A jury must decide whether a shooting was malicious or in self-defense. In one study, 181 state judges gave lighter sentences to a violent offender who a scientist testified had a gene that altered brain areas related to aggressiveness (Aspinwall et al., 2012).

image
Dispositional versus situational attributions Should the 2015 slaughter of nine African-Americans attending a church Bible study in Charleston be attributed to the shooter’s disposition (“There is one person to blame here. A person filled with hate,” said South Carolina governor Nikki Haley)? To America’s gun culture (“At some point, we as a country will have to reckon with the fact that this type of mass violence does not happen in other advanced countries . . . with this kind of frequency,” said President Obama)? Or to both?
© Richard Ellis/Alamy

Consider the social and economic effects of attribution. How do we explain poverty or unemployment? In Britain, India, Australia, and the United States (Furnham, 1982; Pandey et al., 1982; Wagstaff, 1982; Zucker & Weiner, 1993), political conservatives have tended to attribute responsibility to the personal dispositions of the poor and unemployed: “People generally get what they deserve. Those who take initiative can choose to get ahead.” After inviting people to reflect on the power of choice—by having them recall their own choices or take note of another’s choices—people become more likely to think that people get what they deserve (Savani & Rattan, 2012). Political liberals, and those not primed to consider the power of choice, are more likely to blame past and present situations: “If you or I had to live with the same poor education, lack of opportunity, and discrimination, would we be any better off? Better to drain the swamps than swat the mosquitoes.”

The point to remember: Our attributions—to a person’s disposition or to the situation—have real consequences.

Attitudes and Actions

444

12-2 How do attitudes and actions interact?

attitude feelings, often influenced by our beliefs, that predispose us to respond in a particular way to objects, people, and events.

Attitudes are feelings, often influenced by our beliefs, that predispose our reactions to objects, people, and events. If we believe someone is threatening us, we may feel fear and anger toward the person and act defensively. The traffic between our attitudes and our actions is two-way. Our attitudes affect our actions. And our actions affect our attitudes.

ATTITUDES AFFECT ACTIONS Consider the climate change debate. On one side are climate change activists and the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (2014), who warn of accumulating greenhouse gases, melting glaciers, shrinking Arctic ice, rising seas, dying coral reefs, migrating species and vegetation, and extreme and warming weather. On the other side are climate change skeptics, who include many in the general public. The 31 percent who in 1998 thought “the seriousness of global warming is generally exaggerated” had increased to 42 percent by 2014 (Dugan, 2014).

Knowing that public attitudes affect public policies, people on both sides aim to persuade. Persuasion efforts generally take two forms:

peripheral route persuasion occurs when people are influenced by incidental cues, such as a speaker’s attractiveness.

central route persuasion occurs when interested people focus on the arguments and respond with favorable thoughts.

Persuaders try to influence our behavior by changing our attitudes. But other factors, including the situation, also influence our behavior. Strong social pressures, for example, can weaken the attitude-behavior connection (Wallace et al., 2005). In roll-call votes, politicians will sometimes vote what their supporters demand, despite privately disagreeing with those demands (Nagourney, 2002). In such cases, external pressure overrides the attitude-behavior link.

Attitudes are especially likely to affect behavior when external influences are minimal, and when the attitude is stable, specific to the behavior, and easily recalled (Glasman & Albarracín, 2006). One experiment used vivid, easily recalled information to persuade people that sustained tanning put them at risk for future skin cancer. One month later, 72 percent of the participants, and only 16 percent of those in a waitlist control group, had lighter skin (McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001). Persuasion changed attitudes (concerning skin cancer risk), which changed behavior (less tanning).

ACTIONS AFFECT ATTITUDES Now consider a more surprising principle: Not only will people stand up for what they believe, they also will more strongly believe in what they have stood up for. Many streams of evidence confirm that attitudes follow behavior (FIGURE 12.1).

image
Figure 12.1: FIGURE 12.1 Attitudes follow behavior Cooperative actions, such as those performed by people on sports teams (including Germany, shown here celebrating their World Cup 2014 victory), feed mutual liking. Such attitudes, in turn, promote positive behavior.
Jeff J. Mitchell/Getty Images

THE FOOT-IN-THE-DOOR PHENOMENON How do you think you would react if someone induced you to act against your beliefs? In many cases, people adjust their attitudes. During the Korean war, many U.S. prisoners of war were held in war camps run by Chinese communists. Without using brutality, the captors secured the prisoners’ collaboration in various activities. Some prisoners merely ran errands or did simple tasks to gain privileges. Others made radio appeals and false confessions. Still others informed on fellow prisoners and divulged military information. When the war ended, 21 prisoners chose to stay with the communists. Some of the others returned home “brainwashed”—convinced that communism was a good thing for Asia.

445

foot-in-the-door phenomenon the tendency for people who have first agreed to a small request to comply later with a larger request.

How did the Chinese captors achieve these amazing results? A key ingredient was their effective use of the foot-in-the-door phenomenon: They knew that people who agreed to a small request would find it easier to comply later with a larger one. The Chinese began with harmless requests, such as copying a trivial statement, but gradually escalated their demands (Schein, 1956). The next statement to be copied might list flaws of capitalism. Then, to gain privileges, the prisoners participated in group discussions, wrote self-criticisms, or uttered public confessions. After doing so, they often adjusted their beliefs to be more consistent with their public acts. The point is simple: To get people to agree to something big, start small and build (Cialdini, 1993). A trivial act makes the next act easier. Telling a small lie paves the way to telling a bigger lie. Succumb to a temptation and the next temptation becomes harder to resist.

In dozens of experiments, researchers have coaxed people into acting against their attitudes or violating their moral standards, with the same result: Doing becomes believing. After giving in to a request to harm an innocent victim—by making nasty comments or delivering presumed electric shocks—people begin to look down on their victim. After speaking or writing on behalf of a position they have qualms about, they begin to believe their own words.

“If the King destroys a man, that’s proof to the King it must have been a bad man.”

Thomas Cromwell, in Robert Bolt’s A Man for All Seasons, 1960

Fortunately, the attitudes-follow-behavior principle works with good deeds as well. The foot-in-the-door tactic has helped boost charitable contributions and blood donations. In one classic experiment, researchers posing as safe-driving volunteers asked Californians to permit the installation of a large, poorly lettered “Drive Carefully” sign in their front yards. Only 17 percent consented. They approached other homeowners with a small request first: Would they display a 3-inch-high “Be a Safe Driver” sign? Nearly all readily agreed. When reapproached two weeks later to allow the large, ugly sign in their front yards, 76 percent consented (Freedman & Fraser, 1966). To secure a big commitment, it often pays to put your foot in the door.

Racial attitudes likewise follow behavior. In the years immediately following the introduction of school desegregation in the United States and the passage of the Civil Rights Act of 1964, White Americans expressed diminishing racial prejudice. And as Americans in different regions came to act more alike—thanks to more uniform national standards against discrimination—they began to think more alike. Experiments confirm the observation: Moral action strengthens moral convictions.

role a set of expectations (norms) about a social position, defining how those in the position ought to behave.

ROLE PLAYING AFFECTS ATTITUDES When you adopt a new rolewhen you become a college student, marry, or begin a new job—you strive to follow the social prescriptions. At first, your behaviors may feel phony, because you are acting a role. Soldiers may at first feel they are playing war games. Newlyweds may feel they are “playing house.” Before long, however, what began as playacting in the theater of life becomes you. Researchers have confirmed this effect by assessing people’s attitudes before and after they adopt a new role—sometimes in laboratory situations and sometimes in everyday situations, such as before and after taking a job.

“Fake it until you make it.”

Alcoholics Anonymous saying

image

Video material is provided by BBC Worldwide Learning and CBS New Archives, and produced by Princeton Academic Resources.

image To view Philip Zimbardo’s 14-minute illustration and explanation of his famous prison simulation, see the LaunchPad Video—The Stanford Prison Study: The Power of the Situation below.

Role playing morphed into real life in one famous and controversial study in which male college students volunteered to spend time in a simulated prison. Stanford psychologist Philip Zimbardo (1972) randomly assigned some volunteers to be guards. He gave them uniforms, clubs, and whistles and instructed them to enforce certain rules. Others became prisoners, locked in barren cells and forced to wear humiliating outfits. For a day or two, the volunteers self-consciously “played” their roles. Then the simulation became real—too real. Some guards developed disparaging attitudes, and devised cruel and degrading routines. One by one, the prisoners broke down, rebelled, or became passively resigned. After only six days, Zimbardo called off the study.

446

Critics question the reliability of Zimbardo’s results (Griggs, 2014). But this much seems true: Role playing can train torturers (Staub, 1989). In the early 1970s, the Greek military government eased men into their roles. First, a trainee stood guard outside an interrogation cell. After this “foot in the door” step, he stood guard inside. Only then was he ready to become actively involved in the questioning and torture. What we do, we gradually become. In one study of German males, military training toughened their personalities, leaving them less agreeable even five years later, after leaving the military (Jackson et al., 2012). And it’s true of us all: Every time we act like the people around us, we slightly change ourselves to be more like them, and less like who we used to be.

Yet people differ. In Zimbardo’s Stanford Prison simulation and in other atrocity-producing situations, some people have succumbed to the situation and others have not (Carnahan & McFarland, 2007; Haslam & Reicher, 2007, 2012; Mastroianni & Reed, 2006; Zimbardo, 2007). Person and situation interact. Much as water dissolves salt but not sand, so rotten situations turn some people into bad apples while others resist (Johnson, 2007).

image
The power of the situation In his 1972 Stanford Prison simulation, Philip Zimbardo created a toxic situation (left). Those assigned to the guard role soon degraded the prisoners. In real life in 2004, some U.S. military guards tormented Iraqi prisoners at the U.S.-run Abu Ghraib prison (right). To Zimbardo (2004, 2007), it was a bad barrel rather than a few bad apples that led to the Abu Ghraib atrocities: “When ordinary people are put in a novel, evil place, such as most prisons, Situations Win, People Lose.”
Philip G. Zimbardo, Inc.; AP Photo

cognitive dissonance theory the theory that we act to reduce the discomfort (dissonance) we feel when two of our thoughts (cognitions) are inconsistent. For example, when we become aware that our attitudes and our actions clash, we can reduce the resulting dissonance by changing our attitudes.

COGNITIVE DISSONANCE: RELIEF FROM TENSION So far, we have seen that actions can affect attitudes, sometimes turning prisoners of war into collaborators, doubters into believers, and compliant guards into abusers. But why? One explanation is that when we become aware that our attitudes and actions don’t coincide, we experience tension, or cognitive dissonance. Indeed, the brain regions that become active when people experience cognitive conflict and negative arousal also become active when people experience cognitive dissonance (Kitayama et al., 2013). To relieve this tension, according to Leon Festinger’s (1957) cognitive dissonance theory, we often bring our attitudes into line with our actions.

Dozens of experiments have explored this cognitive dissonance phenomenon. Many have made people feel responsible for behavior that clashed with their attitudes and had foreseeable consequences. In one of these experiments, you might agree for a measly $2 to help a researcher by writing an essay that supports something you don’t believe in (perhaps a tuition increase). Feeling responsible for the statements (which are inconsistent with your attitudes), you would probably feel dissonance, especially if you thought your essay might influence an administrator. To reduce the uncomfortable tension, you might start believing your phony words. At such times, it’s as if we rationalize, “If I chose to do it (or say it), I must believe in it.” The less coerced and more responsible we feel for a troubling act—as after writing that essay for only $2 and not $100—the more dissonance we feel. The more dissonance we feel, the more motivated we are to find consistency, such as changing our attitudes to help justify the act.

447

image To check your understanding of cognitive dissonance, visit LaunchPad’s Concept Practice: Cognitive Dissonance.

The attitudes-follow-behavior principle has a heartening implication: We cannot directly control all our feelings, but we can influence them by altering our behavior. If we are down in the dumps, we can do as cognitive-behavioral therapists advise and talk in more positive, self-accepting ways with fewer self–put-downs. If we are unloving, we can become more loving by behaving as if we were so—by doing thoughtful things, expressing affection, giving affirmation. That helps explain why teens’ doing volunteer work promotes a compassionate identity. “Assume a virtue, if you have it not,” says Hamlet to his mother. “For use can almost change the stamp of nature.” Pretense can become reality. Conduct sculpts character. What we do we become.

“Sit all day in a moping posture, sigh, and reply to everything with a dismal voice, and your melancholy lingers. . . . If we wish to conquer undesirable emotional tendencies in ourselves, we must . . . go through the outward movements of those contrary dispositions which we prefer to cultivate.”

William James, Principles of Psychology, 1890

The point to remember: Cruel acts shape the self. But so do acts of good will. Act as though you like someone, and you soon may. Changing our behavior can change how we think about others and how we feel about ourselves.

RETRIEVE IT

Question

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
ANSWER: By attributing the other person's behavior to the person (“he's a terrible driver”) and his own to the situation (“these roads are awful”), Marco has exhibited the fundamental attribution error.

Question

Pp1NVcrcenco+WCxbETLh+0AOUCvEXrD+yEhV9OFnmlwTz7R0DDy24SlwU6zVOJ0NmnZuKM/hezr3/6qjurEo7ZHmmuh6SDEKI1MNzUlsZDJ0sW2yqCqRugqPxICmAp0ivCWaVd/qac=
ANSWER: Our attitudes often influence our actions as we behave in ways consistent with our beliefs. However, our actions also influence our attitudes; we come to believe in what we have done.

Question

When people act in a way that is not in keeping with their attitudes, and then change their attitudes to match those actions, mIAax9GPcA24OeupsMM/dA+0jo2muyaH8koUgA== theory attempts to explain why.

Social Influence

Social psychology’s great lesson is the enormous power of social influence. This influence can be seen in our conformity, our obedience to authority, and our group behavior. Suicides, bomb threats, airplane hijackings, and UFO sightings all have a curious tendency to come in clusters. On campus, jeans are the dress code; on New York’s Wall Street or London’s Bond Street, dress suits are the norm. When we know how to act, how to groom, how to talk, life functions smoothly. Armed with social influence principles, advertisers, fundraisers, and campaign workers aim to sway our decisions to buy, to donate, to vote. Isolated with others who share their grievances, dissenters may gradually become rebels, and rebels may become terrorists. We’ll start by considering the nature of our cultural influences. Then we will examine the pull of our social strings. How strong are they? How do they operate? When do we break them?

“Have you ever noticed how one example—good or bad—can prompt others to follow? How one illegally parked car can give permission for others to do likewise? How one racial joke can fuel another?”

Marian Wright Edelman, The Measure of Our Success, 1992

Cultural Influences

12-3 How does culture affect our behavior?

Compared with the narrow path taken by flies, fish, and foxes, the road along which environment drives us is wider. The mark of our species—nature’s great gift to us—is our ability to learn and adapt. We come equipped with a huge cerebral hard drive ready to receive cultural software.

448

culture the enduring behaviors, ideas, attitudes, values, and traditions shared by a group of people and transmitted from one generation to the next.

Culture is the behaviors, ideas, attitudes, values, and traditions shared by a group of people and transmitted from one generation to the next (Brislin, 1988; Cohen, 2009). Human nature, noted Roy Baumeister (2005), seems designed for culture. We are social animals, but more. Wolves are social animals; they live and hunt in packs. Ants are incessantly social, never alone. But “culture is a better way of being social,” observed Baumeister. Wolves function pretty much as they did 10,000 years ago. We enjoy things that were unknown to most of our century-ago ancestors, including electricity, indoor plumbing, antibiotics, and the Internet.

We can thank our culture’s mastery of language for this preservation of innovation. The division of labor also helps. Although two lucky people get their names on the cover of this book (which transmits accumulated cultural wisdom), the product actually results from the coordination and commitment of a gifted team of people, no one of whom could produce it alone.

Across cultures, we differ in our language, our monetary systems, our sports, even which side of the road we drive on. But beneath these differences is our great similarity—our capacity for culture. Culture works. It transmits the customs and beliefs that enable us to communicate, to exchange money for things, to play, to eat, and to drive with agreed-upon rules and without crashing into one another.

VARIATION ACROSS CULTURES We see our adaptability in cultural variations among our beliefs and our values, in how we nurture our children and bury our dead, and in what we wear (or whether we wear anything at all). We are ever mindful that the readers of this book are culturally diverse. You and your ancestors reach from Australia to Africa and from Singapore to Sweden.

image
The New Yorker Collection, 2010, Harry Bliss, from cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved.

norm an understood rule for accepted and expected behavior. Norms prescribe “proper” behavior.

Riding along with a unified culture is like running with the wind: As it carries us along, we hardly notice it. When we try running against the wind we feel its force. Face to face with a different culture, we become aware of the cultural winds. Stationed in Iraq, Afghanistan, and Kuwait, American and European soldiers were reminded how liberal their home cultures were. Each cultural group evolves its own normsrules for accepted and expected behavior. The British have a norm for orderly waiting in line. Many South Asians use only the right hand’s fingers for eating. Sometimes social expectations seem oppressive: “Why should it matter what I wear?” Yet, norms grease the social machinery and can free us from self-preoccupation.

When cultures collide, their differing norms often befuddle. Should we greet people by shaking hands, bowing, or kissing one or both cheeks? Knowing what sorts of gestures and compliments are culturally appropriate helps us avoid accidental insults and embarrassment.

VARIATION OVER TIME Like biological creatures, cultures vary and compete for resources, and thus evolve over time (Mesoudi, 2009). Consider how rapidly cultures may change. English poet Geoffrey Chaucer (1342-1400) is separated from a modern Briton by only 20 generations, but the two would have difficulty communicating. In the thin slice of history since 1960, most Western cultures have changed with remarkable speed. Minority groups enjoy expanded human rights. Middle-class people today fly to places they once only read about. They enjoy the convenience of air-conditioned housing, online shopping, anywhere-anytime electronic communication, and—enriched by doubled per-person real income—eating out more than twice as often as did their grandparents.

But some changes seem not so wonderfully positive. Had you fallen asleep in the United States in 1960 and awakened today, you would open your eyes to a culture with more divorce and depression. You would also find North Americans—like their counterparts in Britain, Australia, and New Zealand—spending more hours at work, fewer hours with friends and family, and fewer hours asleep (BLS, 2011; Putnam, 2000).

Whether we love or loathe these changes, we cannot fail to be impressed by their breathtaking speed. And we cannot explain them by changes in the human gene pool, which evolves far too slowly to account for high-speed cultural transformations. Cultures vary. Cultures change. And cultures shape our lives.

449

RETRIEVE IT

Question

8ff99bHjS07yYk/8ZdK3BIOWYK7joM3Mi6OYOMFsaUpfakYB8C1N4L1KkyWK+Lm9i28v/iOphfWMvGSLx+AnXSpIXiia2Kt/tz30oLfpmELuDoqWMh63Qs2QIXcWTxqIfdRixWHmqHdXe5Mgk/nzTxWxldL3UENXDDQneNKnybccbr0JqNq+YiT9ZWJVZN5a
ANSWER: Culture represents our shared behaviors, ideas, attitudes, values, and traditions, which we transmit across generations by way of our language ability. Culture, with its language and efficient division of labor, allows us to preserve innovation.

Conformity: Complying With Social Pressures

12-4 What is automatic mimicry, and how do conformity experiments reveal the power of social influence?

AUTOMATIC MIMICRY Fish swim in schools. Birds fly in flocks. And humans, too, tend to go with their group, to think what it thinks and do what it does. Behavior is contagious. Chimpanzees are more likely to yawn after observing another chimpanzee yawn (Anderson et al., 2004). Ditto for humans. If one of us yawns, laughs, coughs, scratches an itch, stares at the sky, or checks a cell phone, others in our group will often do the same (Holle et al., 2009). Yawn mimicry can also occur across species: Dogs more often yawn after observing their owners’ yawn (Silva et al., 2012). Even just reading about yawning increases people’s yawning (Provine, 2012), as perhaps you have noticed?

image
Copyright The New Yorker Collection 2004 Robert Mankoff from cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved.

Like the chameleon lizards that take on the color of their surroundings, we humans take on the emotional tones of those around us. Just hearing someone reading a neutral text in either a happy- or sad-sounding voice creates “mood contagion” in listeners (Neumann & Strack, 2000). We are natural mimics, unconsciously imitating others’ expressions, postures, and voice tones.

Tanya Chartrand and John Bargh captured this automatic mimicry, which they call the chameleon effect (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). They had students work in a room alongside another person, who was actually a “confederate” working for the experimenters. Sometimes the confederates rubbed their own face. Sometimes they shook their foot. Sure enough, the students tended to rub their face with the face-rubbing person and shake their foot with the foot-shaking person.

Automatic mimicry helps us to empathize—to feel what others are feeling. This helps explain why we feel happier around happy people than around depressed people. It also helps explain why studies of groups of British workers have revealed mood linkage—or the sharing of moods (Totterdell et al., 1998). Empathic people yawn more after seeing others yawn (Morrison, 2007). And empathic mimicking fosters fondness (van Baaren et al., 2003, 2004). Rejected people, who crave social acceptance, often try to mimic their way into new relationships (Lakin et al., 2008). Perhaps you’ve noticed that when someone nods their head as you do and echoes your words, you feel a certain rapport and liking?

image
Conforming to nonconformity Are these students asserting their individuality or identifying themselves with others of the same microculture?
© Ted Horowitz Photography, 2014

Suggestibility and mimicry sometimes lead to tragedy. In the eight days following the 1999 shooting rampage at Colorado’s Columbine High School, every U.S. state except Vermont experienced threats of copycat violence. Pennsylvania alone recorded 60 such threats (Cooper, 1999). Sociologist David Phillips and his colleagues (1985, 1989) found that suicides, too, sometimes increase following a highly publicized suicide. In the wake of screen idol Marilyn Monroe’s suicide on August 5, 1962, for example, the number of suicides in the United States exceeded the usual August count by 200.

450

image
Universal Press Syndicate

“When I see synchrony and mimicry—whether it concerns yawning, laughing, dancing, or aping—I see social connection and bonding.”

Primatologist Frans de Waal “The Empathy Instinct,” 2009

What causes behavior clusters? Do people act similarly because of their influence on one another? Or because they are simultaneously exposed to the same events and conditions? Seeking answers to such questions, social psychologists have conducted experiments on group pressure and conformity.

conformity adjusting our behavior or thinking to coincide with a group standard.

CONFORMITY AND SOCIAL NORMS Suggestibility and mimicry are subtle types of conformityadjusting our behavior or thinking toward some group standard. To study conformity, Solomon Asch (1955) devised a simple test. Imagine yourself as a participant in what you believe is a study of visual perception. You arrive in time to take a seat at a table with five other people. The experimenter asks the group to state, one by one, which of three comparison lines is identical to a standard line. You see clearly that the answer is Line 2, and you await your turn to say so. Your boredom begins to show when the next set of lines proves equally easy.

Now comes the third trial, and the correct answer seems just as clear-cut (FIGURE 12.2). But the first person gives what strikes you as a wrong answer: “Line 3.” When the second person and then the third and fourth give the same wrong answer, you sit up straight and squint. When the fifth person agrees with the first four, you feel your heart begin to pound. The experimenter then looks to you for your answer. Torn between the unanimity voiced by the five others and the evidence of your own eyes, you feel tense and suddenly unsure. You hesitate before answering, wondering whether you should suffer the discomfort of being the oddball. What answer do you give?

image
Figure 12.2: FIGURE 12.2 Asch’s conformity experiments Which of the three comparison lines is equal to the standard line? What do you suppose most people would say after hearing five others say, “Line 3”? In this photo from one of Asch’s experiments, the student in the center shows the severe discomfort that comes from disagreeing with the responses of other group members (in this case, accomplices of the experimenter).
William Vendivert/Scientific American

In Asch’s experiments, college students, answering questions alone, erred less than 1 percent of the time. But what about when several others—confederates working for the experimenter—answered incorrectly? Although most people told the truth even when others did not, Asch was disturbed by his result: More than one-third of the time, these “intelligent and well-meaning” college students were “willing to call white black” by going along with the group.

451

Later investigations have not always found as much conformity as Asch found, but they have revealed that we are more likely to conform when we

image
Tattoos: Yesterday’s nonconformity, today’s conformity? As tattoos become perceived as fashion conformity, their popularity may wane.
Sanne Berg/iStock/360/Getty Images

Why do we so often think what others think and do what they do? Why, in college residence halls, do students’ attitudes become more similar to those living near them (Cullum & Harton, 2007)? Why, when asked controversial questions, are students’ answers more diverse when using anonymous electronic clickers than when publicly raising hands (Stowell et al., 2010)? Why do we clap when others clap, eat as others eat, believe what others believe, say what others say, even see what others see?

normative social influence influence resulting from a person’s desire to gain approval or avoid disapproval.

Frequently, we conform to avoid rejection or to gain social approval (Williams and Sommer, 1997). In such cases, we are responding to normative social influence. We are sensitive to social normsunderstood rules for accepted and expected behavior—because the price we pay for being different can be severe. We need to belong.

image
© The New Yorker Collection, 2006, Mike Twohy from cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved.

informational social influence influence resulting from one’s willingness to accept others’ opinions about reality.

At other times, we conform because we want to be accurate. Groups provide information, and only an uncommonly stubborn person will never listen to others. “Those who never retract their opinions love themselves more than they love truth,” observed Joseph Joubert, an eighteenth-century French essayist. When we accept others’ opinions about reality, we are responding to informational social influence. As Rebecca Denton demonstrated in 2004, sometimes it pays to assume others are right and to follow their lead. Denton set a record for the longest distance driven on the wrong side of a British divided highway—30 miles, with only one minor sideswipe, before the motorway ran out and police were able to puncture her tires. Denton, who was intoxicated, later explained that she thought the hundreds of other drivers coming at her were all on the wrong side of the road (Woolcock, 2004).

Like humans, migrating and herding animals conform for both informational and normative reasons (Claidière & Whiten, 2012). Following others is informative: Compared with solo geese, a flock of geese migrate more accurately. (There is wisdom in the crowd.) But staying with the herd also sustains group membership.

Is conformity good or bad? The answer depends partly on our culturally influenced values. Western Europeans and people in most English-speaking countries tend to prize individualism (cultural focus on an independent self). People in many Asian, African, and Latin American countries place a higher value on collectivism (honoring group standards). In social influence experiments across 17 countries, conformity rates have been lower in individualist cultures (Bond & Smith, 1996).

image To review the classic conformity studies and experience a simulated experiment, visit LaunchPad’s PsychSim 6: Everybody’s Doing It!

RETRIEVE IT

Question

4NBJM37GXzveEwUO9xgWM1L1rhyKWftj+WpTca7aCDX63cRBwCa07EWOTRuHqfr5RQZ9zlEfD0xiR833SCacY6aeQ+nwp40OnqZiD9PjtiF4CbuSqRGqF6U9S53uC4prxmGma+ZMM3kmooIPHC7jCmhJi56qKRwlpucKN/iLo9DXnuaQOC82dpQ9KH/Z1o3+rAsqFrGlwnCR+PC2F9w4HiP4Gi296mr99Ifffm4tvs1b6FxBLwVeiVWRl1o=

Obedience: Following Orders

452

12-5 What did Milgram’s obedience experiments teach us about the power of social influence?

Social psychologist Stanley Milgram (1963, 1974), a high school classmate of Philip Zimbardo and later a student of Solomon Asch, knew that people often give in to social pressures. But how would they respond to outright commands? To find out, he undertook what became social psychology’s most famous and controversial experiments (Benjamin & Simpson, 2009).

image See LaunchPad’s Video: Research Ethics below for a helpful tutorial animation.

Imagine yourself as one of the nearly 1000 people who took part in Milgram’s 20 experiments. You respond to an advertisement for participants in a Yale University psychology study of the effect of punishment on learning. Professor Milgram’s assistant asks you and another person to draw slips from a hat to see who will be the “teacher” and who will be the “learner.” Because (unknown to you) both slips say “teacher,” you draw a “teacher” slip and are asked to sit down in front of a machine, which has a series of labeled switches. The supposed learner, a mild and submissive-seeming man, is led to an adjoining room and strapped into a chair. From the chair, wires run through the wall to your machine. You are given your task: Teach and then test the learner on a list of word pairs. If the learner gives a wrong answer, you are to flip a switch to deliver a brief electric shock. For the first wrong answer, you will flip the switch labeled “15 Volts—Slight Shock.” With each succeeding error, you will move to the next higher voltage. With each flip of a switch, lights flash and electronic switches buzz.

The experiment begins, and you deliver the shocks after the first and second wrong answers. If you continue, you hear the learner grunt when you flick the third, fourth, and fifth switches. After you activate the eighth switch (“120 Volts—Moderate Shock”), the learner cries out that the shocks are painful. After the tenth switch (“150 Volts—Strong Shock”), he begins shouting. “Get me out of here! I won’t be in the experiment anymore! I refuse to go on!” You draw back, but the stern experimenter prods you: “Please continue—the experiment requires that you continue.” You resist, but the experimenter insists, “It is absolutely essential that you continue,” or “You have no other choice, you must go on.”

If you obey, you hear the learner shriek in apparent agony as you continue to raise the shock level after each new error. After the 330-volt level, the learner refuses to answer and falls silent. Still, the experimenter pushes you toward the final, 450-volt switch. “Ask the question,” he says. “And if no correct answer is given, administer the next shock level.”

Would you follow the experimenter’s commands to shock someone? At what level would you refuse to obey? Before undertaking the experiments, Milgram asked nonparticipating people what they would do. Most people were sure they would stop soon after the learner first indicated pain, certainly before he shrieked in agony. Forty psychiatrists agreed with that prediction when Milgram asked them. Were the predictions accurate? Not even close. When Milgram conducted the experiment with other men aged 20 to 50, he was astonished. More than 60 percent complied fully—right up to the last switch. When he ran a new study, with 40 new “teachers” and a learner who complained of a “slight heart condition,” the results were similar. A full 65 percent of the new teachers obeyed the experimenter right up to 450 volts (FIGURE 12.3). In 10 later studies, women obeyed at rates similar to men’s (Blass, 1999).

image
FIGURE 12.3 Milgram’s follow-up obedience experiment In a repeat of the earlier experiment, 65 percent of the adult male “teachers” fully obeyed the experimenter’s commands to continue. They did so despite the “learner’s” earlier mention of a heart condition, and despite hearing cries of protest after they administered what they thought were 150 volts and agonized protests after 330 volts. (Data from Milgram, 1974.)
Stanley Milgram, from the film “Obedience.” Rights Courtesy of Alexandra Milgram

Cultures change over time. Researchers wondered if Milgram’s results could be explained by the 1960s American mind-set. To find out, Jerry Burger (2009) replicated Milgram’s basic experiment. Seventy percent of the participants obeyed up to the 150-volt point, only a slight reduction from Milgram’s 83 percent. And in a French reality TV show replication, 81 percent of people, egged on by a cheering audience, obeyed and tortured a screaming victim (Beauvois et al., 2012).

453

Did the teachers figure out the hoax—that no real shock was being delivered and the learner was in fact a confederate who was pretending to feel pain? Did they realize the experiment was really testing their willingness to comply with commands to inflict punishment? No. The teachers typically displayed genuine distress: They perspired, trembled, laughed nervously, and bit their lips.

Milgram’s use of deception and stress triggered a debate over his research ethics. In his own defense, Milgram pointed out that, after the participants learned of the deception and actual research purposes, virtually none regretted taking part (though perhaps by then the participants had reduced their cognitive dissonance—the discomfort they felt when their actions conflicted with their attitudes). When 40 of the teachers who had agonized most were later interviewed by a psychiatrist, none appeared to be suffering emotional aftereffects. All in all, said Milgram, the experiments provoked less enduring stress than university students experience when facing and failing big exams (Blass, 1996). Other scholars, however, after delving into Milgram’s archives, report that his debriefing was less extensive and his participants’ distress greater than what he had suggested (Nicholson, 2011; Perry, 2013).

In later experiments, Milgram discovered some conditions that influence people’s behavior. When he varied the situation, full obedience ranged from 0 to 93 percent. Obedience was highest when

image
Standing up for democracy Some individuals—roughly one in three in Milgram’s experiments—resist social coercion, as did this unarmed man in Beijing, by single-handedly challenging an advancing line of tanks the day after the 1989 Tiananmen Square student uprising was suppressed.
Jeff Widener/AP Photo

The power of legitimate, close-at-hand authorities was apparent among those who followed orders to carry out the Nazis’ Holocaust atrocities. Obedience alone does not explain the Holocaust—anti-Semitic ideology also contributed (Mastroianni, 2002). But obedience was a factor. In the summer of 1942, nearly 500 middle-aged German reserve police officers were dispatched to German-occupied Jozefow, Poland. On July 13, the group’s visibly upset commander informed his recruits, mostly family men, of their orders. They were to round up the village’s Jews, who were said to be aiding the enemy. Able-bodied men would be sent to work camps, and the rest would be shot on the spot.

The commander gave the recruits a chance to refuse to participate in the executions. Only about a dozen immediately refused. Within 17 hours, the remaining 485 officers killed 1500 helpless women, children, and elderly, shooting them in the back of the head as they lay face down. Hearing the victims’ pleas, and seeing the gruesome results, some 20 percent of the officers did eventually dissent, managing either to miss their victims or to wander away and hide until the slaughter was over (Browning, 1992). In real life, as in Milgram’s experiments, those who resisted were the minority.

A different story played out in the French village of Le Chambon. There, villagers openly defied orders to cooperate with the “New Order”: they sheltered French Jews, who were destined for deportation to Germany, and sometimes helped them escape across the Swiss border. The villagers’ Protestant ancestors had themselves been persecuted, and their pastors taught them to “resist whenever our adversaries will demand of us obedience contrary to the orders of the Gospel” (Rochat, 1993). Ordered by police to give a list of sheltered Jews, the head pastor modeled defiance: “I don’t know of Jews, I only know of human beings.” At great personal risk, the resisters made an initial commitment to resist. Throughout the long, terrible war, they suffered poverty and were punished for their disobedience. Still, supported by their beliefs, their role models, their interactions with one another, and their own initial acts, they remained defiant to the war’s end.

LESSONS FROM THE OBEDIENCE STUDIES What do the Milgram experiments teach us about ourselves? How does flicking a shock switch relate to everyday social behavior? Psychological experiments aim not to re-create the literal behaviors of everyday life but to capture and explore the underlying processes that shape those behaviors. Participants in Milgram’s experiments confronted a dilemma we all face frequently: Do I adhere to my own standards, even when they conflict with expectations?

In Milgram’s experiments and their modern replications, participants were torn. Should they respond to the pleas of the victim or the orders of the experimenter? Their moral sense warned them not to harm another, yet it also prompted them to obey the experimenter and to be a good research participant. With kindness and obedience on a collision course, obedience usually won.

“I was only following orders.”

Adolf Eichmann, director of Nazi deportation of Jews to concentration camps

These experiments demonstrated that strong social influences can make people conform to falsehoods or capitulate to cruelty. Milgram saw this as the fundamental lesson of this work: “Ordinary people, simply doing their jobs, and without any particular hostility on their part, can become agents in a terrible destructive process” (1974, p. 6).

455

Focusing on the end point—450 volts, or someone’s real-life violence—we can hardly comprehend the inhumanity. But we ignore how they get there, in tiny increments. Milgram did not entrap his teachers by asking them first to zap learners with enough electricity to make their hair stand on end. Rather, he exploited the foot-in-the-door effect, beginning with a little tickle of electricity and escalating step by step. In the minds of those throwing the switches, the small action became justified, making the next act tolerable. In Le Chambon, as in Milgram’s experiments, those who resisted usually did so early. After the first acts of compliance or resistance, attitudes began to follow and justify behavior.

So it happens when people succumb, gradually, to evil. In any society, great evils often grow out of people’s compliance with lesser evils. The Nazi leaders suspected that most German civil servants would resist shooting or gassing Jews directly, but they found them surprisingly willing to handle the paperwork of the Holocaust (Silver & Geller, 1978). Milgram found a similar reaction in his experiments. When he asked 40 men to administer the learning test while someone else did the shocking, 93 percent complied. Cruelty does not require devilish villains. All it takes is ordinary people corrupted by an evil situation. Ordinary students may follow orders to haze initiates into their group. Ordinary employees may follow orders to produce and market harmful products. Ordinary soldiers may follow orders to punish and torture prisoners (Lankford, 2009).

“All evil begins with 15 volts.”

Philip Zimbardo, Stanford lecture, 2010

RETRIEVE IT

Question

Psychology's most famous obedience experiments, in which most participants obeyed an authority figure's demands to inflict presumed painful, dangerous shocks on an innocent participant, were conducted by social psychologist okKfwbZn2ZohRyjmLuanwZ2ZGAE= .

Question

8pIxpD8GKrzpWYRt3xhK7mtp3x9Gh71NEBO8VF+xpJevIaRJdt9KVfZvE+HMSjtDWMDzRXvb8KObEGYnEti9PSP6E7QUnw/jr25wOgzBNEkhFe1EWXGO6UuVosyzfLsLPKY/iG9zB77dPprCocx38n54f7KLaQzqw1AntaoK+kM2qtTCoBRzbFBy962ha42I
ANSWER: The Milgram studies showed that people were most likely to follow orders when the experimenter was nearby and was a legitimate authority figure, the victim was not nearby, and there were no models for defiance.

Group Behavior

12-6 How is our behavior affected by the presence of others?

Imagine standing in a room, holding a fishing pole. Your task is to wind the reel as fast as you can. On some occasions you wind in the presence of another participant, who is also winding as fast as possible. Will the other’s presence affect your own performance?

In one of social psychology’s first experiments, Norman Triplett (1898) reported that adolescents would wind a fishing reel faster in the presence of someone doing the same thing. Although a modern reanalysis revealed that the difference was modest (Stroebe, 2012), Triplett inspired later social psychologists to study how others’ presence affects our behavior. Group influences operate both in simple groups—one person in the presence of another—and in more complex groups.

social facilitation improved performance on simple or well-learned tasks in the presence of others.

SOCIAL FACILITATION Triplett’s claim—of strengthened performance in others’ presence—is called social facilitation. But on tougher tasks (learning nonsense syllables or solving complex multiplication problems), people perform worse when observers or others working on the same task are present. Further studies revealed that the presence of others sometimes helps and sometimes hinders performance (Guerin, 1986; Zajonc, 1965). Why? Because when others observe us, we become aroused, and this arousal amplifies our reactions. It strengthens our most likely response—the correct one on an easy task, an incorrect one on a difficult task. Thus, expert pool players who made 71 percent of their shots when alone made 80 percent when four people came to watch them (Michaels et al., 1982). Poor shooters, who made 36 percent of their shots when alone, made only 25 percent when watched.

456

The energizing effect of an enthusiastic audience probably contributes to the home advantage that has shown up in studies of more than a quarter-million college and professional athletic events in various countries (Allen & Jones, 2014; Jamieson, 2010). Home teams win about 6 in 10 games—see TABLE 12.1. For most sports, home cooking is best.

image
image
Social facilitation Skilled athletes often find they are “on” before an audience. What they do well, they do even better when people are watching.
Hope College

Social facilitation also helps explain a funny effect of crowding. Comedians and actors know that a “good house” is a full one. Crowding triggers arousal. Comedy routines that are mildly amusing to people in an uncrowded room seem funnier in a densely packed room (Aiello et al., 1983; Freedman & Perlick, 1979). In experiments, participants, when seated close to one another, like a friendly person even more and an unfriendly person even less (Schiffenbauer & Schiavo, 1976; Storms & Thomas, 1977). So, to ensure an energetic class or event, choose a room or set up seating that will just barely accommodate everyone.

The point to remember: What you do well, you are likely to do even better in front of an audience, especially a friendly audience. What you normally find difficult may seem all but impossible when you are being watched.

SOCIAL LOAFING Social facilitation experiments test the effect of others’ presence on performance of an individual task, such as shooting pool. But what happens when people perform as a group? In a team tug-of-war, would you exert more, less, or the same effort as you would exert in a one-on-one tug-of-war?

To find out, a University of Massachusetts research team asked blindfolded students “to pull as hard as you can” on a rope. When they fooled the students into believing three others were also pulling behind them, students exerted only 82 percent as much effort as when they knew they were pulling alone (Ingham et al., 1974). And consider what happened when blindfolded people seated in a group clapped or shouted as loudly as they could while hearing (through headphones) other people clapping or shouting loudly (Latané, 1981). When they thought they were part of a group effort, the participants produced about one-third less noise than when clapping or shouting “alone.”

social loafing the tendency for people in a group to exert less effort when pooling their efforts toward attaining a common goal than when individually accountable.

Bibb Latané and his colleagues (1981; Jackson & Williams, 1988) described this diminished effort as social loafing. Experiments in the United States, India, Thailand, Japan, China, and Taiwan have found social loafing on various tasks, though it was especially common among men in individualist cultures (Karau & Williams, 1993). What causes social loafing? Three things:

457

deindividuation the loss of self-awareness and self-restraint occurring in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity.

DEINDIVIDUATION We’ve seen that the presence of others can arouse people (social facilitation), or it can diminish their feelings of responsibility (social loafing). But sometimes the presence of others does both. The uninhibited behavior that results can range from a food fight to vandalism or rioting. This process of losing self-awareness and self-restraint, called deindividuation, often occurs when group participation makes people both aroused and anonymous. In one experiment, New York University women dressed in depersonalizing Ku Klux Klan-style hoods. Compared with identifiable women in a control group, the hooded women delivered twice as much electric shock to a victim (Zimbardo, 1970). (As in all such experiments, the “victim” did not actually receive the shocks.)

image
Deindividuation During England’s 2011 riots and looting, rioters were disinhibited by social arousal and by the anonymity provided by darkness and their hoods and masks. Later, some of those arrested expressed bewilderment over their own behavior.
Lewis Whyld/PA Wire/Press Association/AP Images

Deindividuation thrives, for better or for worse, in many settings. Tribal warriors who depersonalize themselves with face paints or masks are more likely than those with exposed faces to kill, torture, or mutilate captured enemies (Watson, 1973). On discussion boards, Internet bullies, who would never say “You’re so fake” to someone’s face, may hide behind anonymity. When we shed self-awareness and self-restraint—whether in a mob, at a rock concert, at a ballgame, or at worship—we become more responsive to the group experience, whether bad or good. For a comparison of social facilitation, social loafing, and deindividuation, see TABLE 12.2.

Table 12.1: TABLE 12.2
Behavior in the Presence of Others: Three Phenomena
Phenomenon Social context Psychological effect of others’ presence Behavioral effect
Social facilitation Individual being observed Increased arousal Amplified dominant behavior, such as doing better what one does well (or doing worse what is difficult)
Social loafing Group projects Diminished feelings of responsibility when not individually accountable Decreased effort
Deindividuation Group setting that fosters arousal and anonymity Reduced self-awareness Lowered self-restraint

* * *

We have examined the conditions under which the presence of others can motivate people to exert themselves or tempt them to free ride on the efforts of others, make easy tasks easier or difficult tasks harder, and enhance humor or fuel mob violence. Research also shows that interacting with others can similarly have both bad and good effects.

Group Polarization

458

12-7 What are group polarization and groupthink, and how much power do we have as individuals?

Over time, initial differences between groups of college and university students tend to grow. If the first-year students at College X tend to have interests in the arts, and those at College Y tend to be business-oriented, those differences will probably be even greater by the time they graduate. Similarly, gender differences tend to widen over time, as Eleanor Maccoby (2002) noted from her decades of observing gender development. Girls talk more intimately than boys do and play and fantasize less aggressively; these differences will be amplified as boys and girls interact mostly with their own gender.

group polarization the enhancement of a group’s prevailing inclinations through discussion within the group.

In each case, the beliefs and attitudes we bring to a group grow stronger as we discuss them with like-minded others. This process, called group polarization, can have beneficial results, as when it amplifies a sought-after spiritual awareness, reinforces the resolve of those in a self-help group, or motivates activists working for a cause. But it can also have dire consequences. George Bishop and I [DM] discovered that when high-prejudice students discussed racial issues, they became more prejudiced (FIGURE 12.4). Low-prejudice students, alternatively, became even more accepting.

image
FIGURE 12.4 Group polarization If a group is like-minded, discussion strengthens its prevailing opinions. Talking over racial issues increased prejudice in a high-prejudice group of high school students and decreased it in a low-prejudice group. (Data from Myers & Bishop, 1970.)

Group polarization can feed extremism and even suicide terrorism. Analyses of terrorist organizations around the world reveal that the terrorist mentality does not erupt suddenly, on a whim (McCauley, 2002; McCauley & Segal, 1987; Merari, 2002). It usually begins slowly, among people who share a grievance. As they interact in isolation (sometimes with other “brothers” and “sisters” in camps) their views grow more and more extreme. Increasingly, they categorize the world as “us” against “them” (Moghaddam, 2005; Qirko, 2004). Given that the self-segregation of the like-minded polarizes people, a 2006 U.S. National Intelligence estimate speculated “that the operational threat from self-radicalized cells will grow.”

“What explains the rise of fascism in the 1930s? The emergence of student radicalism in the 1960s? The growth of Islamic terrorism in the 1990s?. . . The unifying theme is simple: When people find themselves in groups of like-minded types, they are especially likely to move to extremes. [This] is the phenomenon of group polarization.”

Cass Sunstein, Going to Extremes, 2009

When I [DM] got my start in social psychology with experiments on group polarization, I never imagined the potential dangers, or the creative possibilities, of polarization in virtual groups. Electronic communication and social networking have created virtual town halls where people can isolate themselves from those with different perspectives. By attuning our bookmarks and social media feeds to sites that trash the views we despise, we can retreat into partisan tribes and revel in foregone conclusions. Facebook prioritizes News Feed posts that it anticipates we will like. But we ourselves have a bigger effect as we click to content we agree with and unfollow irksome friends, thus blocking those with opposing political views (Bakshy et al., 2015). People read blogs that reinforce their views, and those blogs link to kindred blogs (FIGURE 12.5). Over time, the resulting political polarization—“loathing across party lines,” say some political scientists (Iyengar & Westwood, 2014)—has become much more intense than racial polarization.

image
FIGURE 12.5 Like minds network in the blogosphere Blue liberal blogs link mostly to one another, as do red conservative blogs. (The intervening colors display links across the liberal-conservative boundary.) Each dot represents a blog, and each dot’s size reflects the number of other blogs linking to that blog. (From Lazer et al., 2009.)
Lada Adamic and Natalie Glance.

As the Internet connects the like-minded and pools their ideas, climate change skeptics, UFO abductees, and conspiracy theorists find support for their shared ideas and suspicions. White supremacists may become more racist. And militia members may become more terror prone. The longer participants spend in closed “Dark Web” forums, the more violent their messages become (Chen, 2012). Boston Marathon bombers Dzhokhar and Tamerlan Tsarnaev reportedly were “self-radicalized” through their Internet participation (Wilson et al., 2013). In the echo chambers of virtual worlds, as in the real world, separation + conversation = polarization.

But the Internet-as-social-amplifier can also work for good. Social networking sites connect friends and family members sharing common interests or coping with similar challenges. Peacemakers, cancer survivors, and bereaved parents can find strength and solace from kindred spirits. By amplifying shared concerns and ideas, Internet-enhanced communication can also foster social ventures. (I [DM] know this personally from social networking with others with hearing loss in an effort to transform American assistive listening technology.)

459

image
Frank Cotham/The New Yorker Collection/Condé Nast

The point to remember: By connecting and magnifying the inclinations of like-minded people, the Internet can be very, very bad, but also very, very good.

GROUPTHINK So, group interaction can influence our personal decisions. Does it ever distort important national decisions? Consider the “Bay of Pigs fiasco.” In 1961, President John F. Kennedy and his advisers decided to invade Cuba with 1400 CIA-trained Cuban exiles. When the invaders were easily captured and soon linked to the U.S. government, Kennedy wondered aloud, “How could we have been so stupid?”

groupthink the mode of thinking that occurs when the desire for harmony in a decision-making group overrides a realistic appraisal of alternatives.

Social psychologist Irving Janis (1982) studied the decision-making process leading to the ill-fated invasion. He discovered that the soaring morale of the recently elected president and his advisers fostered undue confidence. To preserve the good feeling, group members suppressed or self-censored their dissenting views, especially after President Kennedy voiced his enthusiasm for the scheme. Since no one spoke strongly against the idea, everyone assumed the support was unanimous. To describe this harmonious but unrealistic group thinking, Janis coined the term groupthink.

“One of the dangers in the White House, based on my reading of history, is that you get wrapped up in groupthink and everybody agrees with everything, and there’s no discussion and there are no dissenting views.”

Barack Obama, December 1, 2008, press conference

Later studies showed that groupthink—fed by overconfidence, conformity, self-justification, and group polarization—contributed to other fiascos as well. Among them were the failure to anticipate the 1941 Japanese attack on Pearl Harbor; the escalation of the Vietnam war; the U.S. Watergate cover-up; the Chernobyl nuclear reactor accident (Reason, 1987); the U.S. space shuttle Challenger explosion (Esser & Lindoerfer, 1989); and the Iraq war, launched on the false idea that Iraq had weapons of mass destruction (U.S. Senate Intelligence Committee, 2004).

“Truth springs from argument among friends.”

Philosopher David Hume, 1711–1776

Despite the dangers of groupthink, two heads are often better than one. Knowing this, Janis also studied instances in which U.S. presidents and their advisers collectively made good decisions, such as when the Truman administration formulated the Marshall Plan, which offered assistance to Europe after World War II, and when the Kennedy administration successfully prevented the Soviets from installing missiles in Cuba. In such instances—and in the business world, too, Janis believed—groupthink is prevented when a leader welcomes various opinions, invites experts’ critiques of developing plans, and assigns people to identify possible problems. Just as the suppression of dissent bends a group toward bad decisions, open debate often shapes good ones. This is especially the case with diverse groups, whose varied perspectives often enable creative or superior outcomes (Nemeth & Ormiston, 2007; Page, 2007). None of us is as smart as all of us.

460

“If you have an apple and I have an apple and we exchange apples then you and I will still each have one apple. But if you have an idea and I have an idea and we exchange these ideas, then each of us will have two ideas.”

Attributed to dramatist George Bernard Shaw, 1856–1950

THE POWER OF INDIVIDUALS In affirming the power of social influence, we must not overlook the power of individuals. Social control (the power of the situation) and personal control (the power of the individual) interact. People aren’t billiard balls. When feeling coerced, we may react by doing the opposite of what is expected, thereby reasserting our sense of freedom (Brehm & Brehm, 1981).

Committed individuals can sway the majority and make social history. Were this not so, communism would have remained an obscure theory, Christianity would be a small Middle Eastern sect, and Rosa Parks’ refusal to sit at the back of the bus would not have ignited the U.S. civil rights movement. Technological history, too, is often made by innovative minorities who overcome the majority’s resistance to change. To many, the railroad was a nonsensical idea; some farmers even feared that train noise would prevent hens from laying eggs. People derided Robert Fulton’s steamboat as “Fulton’s Folly.” As Fulton later said, “Never did a single encouraging remark, a bright hope, a warm wish, cross my path.” Much the same reaction greeted the printing press, the telegraph, the incandescent lamp, and the typewriter (Cantril & Bumstead, 1960).

image
Gandhi As the life of Hindu nationalist and spiritual leader Mahatma Gandhi powerfully testifies, a consistent and persistent minority voice can sometimes sway the majority. Gandhi’s nonviolent appeals and fasts were instrumental in winning India’s independence from Britain in 1947.
akg-images/Newscom

The power of one or two individuals to sway majorities is minority influence (Moscovici, 1985). In studies of groups in which one or two individuals consistently express a controversial attitude or an unusual perceptual judgment, one finding repeatedly stands out: When you are the minority, you are far more likely to sway the majority if you hold firmly to your position and don’t waffle. This tactic won’t make you popular, but it may make you influential, especially if your self-confidence stimulates others to consider why you react as you do. Even when a minority’s influence is not yet visible, people may privately develop sympathy for the minority position and rethink their views (Wood et al., 1994).

The bottom line: The powers of social influence are enormous, but so are the powers of the committed individual. For classical music, Mozart mattered. For drama, Shakespeare mattered. For world history, Hitler and Mao—and Gandhi and Mandela—mattered. Social forces matter. But individuals matter, too.

RETRIEVE IT

Question

3UyWAEGrnYRHOmWIT4AuKKs0UWWlGkBA+IdAhQXqpHojNME/Q/FiUnnVK97otZR5dJn9Uw6zUdOuMvxTc64MVZLpglKngnbJP3uLBcQt/4pzI8lunyblNJTP5qEqOqTWX1OFLv8A6JfaCDZ8VYYO+u+5iGk19nvKNTwrZhW7wK0Mtz0364AGcbiZf05YJxPZP/nCcHBeP+cnziT7+egqpQiIWdNTF3wy+o2FH0sPfd2uAuWwquDMTw==
ANSWER: This improved performance in the presence of others is most likely to occur with a well-learned task, because the added arousal caused by an audience tends to strengthen the most likely response. This also predicts poorer performance on a difficult task in others' presence.

Question

People tend to exert less effort when working with a group than they would alone, which is called s0nqqgeEWzuKH8XhFDaAHrOIl10= .

Question

bolun+EaAv13RUJWPsZRnyHWFLHiFvA7d7Xx+Crl867HpAWbJMcXMQj9I+PjjtD3gtgXN5ZHJvKZRzA8+eXMvqs+CiegqkF0IajM0QgLVRajoPwtveAL7Yj2CDOonHyPUZ1+6lNKq1Q1a8KFotTLKP+6ryZCZm4655HeksMi7cAStrlAGHcUnzvZQm6xdr9lSNSP9UK1sa/GNe9RkI6VBG3my5Ik6ES+07TOOBkAFkTfYYcf/sYXRmyiMTNK/CaOsrbRXj/hfGJ2fDu1KNY9bDo362DE8XWaOJWPLj3k8bDRAGm5TsgY2r4+gmtKYcTmyMGFLrUX95kYAeyk28SWTIWPtPFfDs0WNBVk7tOEwJyV0dW0vlq9bXdSKraA8pOhlqBlpg==
ANSWER: The anonymity provided by the masks, combined with the arousal of the contentious setting, might create deindividuation (lessened self-awareness and self-restraint).

Question

When like-minded groups discuss a topic, and the result is the strengthening of the prevailing opinion, this is called QEtGlbjJACk4pvjhOPhrcKnmMmgw+xHz .

Question

When a group's desire for harmony overrides its realistic analysis of other options, 2Re89+8fo5Fs6Lz7w/2ocw== has occurred.

REVIEW Social Thinking and Social Influence

Learning Objectives

461

Test Yourself by taking a moment to answer each of these Learning Objective Questions (repeated here from within the chapter). Research suggests that trying to answer these questions on your own will improve your long-term memory of the concepts (McDaniel et al., 2009).

Question

1EnVSu072FuYHVHHwE7cn2M/76md72bbg23UupR/FoTKPbXmKBhYwh0TcuUdRhWx+M6mAa0GITNORCygIkDHJn5/Ig0TcqfE4arvM8tPUsNQ9EPi4KAYVw8eRnCC+0t5R8D7blKyXMy0UVqAvf6QXMgEbZn/1jgEKeHwVzRo5UpdOETCRTlzWcqsfc3kkk+vUNgXtVyUowgJc1PwjMQTOGDZo/ri9tOAXKHfcdMmrak1gv1pjWUgSgSu0CjnkTY2ZxVGAU5CmttaEzMGxCpDiA==
ANSWER: Social psychologists use scientific methods to study how people think about, influence, and relate to one another. They study the social influences that explain why the same person will act differently in different situations. When explaining others' behavior, we may—especially if we come from an individualist Western culture—commit the fundamental attribution error, by underestimating the influence of the situation and overestimating the effects of stable, enduring traits. When explaining our own behavior, we more readily attribute it to the influence of the situation.

Question

KftNPV3SH56PYNVGlNno9pnIpcPeTHmbK7C1n9EButZ6q0gxwG1ADfB3Eqw5klbBv/L0mLc5aMeMbFadiB6XNTwXOgI0aClUIHfgkusRmNK3o0wcVE2bx2bp7MnTDu/CSwaJn4kwBB1Xhrn16kVF1AP8tP48Eoicl+mrAZ6vOZ/P1ftyVqhdLNM19ASXpAP1pUw3fiTtQMBELNv+
ANSWER: Attitudes are feelings, often influenced by our beliefs, that predispose us to respond in certain ways. Peripheral route persuasion uses incidental cues (such as celebrity endorsement) to try to produce fast but relatively thoughtless changes in attitudes. Central route persuasion offers evidence and arguments to trigger thoughtful responses. When other influences are minimal, attitudes that are stable, specific, and easily recalled can affect our actions. Actions can modify attitudes, as in the foot-in-the-door phenomenon (complying with a large request after having agreed to a small request) and role playing (acting a social part by following guidelines for expected behavior). When our attitudes don't fit with our actions, cognitive dissonance theory suggests that we will reduce tension by changing our attitudes to match our actions.

Question

DFPXPd8niaLvQ2hgt48rs08FGxNHxArhBlUkEkbIZ1+ECy5w8UOJ3l40p9V0OSedpIsC2mwzKRV86cCAEUfrSyv1/KIfq4FP96V4VLKmD9Js147ugIWReSx5tIGhw6jNzV2ueYIdMl5s/LasnYYhprshwqk31fFjRgi5R5fIbpaKMtyfBn3NUurCgUK3HszmbGPLOozhR9Q=
ANSWER: A culture is an enduring set of behaviors, ideas, attitudes, values, and traditions shared by a group and transmitted from one generation to the next. Cultural norms are understood rules that inform members of a culture about accepted and expected behaviors. Cultures differ across time and space.

Question

TfMvm9GDqEq6BNxLxPvR4Q/XNZ7GjNtj+ObBEgSHlG7zUWx8GeM1xV5Nyr/zNy8A/S4xxBEltCfXWy8PQxEq4IBvQ208diG1KtdLdyDRl1ToUw7XzW3GbFhWCTsYYgEhlDvZ5OrEwYV/wMYeO5q9URUmOgUhxpIda+yOZ9x3Xs84HizBBIm7mUH8hzdNmL1z65QVBDFvNMMdHVSJi0L8sxVOXgYLYcZ8GPyNL3Ac5nvKLXUWT9tkyNwYdTcwLwlscj5+S9JhOAa38/4Z4S6aqe9h3os4sg9o
ANSWER: Automatic mimicry (the chameleon effect)—our tendency to unconsciously imitate others' expressions, postures, and voice tones—is a form of conformity. Solomon Asch and others have found that we are most likely to adjust our behavior or thinking to coincide with a group standard when (a) we feel incompetent or insecure, (b) our group has at least three people, (c) everyone else agrees, (d) we admire the group's status and attractiveness, (e) we have not already committed to another response, (f) we know we are being observed, and (g) our culture encourages respect for social standards. We may conform to gain approval (normative social influence) or because we are willing to accept others' opinions as new information (informational social influence).

Question

4+kGxS1DEm27YTnb/WV+mRoDDDYua1iempWtjJ0P/e8ZOt03PuiRRcaOshZ+laHNIlI9CJ0ao2s8I4ZefARNmdoeo9BNYcmyHIc4y6T/YlcIUNasiAmOhk2aba2KiL2hfAYZw6OYpnbXPIEfycFIPlk9WrqrGk85u7g8EmrlXOxJFYjxgZ+csuLbUOzn+qKo2VUHqQ8qjF54kDpHyed3d9uPuK7Z8QAh5HgXwdtiUrWP2p2O1rsK3bj71MiuFE95lS5b8wQlYareRVkW
ANSWER: Stanley Milgram's experiments—in which people obeyed orders even when they thought they were harming another person—demonstrated that strong social influences can make ordinary people conform to falsehoods or give in to cruelty. Obedience was highest when (a) the person giving orders was nearby and was perceived as a legitimate authority figure; (b) the research was supported by a prestigious institution; (c) the victim was depersonalized or at a distance; and (d) there were no role models for defiance.

Question

CNf3n9QDcTc0axgCXAV0dxYB5Ab2H8DnAMu4pZHMBOa1SHBKyayrrweJvwYmdWOEJWMU2n6xtdKaN3kQaTF0q7bTbyo0Ha2135EPGZyXjjOge48C3BwmyzHCT+G49jegkbiXpgoFs/ASlCWQzH3XdVQsp4dC3uca1S//3sb92h/r+z1mOvM+zwXbO9W4INiuuErxfZJ7H8gNy375cvvYM1TnyZt7yb/rbUX7RA==
ANSWER: In social facilitation, the mere presence of others arouses us, improving our performance on easy or well-learned tasks but decreasing it on difficult ones. In social loafing, participating in a group project makes us feel less responsible, and we may free ride on others' efforts. When the presence of others both arouses us and makes us feel anonymous, we may experience deindividuation—loss of self-awareness and self-restraint.

Question

mgu3Yhvso6pZQH4IvFNK7ZOonKuFw6mspTQC3FCYx+hmps9mDym1s1L7LwGRyA3iAXo8iuU8bkYjnY5ZPTR0MxWbXSgLId/OS0q8OzSBzimGuMkQLfzhL528F9UeSVr5d9i+4qpT8t/JbNKrUA6riSCLCF3nPGd0vSpYRg36inHmz7ONfzX2X8zITR+f6t2VHIGVLcukrsisI9gHQEO64ioB7t4/gm0MyX8aacHRjes1ALNcxc+hMQXnqTP6s7TTyH6bpU52TiVW2DB7ryjPyBA8OmV1B9w47RDD3aWZ+Q8=
ANSWER: In group polarization, group discussions with like-minded others strengthen members' prevailing beliefs and attitudes. Internet communication magnifies this effect, for better and for worse. Groupthink is driven by a desire for harmony within a decision-making group, overriding realistic appraisal of alternatives. The power of the individual and the power of the situation interact. A small minority that consistently expresses its views may sway the majority.

Terms and Concepts to Remember

Test yourself on these terms.

Question

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

Experience the Testing Effect

Test yourself repeatedly throughout your studies. This will not only help you figure out what you know and don’t know; the testing itself will help you learn and remember the information more effectively thanks to the testing effect.

Question 12.1

yf+nyntXsewIo3e0RtXqCibSWuT3ulROe6pVg+4qUp4CEa57VYd9A2SZoJuXkyp1XlgX4xfPQnthBp0Mp6KwMKbhbW/7RK9Gs2yRqhsF5flTU6ZyypIeiNIYgXZdMGk8nbSCcFX/URDlirBYj8RvMmFWiiICDxJ90Em0LrUJOmABZoLr6jJoJ3dHJ/3Z6ru4pvlRNUiFhnpRhIekhqFuD9taBCsr3TX3HHmCgXLcn94VWDy+VkUdOiWPzyuk4r1ZbU2+YWvYkTdOW42ci7+tYTz6+CgyQg9JDPeSah4CHL0bL+XOuACKXIYm2D88EAPm7Vp3WMRac7p9sVOIrYfR5gaUIv/3eRLxjMFHFjG8O2z8q4OkEdCM7Mr8ZcZLhDFoU4KQBDD5I4fqZeZJTfpgfrtuDJUyFd0w1tE1oOsbOkAEYNgJscY3moJvEiA5/Vbg9/mj1HwrVoDNrc7YZbiNTdG/uf5cQT8U

Question 12.2

2. Celebrity endorsements in advertising often lead consumers to purchase products through ubDvDDszp2evDFUIaQC/7g== (central/peripheral) route persuasion.

Question 12.3

3. We tend to agree to a larger request more readily if we have already agreed to a small request. This tendency is called the rZpxE+UqLI2rwvzbbvGmPZ3Cb3IJPXVn phenomenon.

Question 12.4

YEu0ieYnr8GgOpAL0txfuB5vUaXfhMzaNISWCIDvsLRqq5/Ja9gOxOkv/dtkxRl92DJGEKj1kOu5XxTVpfwRSN0gtsG1AtkpWFgyipoBz1G/wJab5z2aTXuXw9ASo4qIUaWUuxT5c54vNi1V8RL06M8vmFofGJQlJflatjpubw89sxXygoRhL8EW9XrLh33uWvOPvKBaMJUo89YCoceFH7hecncPq/gSu5xAOHyFKfPjX8eK5xjpUDO3145vitccTCVirnErQ8RUMGVJKv11uQ0Z6NjhXgkhjpwe0ygO6GvxzPPaCCwi7Yt6uEiAHMNIiqsO8iXP5tKmLusHHzhn1TJgLMwIL+5vKYDo4pIvoVXU7ARNXad3JcGDHA4Ed5Ty+8F+GjDI8MfqI+Znop/Xi3V8BQs7NeddJrd6zfNz1TgFxSAE8jbJ/RoeTpwv8A9X
ANSWER: Cognitive dissonance theory best supports this suggestion. If Jamal acts confident, his behavior will contradict his negative self-thoughts, creating cognitive dissonance. To relieve the tension, Jamal may realign his attitudes with his actions by viewing himself as more outgoing and confident.

Question 12.5

r5Y5kk4NXAsHUNRUKKwQMKYepD+ZbhwtcMNN2A/uqLHEnYPXEc8JJPZcq9zOTAYHiUh7GN9AoXlSqfzhHs6JfDnR59CbqTi+PSPyiaL8ff8/G1QlAxOoDehbijoRRdq2aRbmW5nPoE7l6MQ7+2ZQwVdXTBhhkIX05eEI/iGOfdmzpE6ABYOSa6m94GOqL5ngzBL7QBLk7BEN0LdB8+a83DvpR5X4qhACSsR0RzJuEloXr4eJVFfmw7sgUI2CS6vxhmTYzFfXPw4WemAsmd0MDYFmNIESnALXVo++OMIHXOwakn1K10WaxWN6iqGeCSAC0ghM31tXHAtAEnbl2Z4g7+htKQ3wZMWdQZnzi59JwgMidu2H/PObZJDh4/WVyl4maF/ha/LiwE7UzmKsLUI+y5qCWzA=

Question 12.6

NYq5GGahlNf0UvGKPV81dSgd1Q4+NRGjpudAD1MhqJ4d/LqJB3y2a6AHdbdYSOE/BHjnQVyk+LpoX6ARY9FvbMRdJ5CS+h7QDxYmsmB/hiKXCr3RvSzNNwxHVhKvWrBlCnmGtGNPBu3xH2t9so0SW7p4dgYyHMBj1c758qashMjTQRyZ+LCBTLsJhY3hhC+2+lNYB3XbVDWQad5DivDG+H7twZjEGdAgCe9yB8u/LWepmHLk7zqFkzYcJtjYW9t+cWz5cFvvNmeqafWgVcJDHfACGk/pDAYf8FV0VXWjiqvzCJL6jQq6shTMfxipY14Eg9O96yZn39pPy62q3YqVFvPWyvAje1WCZxjZFXu55kbwBCJ9gGh6ky8DFF5O8qp/Uixts/JM174CHmq4OpgBKt3vtbiv62YnopYwoq8m746MVLM1FFkvKT4NNfGjh1+p

Question 12.7

5HiGaVKPXx82gYTzF3Xlu165W7brC823HweJrXOcr+7d3G+bzusFw5PRRR17uR3mk9Cki82xlKWqoYJ6mtLSU6ujUAPY6Z5oT4Ln8b1znqpo0Qg0tX8rcQJuYwO1ygWzVVOnzS0rtka3aJ+OQDi5PPPJ9imxL/2i0KbrPSfoVA7kBy6++JJeYnBw66Svh2fWJTRLJ/hFiakqY71/qU/JpcvmDAHOkSeIKt3FOX9jN8Ld0A2h6LRJ6xicTRVZHvqnLQtkiHabLL82j5T9oOiTuJdSXoqmeMkaU416V+EsuneBgjFIr3hXFtPrkmXucf1ZXYnexW2HlmFDSjCwByunIBtUj2KpiN7SI1pOhDLG5kBQ1iN33Qj1ND0LcbOcxK5EMyF/Iqg+DCk37T1RpVkHhLOEKBw=
ANSWER: The presence of a large audience generates arousal and strengthens Dr. Huang's most likely response: enhanced performance on a task he has mastered (teaching music history) and impaired performance on a task he finds difficult (statistics).

Question 12.8

8. In a group situation that fosters arousal and anonymity, a person sometimes loses self-consciousness and self-control. This phenomenon is called J48TYDUaPpDXdSHD5ynJtRDksfg= .

Question 12.9

9. Sharing our opinions with like-minded others tends to strengthen our views, a phenomenon referred to as QEtGlbjJACk4pvjhOPhrcKnmMmgw+xHz .

Use image to create your personalized study plan, which will direct you to the resources that will help you most in image .