9-2 The Visual System’s Functional Anatomy

289

Our primary sensory experience is visual. Far more of the human brain is dedicated to vision than to any other sense. Understanding the visual system’s organization, then, is key to understanding human brain function. To build this understanding, we begin by following the routes visual information takes into and within the brain. This exercise is a bit like traveling a road to discover where it goes. As you trace the route, keep in mind the photograph in Focus 9-1 (page 284) and what the different levels of the visual system are doing to capture that image in the brain.

Structure of the Retina

Virtually all retinal neurons are insensitive to light so are unaffected by light passing through them.

Light energy travels from the outside world through the pupil and into the eye, where it strikes a light-sensitive surface, the retina, at the back of the eye (Figure 9-2). From this stimulation of photoreceptor cells on the retina, we begin to construct a visual world. If you are familiar with the properties of the electromagnetic spectrum and with the structure of the eye, read on. To refresh your knowledge of these topics, read The Basics: Visible Light and the Structure of the Eye on pages 290–291 before you continue.

Figure 9-2 includes a photograph of the retina, which is composed of photoreceptors beneath a layer of neurons connected to them. The neurons lie in front of the photoreceptor cells, but they do not prevent incoming light from being absorbed by those receptors, because the neurons are transparent and the photoreceptors are extremely sensitive to light. In addition, special glial cells in the retina called Müller cells span from the retina’s inner membrane at the front to the photoreceptors at the back of the retina and act as optical fibers, channeling light to the buried photoreceptors.

image
Figure 9-2: FIGURE 9-2 Central Focus This cross section through the retina (A) shows the depression at the fovea—also shown in the scanning electron micrograph (B)—where photoreceptors are packed most densely and where our vision is clearest.
SPL/Science Source

Together, the photoreceptor cells and the retinal neurons perform some amazing functions. They translate light into action potentials, discriminate wavelengths so that we can distinguish colors, and work in a range of light intensities from bright to dim. These cells afford visual precision sufficient for us to see a human hair lying on the page of this book from a distance of 18 inches.

As in a camera, the image of objects projected onto the retina is upside down and backward. This flip-flopped orientation poses no problem for the brain. Remember, the brain is constructing the outside world, so it does not really care how the image is oriented initially. In fact, the brain can make adjustments regardless of the orientation of the images that it receives.

If for several days you were to wear glasses that invert visual images, the world would first appear upside down but then would suddenly appear right side up again because your brain would correct the distortion (Held, 1968). Curiously, when you removed the glasses, the world would temporarily seem upside down once more, because your brain at first would be unaware that you had tricked it again. Eventually, though, your brain would solve this puzzle too, and the world would flip back to the correct orientation.

Fovea

Try this experiment. Focus on the print at the left edge of this page. The words will be clearly legible. Now, while holding your eyes still, try to read the words on the right side of the page. It will be very difficult, even impossible, even though you can see that words are there.

290

THE BASICS

Visible Light and the Structure of the Eye

The brain’s visual system analyzes visible light—the part of the electromagnetic (EM) spectrum that the human eye evolved to capture and focus.

Light: The Stimulus for Vision

Light can enter the eye directly from a source that produces it—a lamp, for example, or the sun—or indirectly after reflecting from a surface—the pages of a book, for example, or the surface of water. As illustrated in Electromagnetic Spectrum, not all light waves are the same length, and only a sliver of the EM spectrum is visible to us. If our photoreceptors could detect light in the shorter ultraviolet or longer infrared wavelengths, we would see additional colors.

Structure of the Eye

The range of light visible to humans is constrained not by the properties of light waves but rather by the properties of our visual receptors. How do photoreceptor cells in the retina absorb light energy and initiate the processes leading to vision? How the Eye Works illustrates the eye’s structure and shows how its design captures and focuses light.

Optical Errors of Refraction

A web of muscles adjusts the shape of the eye’s lens to bend light to greater or lesser degrees, which allows near or far images to be focused on the retina. When images are not properly focused, we need corrective lenses.

The eye, like a camera, works correctly only when sufficient light passes through the lens and is focused on the receptor surface—the retina in the eye or the light-sensitive material in the camera. If the focal point of the light falls slightly in front of the receptor surface or slightly behind it, a refractive error causes objects to appear blurry. Refractive errors in the eye are of two basic types, diagrammed in Refractive Errors.

Myopia (nearsightedness) afflicts about 50 percent of young people in the developed world. Hyperopia (farsightedness) is a less common refractive error, but as people age, the lens loses its elasticity and consequently becomes unable to refract light from nearby objects correctly. This form of hyperopia, called presbyopia (old-sightedness), is so common that you rarely find people older than 50 who do not need glasses to see up close, especially for reading.

It is also common to see young children wearing corrective lenses. The incidence of myopia in the United States has doubled in the past 40 years to about 42 percent. It is even higher in Northern Europe (50 percent) and Asia (50 percent to 80 percent). Two factors probably account for the increase.

First, more young people are attending school longer and thus are doing more close work, especially reading. Close work strains the eye muscles. Second, people are spending less and less time outdoors in bright light. Bright light makes the pupil contract, which improves visual depth of field: your eyes focus better. Children should probably spend at least 2 hours each day outside in bright light. Consider that myopia is less common in countries such as Australia (17 percent), where bright light is plentiful.

Electromagnetic Spectrum
image

291

How the Eye Works
image
Refractive Errors
image

292

The lesson is that our vision is better in the center of the visual field than at the margins, or periphery. Letters at the periphery must be much larger than those in the center for us to see them as well. Figure 9-3 shows that the retinal surface is depressed at the fovea. This depression is formed because many optic nerve fibers skirt the fovea to facilitate light access to its receptors.

image
Figure 9-3: FIGURE 9-3 Acuity Across the Visual Field Focus on the star in the middle of the chart to demonstrate the relative sizes of letters legible in the central field of vision compared with the peripheral field.

Blind Spot

Now try another experiment. Stand with your head over a tabletop and hold a pencil in your hand. Close one eye. Stare at the edge of the tabletop nearest you. Now hold the pencil in a horizontal position and move it along the edge of the table, with the eraser on the table. Beginning at a point approximately below your nose, move the pencil slowly along the table in the direction of the open eye.

When you have moved the pencil about 6 inches, the eraser will vanish. You have found your blind spot, a small area of the retina also known as the optic disc. This is the area where blood vessels enter and exit the eye and where fibers leading from retinal neurons form the optic nerve, which goes to the brain. There are therefore no photoreceptors in this part of the retina. You can use Figure 9-4 to demonstrate the blind spot in another way.

image
Figure 9-4: FIGURE 9-4 Find Your Blind Spot Hold this book 30 centimeters (about 12 inches) away from your face. Shut your left eye and look at the cross with your right eye. Slowly bring the page toward you until the red spot in the center of the yellow disc disappears and the entire disc appears yellow. The red spot is now in your blind spot. Your brain replaces the area with the surrounding yellow to fill in the image. Turn the book upside down to test your left eye.

Fortunately, your visual system solves the blind spot problem: your optic disc is in a different location in each eye. The optic disc is lateral to the fovea in each eye, which means that it is left of the fovea in the left eye and right of the fovea in the right eye. Because the two eyes’ visual fields overlap, the right eye can see the left eye’s blind spot and vice versa.

Using both eyes together, then, you can see the whole visual world. For people blind in one eye, the sightless eye cannot compensate for the blind spot in the functioning eye. Still, the visual system compensates for the blind spot in several other ways, and so these people have no sense of a hole in their field of vision.

The blind spot is of particular importance in neurology. It allows neurologists to indirectly view the condition of the optic nerve while providing a window on events in the brain. If intracranial pressure increases, as occurs with a tumor or brain abscess (an infection), the optic disc swells, leading to papilledema (swollen disc). The swelling occurs in part because, like all neural tissue, the optic nerve is surrounded by cerebrospinal fluid. Pressure inside the cranium can displace CSF around the optic nerve, causing swelling at the optic disc.

Another cause of papilledema is inflammation of the optic nerve itself, a condition known as optic neuritis. Whatever the cause, a person with a swollen optic disc usually loses vision owing to pressure on the optic nerve. If the swelling is due to optic neuritis, probably the most common neurological visual disorder, the prognosis for recovery is good.

Photoreceptors

The retina’s photoreceptor cells convert light energy first into chemical energy and then into neural activity. Light striking a photoreceptor triggers a series of chemical reactions that lead to a change in membrane potential (electrical charge) that in turn leads to a change in the release of neurotransmitter onto nearby neurons.

293

Rods and cones, the two types of photoreceptors shown in Figure 9-5, differ in many ways. They are structurally different. Rods are longer than cones and cylindrical at one end, whereas cones have a tapered end. Rods are more numerous than cones; are sensitive to low levels of brightness (luminance), especially in dim light; and function mainly for night vision (see Clinical Focus 9-2, Visual Illuminance). Cones do not respond to dim light, but they are highly responsive to bright light. Cones mediate both color vision and our ability to see fine detail (visual acuity).

image
Figure 9-5: FIGURE 9-5 Photoreceptor Cells Rods and cones are tubelike structures, as the scanning electron micrograph at right shows. They differ, especially in the outer segment that contains the light-absorbing visual pigment. Rods are especially sensitive to broad-spectrum luminance; and cones, to particular wavelengths of light.
SPL/Science Source

Rods and cones are unevenly distributed over the retina. The fovea has only cones, but their density drops dramatically beyond the fovea. For this reason, our vision is not so sharp at the edges of the visual field, as demonstrated in Figure 9-3.

A final difference between rods and cones is in their light-absorbing pigments. All rods have the same pigment. Each cone has one of three pigments. These four pigments, one in the rods and three in the cones, form the basis for our vision.

A nanometer (nm) is one-billionth of a meter.

As shown on the spectrum in Figure 9-6, however, if you were to look at lights with wavelengths of 419, 531, and 559 nanometers (nm), they would not appear blue, green, and red but rather blue-green, yellow-green, and orange. Remember, though, that you are looking at the lights with all three of your cone types and that each cone pigment responds to light across a range of frequencies, not just to its frequency of maximum absorption.

image
Figure 9-6: FIGURE 9-6 Range and Peak Sensitivity Our color perception corresponds to the summed activity of the three cone types: S cones, M cones, and L cones (for short, medium, and long wavelengths). Each type is most sensitive to a narrow range of the visible spectrum. Rods (white curve) prefer a range of wavelengths centered on 496 nm but do not contribute to our color perception. Rod activity is not summed with the cones in the color vision system.

Both the presence of three cone receptor types and their relative numbers and distribution across the retina contribute to our perception of color. As Figure 9-7 shows, the three cone types are distributed more or less randomly across the retina, making our ability to perceive different colors fairly constant across the visual field. The numbers of red and green cones are approximately equal, but blue cones are fewer. As a result, we are not as sensitive to wavelengths in the blue part of the visible spectrum as we are to red and green wavelengths.

image
Figure 9-7: FIGURE 9-7 Retinal Receptors The retinal mosaic of rods and three cone types. This diagram represents the distribution near the fovea, where cones outnumber rods. Red and green cones outnumber the blue.

294

CLINICAL FOCUS 9-2

Visual Illuminance

The eye, like a camera, works correctly only when sufficient light passes through the lens and is focused on the receptor surface—the retina of the eye or the light-sensitive surface in the camera. Too little light entering the eye or the camera produces a problem of visual illuminance: it is hard to see any image at all.

Visual illuminance is typically a complication of aging eyes, It cannot be cured by corrective lenses. As we age, the eye’s lens and cornea allow less light through, so less light strikes the retina. Don Kline (1994) estimated that between ages 20 and 40, people’s ability to see in dim light drops by 50 percent; and by a further 50 percent over every 20 additional years. As a result, seeing in dim light becomes increasingly difficult, especially at night.

The only solution to compensate for visual illuminance is to increase lighting. Night vision is especially problematic. Not surprisingly, statistics show a marked drop in the number of people driving at night in each successive decade after age 40.

image
These photographs represent the typical drop in luminance between age 20 (left) and age 60 (right).
Photo Courtesy of Dr. Donald Kline, University of Calgary

Other species that have color vision similar to humans’ also have three types of cones with three color pigments. Because of slight variations in these pigments, the exact frequencies of maximum absorption differ among species. For humans, the exact frequencies are not identical with the numbers given earlier, which are an average across mammals. They are actually 426 and 530 nm for the blue and green cones, respectively, and 552 or 557 nm for the red cone. The two peak sensitivity levels of red cones represent the two variants that humans have evolved. The difference in these two red cones appears minuscule, but it does make a functional difference in some females’ color perception.

The gene for the red cone is carried on the X chromosome. Males have only one X chromosome, so they have only one of these genes and only one type of red cone. The situation is more complicated for females, who possess two X chromosomes. Although most women have only one type of red cone, those who have both are more sensitive than the rest of us to color differences at the red end of the spectrum. We could say that women who have both red cone types have a slightly rosier view of the world: their color receptors construct a world with a richer range of red experiences. But they also have to contend with seemingly peculiar color coordination by others.

Types of Retinal Neurons

Photoreceptors are connected to two layers of retinal neurons. In the procession from the rods and cones toward the brain shown in Figure 9-8, the first layer contains three cell types: bipolar, horizontal, and amacrine. Horizontal cells link photoreceptors with bipolar cells, whereas amacrine cells link bipolar cells with cells in the second neural layer, the retinal ganglion cells (RGCs). RGC axons collect in a bundle at the optic disc and leave the eye to form the optic nerve.

image
Figure 9-8: FIGURE 9-8 Retinal Cells Neurons in the retina—bipolar, horizontal, amacrine, and ganglion cells—form two layers moving outward from the rods and cones at the retinal surface. Light must pass through both transparent neuron layers to reach the photoreceptors.

295

In Latin, magno means large and parvo means small.

Retinal ganglion cells fall into two major categories, called M and P cells in the primate retina. The designations derive from the distinctly different cell populations in the visual thalamus to which these two classes of RGCs send their axons. As shown in Figure 9-9, one population consists of magnocellular cells (hence M); the other consists of parvocellular cells (hence P). The larger M cells receive their input primarily from rods and so are sensitive to light but not to color. The smaller P cells receive their input primarily from cones and so are sensitive to color.

image
Figure 9-9: FIGURE 9-9 Visual Thalamus The optic nerves connect with the lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus. The LGN has six layers: two magnocellular layers, which receive input mainly from rods, and four parvocellular layers, which receive input mainly from cones.

M cells are found throughout the retina, including the periphery, where we are sensitive to movement but not to color or fine detail. P cells are found largely in the region of the fovea, where we are sensitive to color and fine details. As we follow the ganglion cell axons into the brain, you will see that these two categories of RGCs maintain their distinctiveness throughout the visual pathways.

Visual Pathways

RGCs form the optic nerve, the road into the brain. This road forks off to several places. The destinations of these branches give us clues to what the brain is doing with visual input and how the brain constructs our visual world.

The optic chiasm gets its name from the shape of the Greek letter chi (χ) (pronounced ki).

Crossing the Optic Chiasm

We begin with the optic nerves, one exiting from each eye. Just before entering the brain, the optic nerves partly cross, forming the optic chiasm.

By connecting both eyes with both hemispheres, our visual system represents the world seen through two eyes as a single perception.

About half the fibers from each eye cross in such a way that the left half of each optic nerve goes to the left side of the brain, and the right half goes to the brain’s right side, as diagrammed in Figure 9-10. The medial path of each retina, the nasal retina, crosses to the opposite side. The lateral path, the temporal retina, travels straight back on the same side. Because light that falls on the right half of each retina actually comes from the left side of the visual field, information from the left visual field goes to the brain’s right hemisphere, and information from the right visual field goes to the left hemisphere. Thus, half of each retina’s visual field is represented on each side of the brain.

image
Figure 9-10: FIGURE 9-10 Crossing the Optic Chiasm This dorsal view shows the visual pathway from each eye to the primary visual cortex of each hemisphere. Information from the right side of the visual field (blue) moves from the two left halves of the retinas, ending in the left hemisphere. Information from the left side of the visual field (red) hits the right halves of the retinas and travels to the right side of the brain.

Three Routes to the Visual Brain

296

Two main pathways lead to the visual cortex in the occipital lobe: the geniculostriate pathway for processing the object’s image and the tectopulvinar pathway for directing rapid eye movements. Another smaller pathway tracks into the hypothalamus.

GENICULOSTRIATE SYSTEM On entering the brain, the RGC axons separate, forming the two distinct pathways shown in Figure 9-11. All of the P ganglion axons and some of the M ganglion axons form a pathway called the geniculostriate system. This pathway goes from the retina to the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of the thalamus and then to layer IV of the primary visual cortex in the occipital lobe.

image
Figure 9-11: FIGURE 9-11 Main Visual Pathways into the Brain The optic nerve follows (1) the geniculostriate path to the primary visual cortex and (2) the tectopulvinar path to the temporal and parietal lobes. (The LGN of the thalamus is part of the diencephalon, shown in Figure 2-19; the superior colliculus in the tectum is part of the midbrain, shown in Figure 2-18.)

When stained, the primary visual cortex shows a broad stripe across it in layer IV and so is known as striate (striped) cortex (Figure 9-12). The geniculostriate system therefore bridges the thalamus (geniculate) and the striate cortex. From the striate cortex, the axon pathway divides. One route goes to vision-related regions of the parietal lobe, and another route goes to vision-related regions of the temporal lobe.

image
Figure 9-12: FIGURE 9-12 Striate Cortex Area V1 is also called the striate cortex because sections appear striated (striped) when stained with either a cell body stain (left) or a myelin stain (right). The sections shown here come from a rhesus monkey’s brain.
Bryan Kolb

TECTOPULVINAR SYSTEM The second pathway leading from the eye is formed by the axons of the remaining M ganglion cells. These cells send their axons to the midbrain’s superior colliculus, which sends connections to the pulvinar region of the thalamus. This pathway is therefore known as the tectopulvinar system because it runs from the eye through the midbrain tectum to the pulvinar (see Figure 9-11). The pulvinar sends connections to the parietal and temporal lobes, bypassing the occipital visual areas.

RETINOHYPOTHALAMIC TRACT Between 1 percent and 3 percent of RGCs are unique in that they are photosensitive: they act as photoreceptors. These pRGCs, which contain the pigment melanopsin, absorb blue light at a wavelength (between 460 and 480 nm) different from the wavelengths of rods or cones (see Figure 9-6). Axons of pRGCs form a small third visual pathway, the retinohypothalamic tract.

Figure 13-6 maps the retinohypothalamic tract into the SCN.

The retinohypothalamic tract synapses in the tiny suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in the hypothalamus, next to the optic chiasm. Photosensitive RGCs participates both in regulating circadian rhythms and in the pupillary reflex that expands and contracts the pupil in response to the amount of light falling on the retina. Farhan Zaidi and colleagues (2007) studied two profoundly blind subjects who lack functional rods and cones. The researchers found that stimulation with 480-nm (blue) light increases alertness and appears to play some rudimentary role in visual awareness.

Dorsal and Ventral Visual Streams

The geniculostriate and tectopulvinar pathways extend into the visual brain. Each eventually leads to either the parietal lobe or the temporal lobe. Our next task is to determine the role each lobe plays in building our visual world. As we look at the photograph in Clinical Focus 9-1, we can identify objects, and we can point to them. Identifying and pointing are different functions.

297

Having identified the temporal lobe and parietal lobe visual pathways, researchers went searching for their possible functions. Why would evolution produce two different destinations for these neural pathways? Each route must produce visual knowledge for a different purpose.

David Milner and Mel Goodale (2006) proposed that these two purposes are to identify a stimulus (the what function) and to control movement to or away from the stimulus (the how function). This whathow distinction came from an analysis of the routes visual information takes when it leaves the striate cortex. Figure 9-13 shows the two distinct visual pathways that originate in the striate cortex, one progressing to the temporal lobe and the other to the parietal lobe. The pathway to the temporal lobe is the ventral stream, whereas the pathway to the parietal lobe is the dorsal stream.

image
Figure 9-13: FIGURE 9-13 Visual Streaming Information travels from the occipital visual areas to the parietal and temporal lobes, forming the dorsal (how) and ventral (what) streams, respectively.

Both the geniculostriate and the tectopulvinar pathways contribute to the dorsal and ventral streams. To understand how the two streams function, we return to the details of how visual input from the eyes contributes to them.

Geniculostriate Pathway

The RGC fibers from the two eyes distribute their connections to the two lateral geniculate nuclei (left and right) of the thalamus. At first glance, this appears to be an unusual arrangement. As seen in Figure 9-10, the fibers from the left half of each retina go to the left LGN; those from the right half of each retina go to the right LGN. But the fibers from each eye do not go to exactly the same LGN location.

Each LGN has six layers, and the projections from the two eyes go to different layers, as illustrated in anatomical context in Figure 9-14. Layers 2, 3, and 5 receive fibers from the ipsilateral eye (the eye on the same side), whereas layers 1, 4, and 6 receive fibers from the contralateral eye (the eye on the opposite side). This arrangement provides both for combining the information from the two eyes and for segregating the information from the P and M ganglion cells.

image
Figure 9-14: FIGURE 9-14 Geniculostriate Pathway

Axons from the P cells go only to layers 3 through 6 (the parvocellular layers). Axons from the M cells go only to layers 1 and 2 (the magnocellular layers). Because the P cells are responsive to color and fine detail, LGN layers 3 through 6 must be processing information about color and form. In contrast, the M cells mostly process information about movement, so layers 1 and 2 must deal with movement.

Figure 2-22 maps layers I through VI in the primary motor and sensory cortices.

Just as there are six layers in the thalamic LGN (numbered 1 through 6), there are also six layers in the striate cortex (numbered I through VI). That there happen to be six layers in each location is an accident of evolution found in all primate brains. Let us now see where these LGN cells from the thalamus send their connections within the visual cortex.

298

Layer IV is the main afferent (incoming) layer of the cortex. In the visual cortex, layer IV has several sublayers, two of which are known as IVCα and IVCβ. LGN layers 1 and 2 go to IVCα, and layers 3 through 6 go to IVCβ. A distinction between the P and M functions thus continues in the striate cortex.

As illustrated in Figure 9-15, input from the two eyes also remains separated in the cortex. The input from the ipsilaterally and contralaterally connected parts of the LGN go to adjacent strips of occipital cortex. These strips, which are about 0.5 mm across, are known as cortical columns.

image
Figure 9-15: FIGURE 9-15 Maintaining Separate Visual Input Left: Information from the eyes is segregated by layers in the LGN, which maintains this segregation in its projections from the thalamus to the primary visual cortex. Information from each eye travels to adjacent regions in cortical layer IV. Right: A horizontal plane through striate cortex shows a zebralike effect of alternating left and right eye regions.

In summary, P and M retinal ganglion cells send separate pathways to the thalamus, and this segregation continues in the striate cortex. The left and right eyes also send separate pathways to the thalamus, and these pathways, too, remain segregated in the striate cortex.

Tectopulvinar Pathway

To review, magnocellular RGCs found throughout the retina receive input primarily from the rods and so are sensitive to light but not to color. M cells in the periphery of the retina are sensitive to movement but not to color or fine details. In the brain, some M cells join P cells to form the geniculostriate pathway. The tectopulvinar pathway is formed by the axons of the remaining M cells.

These M cells send their axons to the superior colliculus in the midbrain’s tectum. One function of the tectum is to produce orienting movements—to detect stimuli and shift the eyes toward them. The superior colliculus sends connections to the pulvinar region of the thalamus. The medial pulvinar sends connections to the parietal lobe, and the lateral pulvinar sends connections to the temporal lobe. One type of information that these connections are conveying is related to where. Where is important in both the what and how visual streams.

Many textbooks emphasize the how pathway as a where function. Because where is both a property of what a stimulus is and a cue for how to control movement, we use Milner and Goodale’s what–how distinction.

The where function of the tectopulvinar system is useful in understanding D. B.’s blindsight, described in Clinical Focus 9-1. His geniculostriate system was disrupted by surgery, but his tectopulvinar system was not, which allowed him to identify the location of stimuli (where) that he could not identify (what).

Occipital Cortex

299

Our route down the visual pathways has led us from the retina all the way back to the occipital lobe and into the parietal and temporal lobes. Now we explore how visual information proceeds from the striate cortex through the rest of the occipital lobe to the dorsal and ventral streams.

All mammals have at least one primary cortical area for each sensory system. The primary area relays most information that reaches secondary areas.

As shown in Figure 9-16, the occipital lobe is composed of at least six visual regions: V1, V2, V3, V3A, V4, and V5. The striate cortex is region V1, the primary visual cortex. The remaining occipital visual areas form the extrastriate cortex, with each region processing specific features of visual information. Because each occipital region has a unique cytoarchitecture (cellular structure) and unique inputs and outputs, we can infer that each must be doing something different from the others.

image
Figure 9-16: FIGURE 9-16 Visual Regions of the Occipital Lobe

As shown in Figures 9-12 and 9-15, a remarkable feature of region V1 is its striations—its distinctly visible layers. When Margaret Wong-Riley and her colleagues (1993) stained region VI for the enzyme cytochrome oxidase, which has a role in cell metabolism, they found an unexpected heterogeneity. So they sectioned the V1 layers in such a way that each cortical layer was in one plane of section, much like peeling off the layers of an onion and laying them flat on a table. The surface of each flattened layer can then be viewed from above.

The heterogeneous cytochrome staining now appears as random blobs in the V1 layers, as diagrammed in Figure 9-17. These darkened regions have in fact become known as blobs, the less-dark regions separating them as interblobs. Blobs and interblobs serve different functions. Neurons in the blobs take part in color perception; neurons in the interblobs participate in perception of form and motion. Within region V1, then, input arriving from the P cell and M cell pathways of the geniculostriate system is segregated into three separate types of information: color, form, and motion.

image
Figure 9-17: FIGURE 9-17 Heterogeneous Layering Blobs in region V1 and stripes in region V2 are illustrated in this drawing of a flattened section through the visual cortex of a monkey. The blobs and stripes are revealed by a special stain for cytochrome oxidase, a marker for mitochondria, the organelles in cells that gather, store, and release energy.

All three types of information move from region V1 to the adjoining region V2. Here, the color, form, and motion inputs remain segregated, again seen through the pattern of cytochrome oxidase staining. But as Figure 9-17 shows, the staining pattern in region V2 differs from that in region V1. Region V2 has a pattern of thick and thin stripes intermixed with pale zones. The thick stripes receive input from the movement-sensitive neurons in region V1; the thin stripes receive input from V1’s color-sensitive neurons; and the pale zones receive input from V1’s form-sensitive neurons.

300

As charted in Figure 9-18, the visual pathways proceed from region V2 to the other occipital regions and then to the parietal and temporal lobes, forming the dorsal and ventral streams. Although many parietal and temporal regions take part, the major regions are region G in the parietal lobe (thus called region PG) and region E in the temporal lobe (thus called region TE).

image
Figure 9-18: FIGURE 9-18 Charting the Visual Streams The dorsal stream, which controls visual action (top), begins in region V1 and flows through V2 to the other occipital areas and finally to the parietal cortex, ending in area PG. The ventral stream, which controls object recognition (bottom), begins in region V1 and flows through V2 to the other occipital areas and finally to the temporal cortex, ending in area TE. Information from the blobs and interblobs in V1 flows to the thick, thin, and pale zones of V2 and then to regions V3 and V4 to form the ventral stream. Information in the thick and pale zones goes to regions V3A and V5 to form the dorsal stream.

The simple records of color, form, and motion from the occipital regions are assembled in the dorsal and ventral streams to produce a rich, unified visual world of complex objects, such as faces and paintings, and complex skills, such as bike riding and ball catching. We can think of the complex representations of the dorsal and ventral streams as consisting of how functions and what functions. How is looking at, reaching for, and grasping; what is the spoon.

Vision Beyond the Occipital Cortex

Visual processing that begins in occipital cortex continues via the ventral and dorsal streams into the temporal and parietal visual cortex. Each region has multiple areas specialized for specific visual functions. For example, Figure 9-19A shows two regions on the ventral surface of the temporal lobes. One is specialized for recognizing faces (fusiform face area, or FFA), the other for analyzing landmarks such as buildings or trees (parahippocampal place area, or PPA). As we gaze at the photograph in Clinical Focus 9-1 (p. 284), then, the three faces activate the FFA and the trees engage the PPA. Figure 9-19B shows three regions in the parietal lobe related to eye movements (lateral intraparietal area, or LIP) and visual control of grasping (anterior intraparietal area, or AIP). The parietal reach region (PRR) has a role in visually guided reaching movements.

image
Figure 9-19: FIGURE 9-19 Vision Beyond the Occipital Cortex (A) In the temporal lobe, the fusiform face area (FFA) processes faces, and the parahippocampal place area (PPA) processes scenes. (B) In the parietal lobe, the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) contributes to eye movements; the anterior intraparietal area (AIP) is involved in visual control of grasping; and the parietal reach region (PRR) participates in visually guided reaching.
(A) Republished with permission of Hasson, U., Y. Nir, I. Levy, G. Fuhrmann, and R. Malach. Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science 303:1634–1640, 2004, permission conveyed through Copyright Clearance Center, Inc.

301

Literally, agnosia means not knowing. Section 15-7 ties conditions like agnosia to the search for a neural basis of consciousness.

Damage to these regions can produce surprisingly specific deficits. For example, damage to the FFA leads to facial agnosia, or prosopagnosia, a condition in which an individual cannot recognize faces. We saw one patient with prosopagnosia so severe that she could not recognize her identical twin sister’s face. Curiously, her other visual functions seemed to be normal.

9-2 REVIEW

The Visual System’s Functional Anatomy

Before you continue, check your understanding.

Question 1

Neurons that project into the brain from the retina and form the optic nerve are called ____________ .

Question 2

____________ retinal ganglion cells receive input mostly from cones and carry information about color and fine detail, whereas ____________ retinal ganglion cells receive input mostly from rods and carry information about light but not color.

Question 3

The two major pathways from the retina into the brain are ____________ and ____________.

Question 4

Damage to the fusiform face area in the temporal lobe can produce ____________.

Question 5

Contrast the paths and functions of the dorsal and ventral streams.

Answers appear in the Self Test section of the book.