In this chapter we have considered several different types of learning from behavioural, cognitive, evolutionary, and neural perspectives. Yet it may seem strange to you that we have not discussed the kind of learning to which you are currently devoting much of your life: learning in educational settings such as the classroom. Way back in the first chapter of this book (Psychology: Evolution of a Science), we reviewed some techniques that we think are useful for studying the material in this course and others (see The Real World: Improving Study Skills) in Chapter 1. But, we did not say much about the actual research that supports these suggestions. During the past several years, psychologists have published a great deal of work specifically focused on enhancing learning in educational settings. Let us consider what some of this research says about learning techniques, and then turn to the equally important topic of exerting control over learning processes.
Students use a wide variety of study techniques in attempts to increase learning. Popular techniques—
Technique |
Description |
Utility |
Elaborative interrogation |
Generating an explanation for why an explicitly stated fact or concept is true |
Moderate |
Self- |
Explaining how new information is related to known information, or explaining steps taken during problem solving |
Moderate |
Summarization |
Writing summaries (of various lengths) of to- |
Low |
Highlighting/underlining |
Marking potentially important portions of to- |
Low |
Keyword mnemonic |
Using keywords and mental images of text materials while reading or listening |
Low |
Imagery for text |
Attempting to form mental images of text materials while reading or listening |
Low |
Re- |
Re- |
Low |
Practice testing |
Self- |
High |
Distributed practice |
Implementing a schedule of practice that spreads out study activities over time |
High |
Interleaved practice |
Implementing a schedule of practice that mixes different kinds of problems, or a schedule of study that mixes different kinds of material, within a single study session |
Moderate |
305
Despite their popularity, highlighting, re-
Cramming for exams (neglecting to study for an extended period of time and then studying intensively just before an exam) (Vacha & McBride, 1993) is a common occurrence in educational life. Surveys of undergraduates across a range of universities indicate that anywhere from about 25 percent to as many as 50 percent of students report relying on cramming as part of their studying technique (McIntyre & Munson, 2008). Though cramming is better than not studying at all, when students cram for an exam, they repeatedly study the to-
306
The benefits of distributed practice relative to massed practice have been known for a long time; in fact, they were first reported in the classic studies of Ebbinghaus (1885/1964) concerning retention of nonsense syllables (see the Memory chapter). What is most impressive is just how widespread the benefits of distributed practice are: They have been observed for numerous different kinds of materials, including foreign vocabulary, definitions, and face–
Despite all the evidence indicating that distributed practice is an effective learning strategy, we still do not fully understand why that is so. One promising idea is that when engaging in massed practice, retrieving recently studied information is relatively easy, whereas during distributed practice, it is more difficult to retrieve information that was studied less recently. More difficult retrievals benefit subsequent learning more than easy retrievals, in line with idea of “desirable difficulties” (Bjork & Bjork, 2011) introduced in The Real World: Improving Study Skills. Whatever the explanation for the effects of distributed practice, there is no denying its benefits for students.
Why does a difficult practice test have the greatest benefit?
Practice testing, like distributed practice, has proven useful across a wide range of materials, including learning of stories, facts, vocabulary, and lectures (Dunlosky et al., 2013; Karpicke, 2012) (see also the LearningCurve system associated with this text, which uses practice testing). As you learned in the Memory chapter, practice testing is effective, in part, because actively retrieving an item from memory on a test improves subsequent retention of that item more than simply studying it again (Roediger & Karpicke, 2006). Yet when asked about their preferred study strategies, students indicated by a wide margin that they prefer re-
Recent labratory research by one of your textbook authors highlights yet another benefit of testing: Including brief tests during a lecture can improve learning by reducing the tendency to mind wander (Szpunar, Khan, & Schacter, 2013). How often have you found your mind wandering—
307
Participants in the tested group received brief tests on each segment.
Participants in the nontested group did not receive a test until after the final segment (they worked on arithmetic problems after each of the earlier segments).
Participants in a re-
How does taking practice tests help focus a wandering mind?
At random times during the lectures, participants in all groups were probed about whether they were paying attention to the lecture or mind wandering off to other topics. Participants in the nontested and re-
It is the night before the final exam in your introductory psychology course. You have put in a lot of time reviewing your course notes and the material in this textbook, and you feel that you have learned most of it pretty well. You are coming down the home stretch with little time left, and you have got to decide whether to devote those precious remaining minutes to studying psychological disorders or social psychology. How do you make that decision? What are its potential consequences? An important part of learning involves assessing how well we know something and how much more time we need to devote to studying it.
Recent research has shown that people’s judgments about what they have learned play a critical role in guiding further study and learning (Dunlosky & Thiede, 2013; Metcalfe, 2009). Experimental evidence reveals that these subjective assessments, which psychologists refer to as judgments of learning (JOLs), have a causal influence on learning: People typically devote more time to studying items that they judge they have not learned well (Metcalfe & Finn, 2008; Son & Metcalfe, 2000).
The finding that JOLs are causally related to decisions about how much to study a particular item is important because JOLs are sometimes inaccurate (Castel, McCabe, & Roediger, 2007). For example, after reading and re-
308
In what ways can JOLs be misleading?
So, if you are preparing for the final exam in this course and need to decide whether to devote more time to studying psychological disorders or social psychology, try to exert control over learning by testing yourself on material from the two chapters; you can use the results of those tests to help you decide which chapter requires further work. Heed the conclusion from researchers (Bjork, Dunlosky, & Kornell, 2013) that becoming a more sophisticated and effective learner requires understanding: (a) key features of learning and memory; (b) effective learning techniques; (c) how to monitor and control one’s own learning; and (d) biases that can undermine judgments of learning.
Online learning has become a hot topic recently as a result of new online initiatives from a number of leading brick-
Our education system is in a state of crisis. Among developed countries, the United States is 55th in quality rankings of elementary math and science education, 20th in high school completion rate and 27th in the fraction of [university] students receiving undergraduate degrees in science or engineering.
As a society, we can and should invest more money in education. But that is only part of the solution. The high costs of high-
If these goals seem contradictory, let’s consider an example from history. In the 19th century, 60 percent of the American work force was in agriculture, and there were frequent food shortages. Today, agriculture accounts for less than 2 percent of the work force, and there are food surpluses.
The key to this transition was the use of technology—
How can we improve performance in education, while cutting costs at the same time? In 1984, Benjamin Bloom showed that individual tutoring had a huge advantage over standard lecture environments: The average tutored student performed better than 98 percent of the students in the standard class.
Until now, it has been hard to see how to make individualized education affordable. But I argue that technology may provide a path to this goal.
Consider the success of the Khan Academy, which began when Salman Khan tried to teach math remotely to his young cousins. He recorded short videos with explanations and placed them on the Web, augmenting them with automatically graded exercises. This simple approach was so compelling that by now, more than 700 million videos have been watched by millions of viewers.
At Stanford, we recently placed three computer science courses online, using a similar format. Remarkably, in the first four weeks, 300 000 students registered for these courses, with millions of video views and hundreds of thousands of submitted assignments.
What can we learn from these successes? First, we see that video content is engaging to students—
Second, presenting content in short, bite-
Conversely, students with an aptitude for the topic can move ahead rapidly, avoiding boredom and disengagement. In short, everyone has access to a personalized experience that resembles individual tutoring.
Watching passively is not enough. Engagement through exercises and assessments is a critical component of learning. These exercises are designed not just to evaluate the student’s learning, but also, more important, to enhance understanding by prompting recall and placing ideas in context.
Moreover, testing allows students to move ahead when they master a concept, rather than when they have spent a stipulated amount of time staring at the teacher who is explaining it.
For many types of questions, we now have methods to automatically assess students’ work, allowing them to practice while receiving instant feedback about their performance. With some effort in technology development, our ability to check answers for many types of questions will get closer and closer to that of human graders.
Of course, these student–
Our Stanford courses provide a forum in which students can vote on questions and answers, allowing the most important questions to be answered quickly—
More broadly, the online format gives us the ability to identify what works. Until now, many education studies have been based on populations of a few dozen students. Online technology can capture every click: what students watched more than once, where they paused, what mistakes they made. This mass of data is an invaluable resource for understanding the learning process and figuring out which strategies really serve students best.
Some argue that online education cannot teach creative problem-
In this format, which we call the flipped classroom, teachers have time to interact with students, motivate them and challenge them. Though attendance in my Stanford class is optional, it is considerably higher than in many standard lecture-
A 2010 analysis from the Department of Education, based on 45 studies, showed that online learning is as effective as face-
Online education, then, can serve two goals. For students lucky enough to have access to great teachers, blended learning can mean even better outcomes at the same or lower cost. And for the millions here and abroad who lack access to good, in-
Nelson Mandela said, “Education is the most powerful weapon which you can use to change the world.” By using technology in the service of education, we can change the world in our lifetime.
Koller makes a strong positive case for online learning, and the rapid spread of online courses since the publication of this article indicates that others agree with her. Furthermore, Koller’s comments about the delivery of information in “bite-
What do you see as the major challenges for online learning? How important is face-
From the New York Times, December 5, 2011 © 2011 The New York Times. All rights reserved. Used by permission and protected by the Copyright Laws of the United States. The printing, copying, redistribution, or retransmission of this Content without express written permission is prohibited. http:/
309
Research on learning techniques indicates that some popular study methods such as highlighting, underlining, and re-
Practice testing improves retention and transfer of learning and can also enhance learning and reduce mind wandering during lectures.
Judgments of learning play a causal role in determining what material to study, but they can be misleading.
310