You have a problem when you find yourself in a place where you do not want to be. In such circumstances, you try to find a way to change the situation so that you end up in a situation you do want. Let us say that it is the night before a test, and you are trying to study but just cannot settle down and focus on the material. This is a situation you do not want. So, you try to think of ways to help yourself focus. You might begin with the material that most interests you, or provide yourself with rewards, such as a music break or a trip to the refrigerator. If these activities enable you to get down to work, your problem is solved.
Two major types of problems complicate our daily lives. The first and most frequent is the ill-
In 1945, German psychologist Karl Duncker reported some important studies of the problem-
Analyze the goal state (i.e., the desired outcome you want to attain).
Analyze the current state (i.e., your starting point, or the current situation).
List the differences between the current state and the goal state.
Reduce the list of differences by
Consider, for example, one of Duncker’s problems:
A patient has an inoperable tumour in his abdomen. The tumour is inoperable because it is surrounded by healthy but fragile tissue that would be severely damaged during surgery. How can the patient be saved?
The goal state is a patient without the tumour and with undamaged surrounding tissue. The current state is a patient with an inoperable tumour surrounded by fragile tissue. The difference between these two states is the tumour. A direct-
When we engage in analogical problem solving, we attempt to solve a problem by finding a similar problem with a known solution and applying that solution to the current problem. Consider the following story:
An island surrounded by bridges is the site of an enemy fortress. The massive fortification is so strongly defended that only a very large army could overtake it. Unfortunately, the bridges would collapse under the weight of such a huge force. So, a clever general divides the army into several smaller units and sends the units over different bridges, timing the crossings so that the many streams of soldiers converge on the fortress at the same time and the fortress is taken.
Does this story suggest a solution to the tumour problem? It should. Removing a tumour and attacking a fortress are very different problems, but the two problems are analogous because they share a common structure: The goal state is a conquered fortress with undamaged surrounding bridges. The current state is an occupied fortress surrounded by fragile bridges. The difference between the two states is the occupying enemy. The solution is to divide the required force into smaller units that are light enough to spare the fragile bridges and send them down the bridges simultaneously so that they converge on the fortress. The combined units will form an army strong enough to take the fortress (see FIGURE 9.13).
This analogous problem of the island fortress suggests the following direct-
Surround the patient with X-
Did this solution occur to you after reading the fortress story? In studies that have used the tumour problem, only 10 percent of participants spontaneously generated the correct solution. This percentage rose to 30 percent if participants read the island fortress problem or other analogous story. However, the success climbed dramatically to 75 percent among participants who had a chance to read more than one analogous problem or were given a deliberate hint to use the solution to the fortress story (Gick & Holyoak, 1980).
384
Why are analogies useful in problem solving?
Why was the fortress problem so ineffective by itself? Problem solving is strongly affected by superficial similarities between problems, and the relationship between the tumour and fortress problems lies deep in their structure (Catrambone, 2002).
Analogical problem solving shows us that successfully solving a problem often depends on learning the principles underlying a particular type of problem and also that solving lots of problems improves our ability to recognize certain problem types and generate effective solutions. Some problem solving, however, seems to involve brilliant flashes of insight and creative solutions that have never before been tried. Creative and insightful solutions often rely on restructuring a problem so that it turns into a problem you already know how to solve (Cummins, 2012).
Consider the exceptional mind of the mathematician Friedrich Gauss (1777–
What is the role of the unconscious in flashes of insight?
According to Gestalt psychologists, insights such as these reflect a spontaneous restructuring of a problem. A sudden flash of insight contrasts with incremental problem-
Later research, however, suggests that sudden insightful solutions may actually result from unconscious incremental processes (Bowers et al., 1990). In one study, research participants were shown paired, three-
385
Even if participants could not find a solution, they could reliably decide, more than by chance alone, which of the pairs was coherent. However, if insightful solutions actually occur in a sudden, all-
Finding a connection between the words strawberry and traffic might take some time, even for someone motivated to figure out how they are related. But if the word strawberry activates jam in long-
If insight is a simple incremental process, why is not its occurrence more frequent? In the research discussed previously, participants produced insightful solutions only 25 percent of the time. Insight is rare because problem solving (like decision making) suffers from framing effects. In problem solving, framing tends to limit the types of solutions that occur to us. Functional fixedness—the tendency to perceive the functions of objects as fixed—is a process that constricts our thinking. Look at FIGURES 9.16 and 9.17 and see if you can solve the problems before reading on. In Figure 9.16, your task is to illuminate a dark room using the following objects: some thumbtacks, a box of matches, and a candle. In Figure 9.17, you need to tie the two strings together, but they are too far apart to reach one while holding on to the other. Using the items on the table, you are expected to reach the string that is too far away to grasp.
386
Difficulty solving these problems derives from our tendency to think of the objects only in terms of their normal, typical, or “fixed” functions. We do not think to use the matchbox for a candleholder because boxes typically hold matches, not candles. Similarly, using the hammer as a pendulum weight does not spring to mind because hammers are typically used to pound things. Did functional fixedness prevent you from solving these problems? (The solutions are shown in FIGURES 9.19 and 9.20.)
Sometimes framing limits our ability to generate a solution. Before reading on, look at FIGURE 9.18. Without lifting your pencil from the page, try to connect all nine dots with only four straight lines.To solve this problem, you must allow the lines you draw to extend outside the imaginary box that surrounds the dots (see FIGURE 9.21). This constraint does not reside in the problem but in the mind of the problem solver (Kershaw & Ohlsson, 2004). Despite the apparent sudden flash of insight that seems to yield a solution to problems of this type, research indicates that the thought processes people use when solving even this type of insight problem are best described as an incremental, means–
The “aha!” moment that accompanies a sudden flash of insight is a compelling experience, one that highlights that solving a problem based on insight feels radically different from solving it through step-
To examine brain activity associated with insight, researchers used a procedure called compound remote associates that is similar in some respects to the three-
Participants are instructed to press a button the moment a solution comes to mind, then describe whether they arrived at the solution via insight or through an analytic strategy. In an initial study, researchers used the electroencephalograph (EEG) (see the Neuroscience chapter) to measure brain electrical activity as participants attempted to solve the problems. What they observed was striking: Beginning about one-
The great French scientist Louis Pasteur once stated: “Chance favors only the prepared mind.” Inspired by this observation, researchers asked whether brain activity occurring just before presentation of a problem influenced whether that problem was solved via insight or analytic strategies (Kounios et al., 2006). It did. In the moments before a problem was solved with an insight solution, there was increased activity deep in the frontal lobes, in a part of the brain known as the anterior cingulate, which controls cognitive processes such as the ability to switch attention from one thing to another. The researchers suggested that this increased activity in the anterior cingulate allowed participants to attend to and detect associations that were only weakly activated, perhaps at a subconscious level, and that facilitate sudden insight.
A related study with the compound remote associates task revealed that when people were in a positive mood, they solved more problems with insight than people who were in a less positive mood (Subramaniam et al., 2009). Moreover, as shown in the figure, positive mood was associated with heightened activity in the anterior cingulate during the moments before a problem was presented—
387
Brain activity prior to problem solving provides clues about which individuals are more likely to rely on insight over analytic strategies to solve compound remote associates (Kounios et al., 2008). Using EEG to measure resting brain activity, researchers found that insight problem solvers showed more resting activity in the right cerebral hemisphere than did analytic problem solvers, which is consistent with other research linking creativity with right-
The results of these studies suggest that the familiar image of a light bulb going off in your head when you experience an “aha!” moment is on the mark: Those moments are indeed accompanied by something like an electrical power surge in the brain, and are preceded by specific types of electrical activity patterns. It seems likely that future research will tell us much more about how to turn on the mental light bulb and keep it burning bright.
388
Like concept formation and decision making, problem solving is a process in which new inputs (in this case, problems) are interpreted in terms of old knowledge. Problems may be ill defined or well defined, leading to less or more obvious solutions.
The solutions we generate depend as much on the organization of our knowledge as they do on the objective characteristics of the problems. Means–
Sometimes, as in the case of functional fixedness, that knowledge can restrict our problem-