1.4 Return of the Mind: Psychology Expands

Watson, Skinner, and the behaviourists dominated psychology from the 1930s to the 1950s. Psychologist Ulric Neisser recalled the atmosphere when he was a student in the early 1950s:

Behaviorism was the basic framework for almost all of psychology at the time. It was what you had to learn. That was the age when it was supposed that no psychological phenomenon was real unless you could demonstrate it in a rat. (quoted in Baars, 1986, p. 275)

Behaviourism would not dominate the field for much longer, however, and Neisser himself would play an important role in developing an alternative perspective. Why was behaviourism replaced? Although behaviourism allowed psychologists to measure, predict, and control behaviour, it did this by ignoring some important things. First, it ignored the mental processes that had fascinated psychologists such as Wundt and James and, in so doing, found itself unable to explain some very important phenomena, such as how children learn language. Second, it ignored the evolutionary history of the organisms it studied and was thus unable to explain why, for example, a rat could learn to associate nausea with food much more quickly than it could learn to associate nausea with a tone or a light. As we will see, the approaches that ultimately replaced behaviourism met these kinds of problems head-on.

1.4.1 The Pioneers of Cognitive Psychology

What do you see when you look at this image? Why do you see more than just random markings?

Even at the height of behaviourist domination, there were a few revolutionaries whose research and writings were focused on mental processes. German psychologist Max Wertheimer (1880–1943) focused on the study of illusions, errors of perception, memory, or judgment in which subjective experience differs from objective reality. In one of Wertheimer’s experiments, a person was shown two lights that flashed quickly on a screen, one after the other. One light was flashed through a vertical slit, the other through a diagonal slit. When the time between two flashes was relatively long (one fifth of a second or more), an observer would see that it was just two lights flashing in alternation. But when Wertheimer reduced the time between flashes to around one twentieth of a second, observers saw a single flash of light moving back and forth (Fancher, 1979; Sarris, 1989). Wertheimer reasoned that the perceived motion could not be explained in terms of the separate elements that cause the illusion (the two flashing lights) but instead that the moving flash of light is perceived as a whole rather than as the sum of its two parts. This unified whole, which in German is called Gestalt, makes up the perceptual experience. Wertheimer’s interpretation of the illusion led to the development of Gestalt psychology, a psychological approach that emphasizes that we often perceive the whole rather than the sum of the parts. In other words, the mind imposes organization on what it perceives, so people do not see what the experimenter actually shows them (two separate lights); instead, they see the elements as a unified whole (one moving light).

This 1950s computer was among the first generation of digital computers. How was the computer analogy helpful in the early days of cognitive psychology?
BETTMANN/CORBIS

Why might people not see what an experimenter actually showed them?

21

Another pioneer who focused on the mind was Sir Frederic Bartlett (1886–1969), a British psychologist interested in memory. Bartlett was dissatisfied with existing research, especially the research of German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850–1909), who had performed groundbreaking experiments on memory in 1885 (described in the Memory chapter). Serving as his own research subject, Ebbinghaus tried to discover how quickly and how well he could memorize and recall meaningless information, such as the three-letter nonsense syllables dap, kir, and sul. Bartlett believed that it was more important to examine memory for the kinds of information people actually encounter in everyday life, so he gave people stories to remember and carefully observed the kinds of errors they made when they tried to recall them at a later time (Bartlett, 1932). Bartlett discovered many interesting things that Ebbinghaus could never have learned with his nonsense syllables. For example, he found that research participants often remembered what should have happened or what they expected to happen rather than what actually did happen. These and other errors led Bartlett to suggest that memory is not a photographic reproduction of past experience and that our attempts to recall the past are powerfully influenced by our knowledge, beliefs, hopes, aspirations, and desires.

Jean Piaget (1896–1980) was a Swiss psychologist who studied the perceptual and cognitive errors of children in order to gain insight into the nature and development of the human mind. For example, in one of his tasks, Piaget gave a 3-year-old a large and a small mound of clay and told the child to make the two mounds equal. Then Piaget broke one of the clay mounds into smaller pieces and asked the child which mound now had more clay. Although the amount of clay remained the same, of course, 3-year-olds usually said that the mound that was broken into smaller pieces was bigger, but by the age of 6 or 7, they no longer made this error. As you will see in the Development chapter, Piaget theorized that younger children lack a particular cognitive ability that allows older children to appreciate the fact that the mass of an object remains constant even when it is divided. For Piaget, errors such as these provided key insights into the mental world of the child (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969).

Jean Piaget (1896–1980) studied and theorized about the developing mental lives of children. How were his methods a marked departure from the methods of the behaviourists?
BILL ANDERSON/SCIENCE SOURCE

22

Kurt Lewin argued that people react to the world as they see it and not to the world as it is.
PETER DAZELEY/GETTY IMAGES

German psychologist Kurt Lewin (1890–1947) was also a pioneer in the study of thought at a time when thought had been banished from psychology. Lewin (1936) argued that a person’s behaviour in the world could be predicted best by understanding the person’s subjective experience of the world. A television soap opera is a meaningless series of unrelated physical movements unless one thinks about the characters’ experiences (how Karen feels about Bruce; what Van was planning to say to Kathy about Emily; whether Linda’s sister, Nancy, will always hate their mother for meddling in her marriage; etc.). Lewin realized that it was not the stimulus, but rather the person’s construal of the stimulus, that determined the person’s subsequent behaviour. A pinch on the cheek can be pleasant or unpleasant depending on who administers it, under what circumstances, and to which set of cheeks. Lewin used a special kind of mathematics called topology to model the person’s subjective experience, and although his topological theories were not particularly influential, his attempts to model mental life and his insistence that psychologists study how people construe their worlds would have a lasting impact on psychology.

How did the advent of computers change psychology?

But, aside from a handful of pioneers such as these, psychologists happily ignored mental processes until the 1950s, when something important happened: the computer. The advent of computers had enormous practical impact, of course, but it also had an enormous conceptual impact on psychology. People and computers differ in important ways, but both seem to register, store, and retrieve information, leading psychologists to wonder whether the computer might be useful as a model for the human mind. Computers are information-processing systems, and the flow of information through their circuits is clearly no fairy tale. If psychologists could think of mental events—such as remembering, attending, thinking, believing, evaluating, feeling, and assessing—as the flow of information through the mind, then they might be able to study the mind scientifically after all. The emergence of the computer led to a re-emergence of interest in mental processes all across the discipline of psychology, and spawned a new approach called cognitive psychology, the scientific study of mental processes, including perception, thought, memory, and reasoning.

1.4.2 Technology and the Development of Cognitive Psychology

Although the contributions of psychologists such as Wertheimer, Bartlett, Piaget, and Lewin provided early alternatives to behaviourism, they did not depose it. That job required the Army. During World War II, the military turned to psychologists to help understand how soldiers could best learn to use new technologies, such as radar. Radar operators had to pay close attention to their screens for long periods while trying to decide whether blips were friendly aircraft, enemy aircraft, or flocks of wild geese in need of a good chasing (Ashcraft, 1998; Lachman, Lachman, & Butterfield, 1979). How could radar operators be trained to make quicker and more accurate decisions? The answer to this question clearly required more than the swift delivery of pellets to the radar operator’s food tray. It required that those who designed the equipment think about and talk about cognitive processes, such as perception, attention, identification, memory, and decision making. Behaviourism solved the problem by denying it, so some psychologists decided to deny behaviourism and forge ahead with a new approach.

23

British psychologist Donald Broadbent (1926–1993) was among the first to study what happens when people try to pay attention to several things at once. For instance, Broadbent observed that pilots cannot attend to many different instruments at once and must actively move the focus of their attention from one to another (Best, 1992). Broadbent (1958) showed that the limited capacity to handle incoming information is a fundamental feature of human cognition and that this limit could explain many of the errors that pilots (and other people) made. At about the same time, American psychologist George Miller (1956) pointed out a striking consistency in our capacity limitations across a variety of situations: We can pay attention to, and briefly hold in memory, about seven (give or take two) pieces of information. Cognitive psychologists began conducting experiments and devising theories to better understand the mind’s limited capacity, a problem that behaviourists had ignored.

What did psychologists learn from pilots during World War II?

As you have already read, the invention of the computer in the 1950s had a profound impact on psychologists’ thinking. A computer is made of hardware (e.g., chips and disk drives today; magnetic tapes and vacuum tubes a half-century ago) and software (stored on optical disks today; on punch cards a half-century ago). If the brain is roughly analogous to the computer’s hardware, then perhaps the mind was roughly analogous to a software program. This line of thinking led cognitive psychologists to begin writing computer programs to see what kinds of software could be made to mimic human speech and behaviour (Newell, Shaw, & Simon, 1958).

Noam Chomsky (b. 1928) pointed out that even young children generate sentences they have never heard before, and therefore could not possibly be learning language by reinforcement. This critique of Skinner’s theory signalled the end of behaviourism’s dominance in psychology and helped spark the development of cognitive psychology.
SHUTTERSTOCK

Ironically, the emergence of cognitive psychology was also energized by the appearance of B. F. Skinner’s (1957) book, Verbal Behaviour, which offered a behaviourist analysis of language. A linguist at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT), Noam Chomsky (b. 1928), published a devastating critique of the book in which he argued that Skinner’s insistence on observable behaviour had caused him to miss some of the most important features of language. According to Chomsky, language relies on mental rules that allow people to understand and produce novel words and sentences. The ability of even the youngest child to generate new sentences that he or she had never heard before flew in the face of the behaviourist claim that children learn to use language by reinforcement. Chomsky provided a clever, detailed, and thoroughly cognitive account of language that could explain many of the phenomena that the behaviourist account could not (Chomsky, 1959).

These developments during the 1950s set the stage for an explosion of cognitive studies during the 1960s. Cognitive psychologists did not return to the old introspective procedures used during the nineteenth century, but instead developed new and ingenious methods that allowed them to study cognitive processes. The excitement of the new approach was summarized in a landmark book, Cognitive Psychology, written by someone you met earlier in this chapter, Ulric Neisser. Neisser’s (1967) book provided a foundation for the development of cognitive psychology, which grew and thrived in the years that followed.

24

1.4.3 The Brain Meets the Mind: The Rise of Cognitive Neuroscience

If cognitive psychologists studied the software of the mind, they had little to say about the hardware of the brain. And yet, as any computer scientist knows, the relationship between software and hardware is crucial: Each element needs the other to get the job done. Our mental activities often seem so natural and effortless—noticing the shape of an object, using words in speech or writing, recognizing a face as familiar—that we fail to appreciate the fact that they depend on intricate operations carried out by the brain. This dependence is revealed by dramatic cases in which damage to a particular part of the brain causes a person to lose a specific cognitive ability. Recall that in the nineteenth century, French physician Paul Broca described a patient who, after damage to a limited area in the left side of the brain, could not produce words, even though he could understand them perfectly well. As you will see later in the book, damage to other parts of the brain can also result in syndromes that are characterized by the loss of specific mental abilities (e.g., prosopagnosia, in which the person cannot recognize human faces) or by the emergence of bizarre behaviour or beliefs (e.g., Capgras syndrome, in which the person believes that a close family member has been replaced by an imposter). These striking—sometimes startling—cases remind us that even the simplest cognitive processes depend on the brain.

Karl Lashley (1890–1958), a psychologist who studied with John B. Watson, conducted a famous series of studies in which he trained rats to run mazes, surgically removed parts of their brains, and then measured how well they could run the maze again. Lashley hoped to find the precise spot in the brain where learning occurred. Alas, no one spot seemed to uniquely and reliably eliminate learning (Lashley, 1960). Rather, Lashley simply found that the more of the rat’s brain he removed, the more poorly the rat ran the maze. Lashley was frustrated by his inability to identify a specific site of learning, but his efforts inspired other scientists to take up the challenge. They developed a research area called physiological psychology. Today, this area has grown into behavioural neuroscience, an approach to psychology that links psychological processes to activities in the nervous system and other bodily processes. To learn about the relationship between brain and behaviour, behavioural neuroscientists observe animals’ responses as the animals perform specially constructed tasks, such as running through a maze to obtain food rewards. The neuroscientists can record electrical or chemical responses in the brain as the task is being performed, or later remove specific parts of the brain to see how performance is affected.

Of course, experimental brain surgery cannot ethically be performed on human beings; thus, psychologists who want to study the human brain often have to rely on nature’s cruel and inexact experiments. Birth defects, accidents, and illnesses often cause damage to particular brain regions and, if that damage disrupts a particular ability, then psychologists deduce that the region is involved in producing the ability. For example, in the Memory chapter you will learn about a patient whose memory was virtually eliminated by damage to a specific part of the brain, and you will see how this tragedy provided scientists with remarkable clues about how memories are stored (Scoville & Milner, 1957). Fortunately in the late 1980s, technological breakthroughs led to the development of noninvasive brain scanning techniques that made it possible for psychologists to watch what happens inside a human brain as a person performs a task such as reading, imagining, listening, and remembering. Brain scanning is an invaluable tool because it allows us to observe the brain in action and to see which parts are involved in which operations (see the Neuroscience and Behaviour chapter).

What have we learned by watching the brain at work?

For example, researchers used scanning technology to identify the parts of the brain in the left hemisphere that are involved in specific aspects of language, such as understanding or producing words (Peterson et al., 1989). Later scanning studies showed that people who are deaf from birth, but who learn to communicate using American Sign Language (ASL), rely on regions in the right hemisphere (as well as the left) when using ASL. In contrast, people with normal hearing who learned ASL after puberty seemed to rely only on the left hemisphere when using ASL (Newman et al., 2002). These findings suggest that although both spoken and signed language usually rely on the left hemisphere, the right hemisphere also can become involved—but only for a limited period (perhaps until puberty). The findings also provide a nice example of how psychologists can now use scanning techniques to observe people with various kinds of cognitive capacities and use their observations to unravel the mysteries of the mind and the brain (see FIGURE 1.2). In fact, there is a name for this area of research. Cognitive neuroscience is the field of study that attempts to understand the links between cognitive processes and brain activity (Gazzaniga, 2000).

Figure 1.2: PET Scans of Healthy and Alzheimer’s Brains PET scans are one of a variety of brain imaging technologies that psychologists use to observe the living brain. The four brain images on the top each come from a person suffering from Alzheimer’s disease; the four on the bottom each come from a healthy person of similar age. The red and green areas reflect higher levels of brain activity compared to the blue areas, which reflect lower levels of activity. In each image, the front of the brain is on the top and the back of the brain is on the bottom. You can see that the person with Alzheimer’s disease, compared with the healthy person, shows more extensive areas of lowered activity toward the front of the brain.
ROGER RESSMEYER/CORBIS

25

1.4.4 The Adaptive Mind: The Emergence of Evolutionary Psychology

Psychology’s renewed interest in mental processes and its growing interest in the brain were two developments that led psychologists away from behaviourism. A third development also pointed them in a different direction. Recall that one of behaviourism’s key claims was that organisms are blank slates on which experience writes its lessons, and hence any one lesson should be as easily written as another. But in experiments conducted during the 1960s and 1970s, psychologist John Garcia and his colleagues showed that rats can learn to associate nausea with the smell of food much more quickly than they can learn to associate nausea with a flashing light (Garcia, 1981). Why should this be? In the real world of forests, sewers, and garbage cans, nausea is usually caused by spoiled food and not by lightning, and although these particular rats had been born in a laboratory and had never left their cages, millions of years of evolution had prepared their brains to learn the natural association more quickly than the artificial one. In other words, it was not only the rat’s learning history but the rat’s ancestors’ learning histories that determined the rat’s ability to learn.

Although that fact was at odds with the behaviourist doctrine, it was the credo for a new kind of psychology. Evolutionary psychology explains mind and behaviour in terms of the adaptive value of abilities that are preserved over time by natural selection. Evolutionary psychology has its roots in Charles Darwin’s theory of natural selection, which, as we saw earlier, holds that the features of an organism that help it survive and reproduce are more likely than other features to be passed on to subsequent generations.

26

In 1925, schoolteacher John Scopes was arrested for teaching students about Darwin’s theory of evolution. Today, that theory is the centrepiece of modern biology—and of evolutionary psychology.
HULTON ARCHIVE/GETTY IMAGES

This theory inspired the functionalist approaches of William James and G. Stanley Hall because it led them to focus on how mental abilities help people to solve problems and therefore increase their chances of survival. But it is only since the publication in 1975 of Sociobiology, by the biologist E. O. Wilson, that evolutionary thinking has had an identifiable presence in psychology. That presence is steadily increasing (Buss, 1999; Pinker, 1997a, 1997b; Tooby & Cosmides, 2000). Evolutionary psychologists think of the mind as a collection of specialized “modules” that are designed to solve the human problems our ancestors faced as they attempted to eat, mate, and reproduce over millions of years. According to evolutionary psychology, the brain is not an all-purpose computer that can do or learn one thing just as easily as it can do or learn another; rather, it is a computer that was built to do a few things well and everything else not at all. It is a computer that comes with a small suite of built-in applications that are designed to do the things that previous versions of that computer needed to have done.

Behaviourists explain behaviour in terms of organisms learning to make particular responses that are paired with reinforcement (and to avoid responses that are paired with punishment). Evolutionary psychology focuses on how abilities are preserved over time if they contribute to an organism’s ability to survive and reproduce. How might a proponent of each approach explain the fact that a rat placed in an unfamiliar environment will tend to stay in dark corners and avoid brightly lit open areas?
© JUNIORS BILDARCHIV GMBH/ALAMY

Consider, for example, how evolutionary psychology treats the emotion of jealousy. All of us who have been in romantic relationships have experienced jealousy, if only because we noticed our partner noticing someone else. Jealousy can be a powerful, overwhelming emotion that we might wish to avoid but, according to evolutionary psychology, it exists today because it once served an adaptive function. If some of our hominid ancestors experienced jealousy and others did not, then the ones who experienced it might have been more likely to guard their mates and threaten their rivals and thus may have been more likely to reproduce their “jealous genes” (Buss, 2000, 2007; Buss & Haselton, 2005).

Critics of the evolutionary approach point out that many traits of modern humans and other animals probably evolved to serve different functions than those they currently serve. For example, biologists believe that the feathers of birds probably evolved initially to perform such functions as regulating body temperature or capturing prey and only later served the entirely different function of flight. Likewise, people are reasonably adept at learning to drive a car, but nobody would argue that such an ability is the result of natural selection; the learning abilities that allow us to become skilled car drivers must have evolved for purposes other than driving cars.

Complications such as these have led the critics to wonder how evolutionary hypotheses can ever be tested (Coyne, 2000; Sterelny & Griffiths, 1999). We do not have a record of our ancestors’ thoughts, feelings, and actions, and fossils will not provide much information about the evolution of mind and behaviour. Testing ideas about the evolutionary origins of psychological phenomena is indeed a challenging task, but not an impossible one (Buss et al., 1998; Pinker, 1997a, 1997b).

What evidence suggests that some traits can be inherited?

Start with the assumption that evolutionary adaptations should also increase reproductive success. So, if a specific trait or feature has been favoured by natural selection, it should be possible to find some evidence of this in the numbers of offspring that are produced by the trait’s bearers. Consider, for instance, the hypothesis that men tend to have deep voices because women prefer to mate with baritones rather than sopranos. To investigate this hypothesis, researchers studied a group of modern hunter-gatherers, the Hadza people of Tanzania. Consistent with the evolutionary hypothesis, they found that the pitch of a man’s voice did indeed predict how many children he would have, but the pitch of a woman’s voice did not (Apicella, Feinberg, & Marlowe, 2007). This kind of study provides evidence that allows evolutionary psychologists to test their ideas. Not every evolutionary hypothesis can be tested, of course, but evolutionary psychologists are becoming increasingly inventive in their attempts.

27

  • Psychologists such as Max Wertheimer, Frederic Bartlett, Jean Piaget, and Kurt Lewin defied the behaviourist doctrine and studied the inner workings of the mind. Their efforts, as well as those of later pioneers such as Donald Broadbent, paved the way for cognitive psychology to focus on inner mental processes such as perception, attention, memory, and reasoning.

  • Cognitive psychology developed as a field due to the invention of the computer, psychologists efforts to improve the performance of the military, and Noam Chomsky’s theories about language.

  • Cognitive neuroscience attempts to link the brain with the mind by studying individuals with brain damage (connecting the area damaged with the loss of specific abilities) and individuals without brain damage, using brain scanning techniques.

  • Evolutionary psychology focuses on the adaptive function that minds and brains serve and seeks to understand the nature and origin of psychological processes in terms of natural selection.